ORIGINAL ARTICLE. The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression

Similar documents
ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Heritability Estimates for Psychotic Disorders. important contribution to our understanding of genetic

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder:

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of

When Assessing Twin Concordance, Use the Probandwise Not the Pairwise Rate

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Tom Fowler, PhD; Stanley Zammit, PhD; Michael J. Owen, PhD; Finn Rasmussen, PhD

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. The Structure of Genetic and Environmental Risk Factors for Anxiety Disorders in Men and Women

Genetic Heterogeneity Between the Three Components of the Autism Spectrum: A Twin Study

Liability Threshold Models

Genetic and environmental risk factors for the weight and shape concerns characteristic of bulimia nervosa

LJEAVES II J EYSENCIZ 1\J G Nli\RTIN

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: A Meta-Analytic Review

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Family History of Psychiatric Illness as a Risk Factor for Schizoaffective Disorder

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

University of Groningen. Children of bipolar parents Wals, Marjolein

Manic-Depressive Illness

The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality

We expand our previous deterministic power

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Keywords: classical twin study, behavioural genetics, Mx, SEM, genetic model fitting

This is the peer reviewed version of the following article:

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins

The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence

Supplementary Online Content

Selecting a Control Group in Studies of the Familial Coaggregation of Two Disorders: A Quantitative Genetics Perspective

The heritability of alcohol use disorders: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies

Twin Imitation for Antisocial Behavior: Implications for Genetic and Family Environment Research

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

APPENDIX 11: CASE IDENTIFICATION STUDY CHARACTERISTICS AND RISK OF BIAS TABLES

Parental depression and offspring psychopathology: A Children of Twins study

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Substance use and perceived symptom improvement among patients with bipolar disorder and substance dependence

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

THE IMPACT of regular tobacco

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

Developing bipolar disorder. A study among children of patients with bipolar disorder Hillegers, Manon Hubertine Johanna

In Australian twins participating in three different

Reliability of a lifetime history of major depression: implications for heritability and co-morbidity

J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper

The objective of this study was to examine the

Assessment of Interrater Agreement for Multiple Nominal Responses Among Several Raters Chul W. Ahn, City of Hope National Medical Center

The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental Well-Being

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study

THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects.

Generalist Genes and Learning Disabilities

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection

Familial Aggregation of Eating Disorders: Results from a Controlled Family Study of Bulimia Nervosa

COMPUTING READER AGREEMENT FOR THE GRE

The Genetics of Major Depressive Disorder

Obsessive compulsive disorder, tics and anxiety in 6-year-old twins

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

King s Research Portal

Twin Studies of Suicidal Behavior

Path Analysis, SEM and the Classical Twin Model

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden

CHAPTER 6. Conclusions and Perspectives

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Diagnosis of Mental Disorders. Historical Background. Rise of the Nomenclatures. History and Clinical Assessment

Genetic Relationships Between Schizophrenia, Bipolar Disorder, and Schizoaffective Disorder

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis

Pamela A. F. Madden, Ph.D., Kathleen K. Bucholz, Ph.D., Nicholas G. Martin, Ph.D., and Andrew C. Heath, D.Phil.

Genetic and environmental influences on eating behaviors in 2.5- and 9-year-old children: a longitudinal twin study

Supplementary Online Content

Genetic and environmental influence on asthma: a populationbased study of 11,688 Danish twin pairs

Goal: To identify the extent to which different aspects of psychopathology might be in some way inherited

The Diagnostic Stability of DSM-IV Diagnoses: An Examination of Major Depressive Disorder, Bipolar I Disorder, and Schizophrenia in Korean Patients

The Schizophrenia Proneness (SzP) Scale: An MMPI-2 Measure of Schizophrenia Liability

It is shown that maximum likelihood estimation of

Biosocial characteristics of patients with paranoid schizophrenia

Testing Hypotheses Regarding the Causes of Comorbidity: Examining the Underlying Deficits of Comorbid Disorders

Genetic Segregation Analysis of Early-Onset Recurrent Unipolar Depression

Common and unique genetic contributions to conduct disorder and two stages of alcohol dependence development in women

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Prospective assessment of treatment use by patients with personality disorders

Genetic and environmental influences on the relationship between peer alcohol use and own alcohol use in adolescents

Resemblance between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2)

BMC Psychiatry. Open Access. Abstract. BioMed Central

Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample

Supplementary appendix

Sibling Resemblance for Psychiatric Disorders in Offspring at High and Low Risk for Depression

Course and Outcome for Schizophrenia Versus Other Psychotic Patients: A Longitudinal Study

Testing a Genetic Structure of Blood-Injury-Injection Fears

CRITICAL ANALYSIS PROBLEMS

Personality traits predict current and future functioning comparably for individuals with major depressive and personality disorders

