Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression

Similar documents
Spatial attention can modulate unconscious orientation processing

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Unconscious orientation processing depends on perceptual load

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies

Pupil dilation accuracy in the prediction of random events. The name, affiliation, and address for the lead experimenter(s) for the study:

Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli

Tsuchiya & Koch Page 1 of 5 Supplementary Note

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Supplemental Information

Effects of attention on visible and invisible adapters

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

The observation that the mere activation

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

Grabbing subitizing with both hands: bimanual number processing

Collinear integration affects visual search at V1

Department of Computer Science, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK;

There s more behind it: Perceived depth order biases perceived numerosity/density

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention

Independent binocular integration for form and colour

Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration

THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Duality in Binocular Rivalry: Distinct Sensitivity of Percept Sequence and Percept Duration to Imbalance between Monocular Stimuli

Social Sciences: Psychological and Cognitive Sciences. A critical reexamination of doing arithmetic nonconsciously

Orientation-selective adaptation to crowded illusory lines

Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Introduction to Functional MRI

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Richman Face Dot Test Paddle

Visible propagation from invisible exogenous cueing

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk

Satiation in name and face recognition

Differential Effects of Continuous Flash Suppression at the Fovea and Periphery

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The path of visual attention

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

University of Groningen

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

Finding a new target in an old display: Evidence for a memory recency effect in visual search

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments

Consciousness is not necessary for visual feature binding

Opposite Influence of Perceptual Memory on Initial and Prolonged Perception of Sensory Ambiguity

Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task

Amodal completion of unconsciously presented objects

Attentional Blink Paradigm

BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

Motion-induced blindness is influenced by global properties of the moving mask

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Unconscious Color Priming Occurs at Stimulus- Not Percept- Dependent Levels of Processing Bruno G. Breitmeyer, 1 Tony Ro, 2 and Neel S.

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

The role of attention in subitizing: Is the magical number 1?

Orientation tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

Supplementary Materials

Six Sigma Glossary Lean 6 Society

Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps

Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing

VISUAL MEMORY. Visual Perception

3/16/2018. Perimetry

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

Perceptual Rivalry: Reflexes Reveal the Gradual Nature of Visual Awareness

Inferential Statistics

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Distinct Contributions of the Magnocellular and Parvocellular Visual Streams to Perceptual Selection

Gage R&R. Variation. Allow us to explain with a simple diagram.

BINOCULAR INFORMATION ACQUISITION AND VISUAL MEMORY

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

Inattentional blindness & visual awareness without report

7 Grip aperture and target shape

Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects?

The Role of the Dorsal Visual Processing Stream in Tool Identification

Transcription:

Supplementary Material Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression 1, 2, * Bahador Bahrami Petra Vetter 1, 3 Eva Spolaore 1, 4 Silvia Pagano 1, 4 Brian Butterworth 1,3 Geraint Rees 1, 2 1 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, Alexandra House, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR UK 2 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG UK 3 Department of Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP UK 4 Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy * Correspondence to: Bahador Bahrami Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR UK Telephone +44-20-7679-1128 Fax +44-20-7813-2835 Email bbahrami@ucl.ac.uk 1

Supplementary Methods Display Participants viewed a CRT display (resolution = 800 600, 14 Sony Multiscan 110ES for which a look-up table linearized output luminance) at a distance of 50cm through a mirror stereoscope that displayed different images to corresponding regions of each retina. Textured black and white bars (0.5 width) were placed binocularly 3 on either side of the fixation point in order to facilitate binocular fusion. Stimuli were generated using the Cogent toolbox (www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent/) for MATLAB (Mathworks Inc). Assessment of departure from chance in Enumeration of invisible prime set Within each participant, the binomial cumulative distribution was used to assess the statistical significance of the accuracy of enumeration of the invisible prime set (rating = 0). With n being the number of trials and three possible alternative answers, the probability of arriving at the correct target number at least k times by chance was calculated as follows: P( k) n i () () () n 1 2 i n = i 3 3 i= k If the probability thus calculated was less than 5%, then that participant s performance had departed significantly from chance. 2

Randomization of suppressed stimulus size The sizes of the suppressed Gabor patches were randomized in such way as to minimize any relation between size and total surface area. For numerosity 3, the diameter of the Gabor patches were sampled from a uniform distribution with mean 0.9 degree (range: 0..8-1.1). For numerosity 2, the diameter of the Gabor patches were sampled from a uniform distribution with mean 1.1 degree (range: 0.9-1.3). For numerosity 1, the diameter of the Gabor patch was sampled from a uniform distribution with mean 1.55 degree (range: 1.2-1.9). These values were chosen to assure that, on average, the total surface area covered by 3 stimuli (i.e. 3 π 0.9/2 0.9/2) was not different from that covered by 2 stimuli (i.e. 2 π 1.1/2 11/2) nor from that of one stimulus (i.e. 1 π 1.55/2 1.55/2). Yet, this randomization with overlapping tails ensured that the size of individual stimuli could not inform the subject about the number of suppressed items. Within each trial, all suppressed Gabor patches had the same size. Exclusion criteria In experiment 2, we first excluded participants for whom the frequency of trials where the prime was rated invisible was less than either of the other two rating levels. This led to exclusion of two participants, presumably due to ineffective continuous flash suppression. We then selected trials for further analysis that were rated zero for prime visibility (indicating that the participant was not aware of the prime) and elicited correct responses to the target within 1500ms. Two further participants were excluded on this basis due to reaction times consistently exceeding 1500ms (i.e. rejection of more than 50% trials). 3