Article. Family History of Suicidal Behavior and Mood Disorders in Probands With Mood Disorders

Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study

Article. Low Extraversion and High Neuroticism as Indices of Genetic and Environmental Risk for Social Phobia, Agoraphobia, and Animal Phobia

Transcription:

ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression Peter McGuffin, MB, PhD, FRCP, FRCPsych; Fruhling Rijsdijk, PhD; Martin Andrew, MB, MRCPsych; Pak Sham, BM, PhD, MRCPsych; Randy Katz, PhD; Alastair Cardno, MB, PhD, MRCPsych Background: Twin studies of bipolar affective disorder (BPD) have either been small or have not used explicit diagnostic criteria. There has been little use of genetic model fitting and no analyses to explore the etiological overlap with unipolar depression (UPD). Methods: Sixty-seven twin pairs, 30 monozygotic and 37 dizygotic, in which the proband had BPD were ascertained, and lifetime diagnoses were made using DSM-IV criteria. Univariate models were applied to estimate the contribution of additive genetic and environmental effects. Bipolar data were then combined with those from 68 monozygotic and 109 dizygotic pairs in which the proband had UPD. Two models were explored: a classic 2-threshold approach, in which BPD and UPD occupy the same continuum of liability but differ in severity, and a correlated liability model of mania and depression. Results: Heritability of BPD was estimated at 85% (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.73-0.93) using narrow concordance and 89% (95% CI, 0.61-1.0) using broad concordance, with no shared environmental effects detected. A 2-threshold model was an unsatisfactory fit. Fitting a correlated liability model revealed a genetic correlation of 0.65 (95% CI, 0.58-0.75) between mania and depression and a correlation of 0.59 (95% CI, 0.15-0.84) for nonfamilial environment. Approximately 71% of the genetic variance for mania was not shared with depression. Conclusions: As defined by the DSM-IV, BPD is highly heritable. There are substantial genetic and nonshared environmental correlations between mania and depression, but most of the genetic variance in liability to mania is specific to the manic syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:497-502 From the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King s College London, London, England (Drs McGuffin, Rijsdijk, and Sham); the Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Wales College of Medicine, Cardiff (Drs Andrew and Cardno); and the Department of Psychology, Toronto University, Toronto, Ontario (Dr Katz). THERE IS general acceptance that genes make an important contribution to the pathogenesis of bipolar affective disorder (BPD), and much recent effort has been aimed at locating and identifying the genes involved. 1 However, several problems and questions relating to the basic genetic epidemiology of BPD still need to be addressed. The first is that the data supporting a genetic pathogenesis for BPD are slender compared with the evidence supporting a genetic basis for other psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia. Although there is consistent evidence of an increase in the frequency of both BPD and unipolar depression (UPD) in the close relatives of probands with BPD, 1,2 the adoption data on BPD 3-5 are based on small samples, one of which showed no evidence of a genetic effect. 5 The largest and most widely cited twin study is that of Bertelsen et al, 6 who derived a sample of 34 monozygotic and 37 dizygotic pairs from Danish twin and psychiatric registers. Although the results were clear in showing a substantially higher monozygotic than dizygotic concordance, the study used clinical diagnostic criteria rather than modern operationalized criteria. A study of Swedish twins by Kendler et al 7 had the advantage of being based on a large epidemiological sample, but this yielded only 13 monozygotic and 22 dizygotic pairs. Criteria from the DSM-III-R were used, but these were derived from questionnaire assessments. All other published twin studies on BPD, except for data briefly described in our own report on the Maudsley Twin Psychosis Series, 8 contained very small numbers of twins. 9-11 A second problem is that the size of the genetic effect is not clear from the data published so far. A reanalysis of the Danish twin data of Bertelsen et al suggested high heritability, but this study adopted a simple approach, lumping together UPD and BPD in co-twins. 2 At least moderate 497

heritability was also suggested by the analysis of the Swedish twin register data, 7 but the very small number of twin pairs inevitably meant that confidence intervals on estimates of heritability were wide. A third problem is the relationship between UPD and BPD. Family genetic studies have been influential in gaining widespread acceptance of the proposal, originally put forward by Leonhard, 12 that major affective disorders can be divided into these 2 broad types. However, the consistent finding that there is an excess of both UPD and BPD among the relatives of probands with BPD suggests at least some degree of genetic overlap. 1,2 The extent of this presumed overlap has not yet been quantified. This presents problems for gene-finding studies, which, to date, have largely concentrated on BPD in families with multiple affected individuals. 1 The difficulty then arises as to how to classify the relatives in such families who are affected by UPD, and many researchers have dealt with this by analyzing their data under 2 or more diagnostic models, for example, a narrow model, in which only subjects with BPD are classified as affected, and a broad model, in which relatives with UPD or BPD are classified as affected. In this study, we estimate the heritability of operationally defined BPD and carry out model fitting to assess the extent to which the same or different genes contribute to UPD and BPD using data from the Maudsley Hospital Twin Register in London, England. This sample is the largest clinically ascertained series of twins with affective disorders that has been studied to date. METHODS SUBJECTS Probands were ascertained from the Maudsley Twin Register. The register contains all patients of multiple births since 1948 who attended any facility of the Maudsley and Bethlem Royal hospitals for clinical reasons unrelated to being a twin and who had a same-sex co-twin surviving at least until the age of 15 years. The twins included in the present study were ascertained via probands who had an initial hospital diagnosis of affective disorder, psychosis (mania and depressive psychosis), or both. The same ascertainment procedures were used in both series, except that the ascertainment period was 1948-1985 for the affective disorder series and 1948-1992 for the psychosis series. As reported in a previous study, 13 our initial focus on the affective disorder series was on UPD, so the 34 twin pairs in that study who were found to have had 1 or more episodes of mania were removed from the analysis. In the psychosis study reported by Cardno et al, 8 there were 58 pairs in which the proband had 1 or more episodes of mania or hypomania. Twenty-five pairs were included in both studies. Therefore, combining the 2 data sets gives a total of 67 pairs, 30 monozygotic and 37 dizygotic, ascertained via probands with BPD. One of the monozygotic pairs contained a double proband and was counted twice (ie, there were 29 independent monozygotic pairs). The assignment of zygosity was based on blood types and/or questionnaires, as previously described. 8,13 There were 68 monozygotic and 109 dizygotic pairs ascertained via probands with UPD. 13 DIAGNOSTIC ASSESSMENT Diagnoses were based on all available clinical information concerning each twin, including a research interview. As previously described, 8,13 research interviews 14-16 were conducted with 73% of probands and 60% of co-twins from the psychosis series and 40% of probands and 40% of co-twins from the affective disorder series. In addition, information was gathered from case notes, case summaries, and relatives and primary care physicians. Diagnostic information was assessed by researchers blind to zygosity and the identity of the twins. Main lifetime diagnoses of DSM-III-R disorders were made using the OPCRIT checklist and its associated computer diagnostic program 17 for the subjects in the psychosis series. Interrater agreement for affective disorders of all types averaged 91% ( =0.68). For the 34 pairs in the affective disorder series, nonblind ratings on a modified OPCRIT checklist were made by one of us (P.M.) and checked against ratings blind to zygosity and co-twin information made by another author (M.A.). Agreement for all types of DSM-IV affective disorder was 97% ( =0.90). Any diagnostic differences in the 25 overlapping pairs were resolved by consensus, and DSM-III-R diagnoses were translated to DSM-IV diagnoses. To allow adequate numbers for statistical analysis, bipolar I and bipolar II disorders were lumped together. GENETIC ANALYSIS The 30 monozygotic and 37 dizygotic proband co-twin pairs were used to calculate probandwise concordance rates. The population lifetime morbid risk for BPD (mania and depression or mania alone) was 0.32%, which was estimated using Camberwell case register data (Camberwell is the old borough of London where Maudsley Hospital is located), as previously described. 8 Similarly, Camberwell data provide a sex-averaged lifetime risk estimate for DSM-IV UPD of approximately 6%. 13 UNIVARIATE MODEL FITTING The data on the 67 BPD proband co-twin pairs were first analyzed under a liability-threshold model with a single threshold using the Mx program. 18 Two sets of biometrical model fitting were then conducted. In the first, concordance was defined narrowly; only pairs in which the co-twin had a diagnosis of BPD were classified as affected. Second, a broader diagnostic perspective was taken, and pairs in which the co-twin had either UPD or BPD were classified as concordant. For both the broad and the narrow definitions of concordance, 4 models were fitted: a model containing additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and specific environmental (E) variance; an additive genetic and specific environmental variance model (AE); a common environmental and specific environmental variance model (CE); and an individual specific environmental model (E). Models were compared using 2 difference for nested models, and the Akaike information criterion (AIC), which is equal to 2 minus twice the df was used as an aid to selecting the best-fitting model on the grounds of parsimony and goodness of fit. MODELLING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN UPD AND BPD Two approaches were adopted, both of which are extensions of the simple single-threshold model. Multiple Thresholds Reich et al 19 have pointed out that when a disorder exists in 2 forms that show familial overlap and when 1 form is comparatively common, the broad form, and the other is less common, the narrow form, the relationship between the 2 can be explored using a 2-threshold model. If the 2 forms are on the same continuum of liability, with the narrow form occurring 498