In experiment 3, applying the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as Experiment 2, data from five participants were excluded from the analysis (ineffective CFS for three participants slow RT for two others). In experiments 4 & 5 data from three participants (ineffective CFS for two participants and slow reaction times for another) were excluded from the analysis. Experiments 4 and 5: details of design and procedure In Experiment 4, the task was identical to Experiment 2 (see Figure 1). Each participant completed 12 blocks of 60 trials. Participants completed one practice block before the main experiment. Prime set contrast and duration were reduced (see Stimuli section in General Methods) to enhance the depth of suppression (Tsuchiya et al., 2006). On 40% of the trials, the prime set was presented binocularly and thus was visible. Trials with monocular and binocular primes were randomly interleaved. In Experiment 5, the stimulus sequence was identical to Experiment 4 but participants were asked to enumerate the prime set and rate its visibility. This experiment provided a strict control for prime visibility by employing identical stimuli and the same observers as Experiment 4. Analysis of data was identical to Experiment 1. Each participant completed 8 blocks of 60 trials. The order of the two experiments was counter-balanced across participants. Supplementary Results Experiment 2: priming effect is not accounted for by change of target numerosity across distance. It is conceivable that these effects might relate to the specific target numerosity rather than the more abstract concept of distance between prime and target. As pointed out 4

above, different levels of target-prime distance correspond to specific target quantities. For example, a target/prime distance of -1 comprises trials where target quantity was either one (and prime set was two) or two (and prime set was three); whereas a difference of +1 comprises trials where target quantity was two (and prime set one) and three (and prime set two). Thus, it is possible that the effect of target/prime distance on reaction time may be confounded by the effects of changes in reaction time to different target numerosities. We therefore conducted two additional analyses. Within each participant and for each level of target-prime distance, we calculated the baseline RT by only considering the zero-prime trials in which target quantity corresponded to the specific target-prime distance. For example, for target/prime -1, we calculated the baseline by selecting all zero-prime trials where target quantity was either 1 or 2. Conversely, for target/prime +1, we selected all zero-prime trials where target quantity was either 2 or 3. Thus, baseline was defined specifically for each level of target-prime distance. Any effects of reaction time specific to target quantity should not be detected in this way as the baseline and the t-p distance of interest correspond to identical target numerosities. Group level analyses replicated the same results as obtained by a common zero-prime (F(4,64) = 10.36, p < 0.0001; One-sample t-test comparison with zero; for target/prime = -2, t(16)=3.67; p = 0.002; for target/prime = -1, t(16) = 2.09; p = 0.052; for target/prime = 0, t(16)= -2.395; p = 0.03; for target/prime = 1, t(16) = -2.20; p = 0.042; for target/prime = 2, t(16) = - 2.48; p = 0.024). Note that for target/prime identity, the results of the original and corrected analysis are identical since this includes all target quantities. Furthermore, the results also showed that in the no-prime condition, reaction times to 1, 2 or 3 target items 5

did not differ significantly (F(2,32) = 1.78, p > 0.1). Therefore, we calculated the baseline reaction time by collapsing across the target set numerosities in all the correctly enumerated no-prime trials. Experiment 2: Directional selectivity of the distance effects The priming effect depended on target/prime distance. For prime sets greater than the target set, enumeration judgment was slowed relative to no-prime baseline: for distance - 2, t(16)=3.03; p = 0.008; for distance -1, t(16) = 3.42; p = 0.003) all tests using onesample t-test comparison with zero. For prime sets equal to or smaller than the target set, enumeration judgment was speeded relative to no-prime baseline (One-sample t-test comparison with zero; for target identical to prime t(16)= -2.395; p = 0.03; for distance 1, t(16) = -2.40; p = 0.03; for distance 2, t(16) = 2.27; p = 0.04). Analysis of performance accuracy showed that participants were consistently and similarly accurate and near ceiling across all levels of target/prime distance (accuracy range: 94-98%; one way ANOVA, F(4,64)=1.47, p > 0.2. Analysis of performance accuracy showed a pattern similar to that obtained in Experiment 2 Direct comparison of Experiment 2 & 3 To compare the results of experiments 2 and 3 directly, RTs were normalised as z-scores relative to the no-prime baseline condition. For each participant, the mean RT from the no-prime condition was subtracted from the mean RTs at each level of target/prime distance, and divided by the standard deviation for the no-prime condition. We then performed a 2 x 5 ANOVA on Z scores with Experiment as one factor (2 levels) and target/prime distance as another factor (5 levels). An independent comparison of baseline 6

no-prime conditions across the two experiments did not show any significant difference. The main effect of target/prime distance was significant (F(4,112) = 5.73, p < 0.001), while neither the main effect of Experiment F(1,28) = 0.026, p > 0.8 nor the interaction between the two factors (F(4,112) = 0.132, p > 0.9) were significant. Experiment 4: Directional selectivity of the distance effects The priming effect depended on target/prime distance: for negative target/prime distances, RTs were slowed relative to the zero-prime baseline (for distance = -2, t(15)=2.26; p = 0.03; for distance = -1, t(15) = -0.11; p = 0.9; One-sample t-test comparison with zero) whereas for positive target/prime distances, RTs were faster relative to baseline (for distance = 0, t(15)= -3.55; p = 0.003; for distance = +1, t(15) = - 2.49; p = 0.025; for distance = +2, t(15) = -0.45; p = 0.6). Experiment 2, 3 and 4: the effect of absolute distance. In both Experiments 2 and 4, compared to no-prime baseline, enumeration RT was faster for identical prime and target numbers. For higher absolute distances, the enumeration RT increased relative to no-prime condition (One-way ANOVA, Experiment 2: F(2,32) = 6.43; p= 0.004; Experiment 4: F(2,30) = 6.53; p= 0.005). Thus, although the priming effects were in opposite directions for positive and negative distances, because of their asymmetric magnitude (e.g. compare t-p = -2 to t-p = 2 in Figure 2B), when collapsed together a net positive priming effect (i.e. interference) is obtained at large distances. In Experiment 3, on the other hand, the effect of absolute distance on priming effect was not significant (One-way ANOVA, Experiment 2: F(2,24) = 0.81; p= 0.4). 7

Experiment 4: Plotting RTs for conscious priming effects In Experiment 4, with monocular invisible primes, participants phenomenal experience was identical for non-zero and zero primes and therefore, the zero-prime trial served as proper baseline for measuring the priming effect. With visible primes, however, the phenomenal experience of the stimulus sequence was radically different for non-zero and zero-prime trials. For example, the appearance of the prime (irrespective of its magnitude) could act as an alerting signal informing the participant about the imminent appearance of the target set. We observed that this was indeed the case and RTs were generally faster under conscious prime conditions. We therefore decided that in the conscious prime condition, the priming effect was best demonstrated by the RTs themselves as depicted in Figure 3B. Supplementary Discussion The recent reports of asymmetric distance-dependent numerical priming such as those reported here as well as by others (Roggeman et al., 2007;Turconi et al., 2006;Van Opstal et al., 2008) point to the possibility of the existence of multiple cognitive processes underlying numerical judgments. For example, one possible multiple-stage interpretation of the asymmetric effects reported here is that targets of different numerosity may be differentially susceptible to priming. If the prime quantity were to be integrated in the decision process involving enumeration of the target, then having 3 items in the target set may leave little room for priming; on the other hand, enumeration of 1 target may be more strongly affected by more numerous primes. Thus, integration of unconscious prime information in the 8

enumeration decision may depend on a 2-stage process that takes prime information into account only when the target numerosity is less than maximal. Such interpretation predicts an interference effect for negative distances, which is, admittedly, confirmed by the data. However, this account does not predict any priming effect, and certainly no facilitation, for zero (identity priming) and positive distances which the data show repeatedly. It will be intriguing for future work to probe the contribution of such mechanisms to the subliminal priming effects we observed. A recent study that employed brief conscious primes in a number naming task showed that non-symbolic primes increased reaction times (relative to prime = target condition) with target enumeration only when prime set was smaller than the target set (Roggeman et al., 2007). Primes larger than targets did not affect naming time. Moreover, larger symbolic (digit) primes also appeared to slow dot number naming. Thus, the general pattern of the results reported in this earlier study of conscious primes is not the same as the pattern we found in the present study for unconscious (Experiments 2, 3 and 4; Figures 2B & 2C and Figure 3A) as well as conscious (Experiment 4; Figure 3B) primes. There were several important differences between the two studies that complicate comparison: dichoptic versus conventional presentation of the stimuli, direct enumeration by manual response versus number naming by verbal response, and extended (at least 1000ms) versus brief (<100ms) prime display time. Moreover, this previous work employed numbers up to five, whereas we used only up to three and hence within the subitizing range. It is possible that priming effects for numbers beyond the subitizing range may be different from within it: if one has to count the dots, then attention is on the 9

stimulus for longer and the process of extracting numerosity may be different. Further investigations are needed to clarify the role of each of these factors in the differences delineated by the two studies. Reference List Roggeman C, Verguts T, Fias W (2007) Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities. Cognition 105:380-394. Tsuchiya N, Koch C, Gilroy LA, Blake R (2006) Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. J Vis 6:1068-1078. Turconi E, Campbell JI, Seron X (2006) Numerical order and quantity processing in number comparison. Cognition 98:273-285. Van Opstal F, Gevers W, De Moor W, Verguts T (2008) Dissecting the symbolic distance effect: comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders. Psychon Bull Rev 15:419-425. 10