beyond a more extreme threshold then the broad form, an isocorrelational model should fit. That is, the correlations within the first disorder, r 11, and within the second disorder, r 22, should be equal to the cross-correlations r 12 and r 21. If, on the other hand, the 2 disorders are independent (ie, show a familial overlap that is no different from chance), then the cross-disorder correlations r 12 and r 21 will not differ significantly from 0. The isocorrelational and the independent model can be compared with the general model in which all 4 correlations are estimated and, again, models can be compared using 2 analysis and the best-fitting model selected with the aid of the AIC. These analyses were performed using the FORTAN program TWIN2. 20 A C a c C C E C A S C S E S e c c c e c c c a s c s e s a c L D L M 1.0 1.0 Depression Mania Correlated Liability In contrast to the multiple-threshold approach, which retains the DSM-IV hierarchy whereby each subject has a single Axis I diagnosis, the correlated liability approach allows individuals to be classified as having mania, depression, or both. Again the correlation between mania and depression is partitioned into additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and specific environmental (E) sources in a Cholesky de-composition 18 (Figure). For all 3 sources of variation, there are common factors affecting both disorders and specific factors influencing the second disorder only. The effect of a common additive genetic factor, A c, on the first disorder is represented by the path coefficient a c. The effect on the second disorder is represented by a c. The effect of specific additive genetic factors, A s, on the second disorder is represented by the path coefficient a s. In addition to the relative magnitudes of the A, C, and E effects, the correlation between the 2 liabilities can be estimated for each of the components of variance (r a, r c, and r e ) indicating the extent to which genetic and environmental effects are shared between the 2 disorders. Correlated liability models were fitted using the Mx program, 18 with a goodness of fit assessed by likelihood ratio 2 analysis. Again, nested models were compared by 2 difference and AIC. As we have described elsewhere, 21 a complication of fitting this type of model is that when ascertainment is probandwise, pairs in which neither twin is affected are omitted from observation. This introduces a potential bias that can be adjusted for by dividing the likelihood of the data by the proportion of the population remaining after ascertainment. Additionally, 3 ascertainment parameters (b, c, and d) were modeled to account for unequal ascertainment probability across disorders. The scalar b represents adjustment of the probability of ascertaining probands with mania only, c of ascertaining probands with depression only, and d of ascertaining probands with both disorders. With the given data set, only 2 of these can be estimated; therefore, we fixed b to 1, and c and d were estimated. RESULTS The probandwise concordance in the 67 pairs ascertained via probands with BPD is presented in Table 1; for comparison, we also provide concordances for pairs ascertained via probands with UPD, as previously reported. 12 Among the twins ascertained via probands with BPD, monozygotic concordance was significantly higher than dizygotic concordance, whether concordance was defined broadly (co-twin had either UPD or BPD) ( 2 1=15.69; P.001) or narrowly (co-twin had BPD) ( 2 1=11.99; P.001; Fisher exact test P.001). The results of univariate model fitting are presented in Table 2. For both definitions of concordance, the data were best explained by an AE model with Path diagram for the correlated liability model. The correlation between mania and depression is partitioned into additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and specific environmental (E) sources in a Cholesky de-composition. For A, C, and E, there are both common factors affecting disorders and specific factors influencing the second disorder only. The effect of common additive genetic factors, A c, on the first disorder is represented by the path coefficient a c, and the effect on the second disorder is represented by a c. The effect of specific additive genetic factors, A s,onthe second disorder is represented by the path coefficient a s. L indicates liability. a heritability of 0.89 (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.61-1.00, with the upper estimate at a bound) for the broad diagnosis and a heritability of 0.85 (95% CI, 0.73-0.93) for the narrow diagnosis. The CE and E models could be rejected. TWO-THRESHOLD MODEL Because we found no evidence of shared environmental effects, either in our univariate analysis of the bipolar sample or in our previous analysis of twins ascertained via probands with UPD, 12 we fit a simplified 2-threshold model in which the only source of correlation was additive genetic effects, such that the within- and acrossdisorder tetrachoric correlations for monozygotic pairs were twice that for dizygotic pairs. The isocorrelational model was a poor fit ( 2 7=14.79; P=.04), and the independent model was a very poor fit, ( 2 6=5517.2; P.001). The data are best explained by the general model in which the within- and across-disorder correlations are unconstrained ( 2 4=1.76; P=.78). We can conclude that BPD and UPD are not genetically independent, but a straightforward 2-threshold model, in which BPD is a more severe form of the disorder on the same continuum of liability as UPD, can be rejected. CORRELATED LIABILITY Fitting a correlated liability model requires that we relax the usual hierarchical diagnostic rules and classify the subjects as having mania, depression, or both. Twentyone of 98 monozygotic probands and 25 of 146 dizygotic probands had both depression and mania corresponding to within-proband across-disorder correlations in liability of 0.64 (95% CI, 0.52-0.74) and 0.63 (95% CI, 0.53-0.72), respectively. The within-pair concordances and correlations in liability within and across disorders are presented in Table 3. These were then used in fitting the correlated liability models, the results of which are summarized in Table 4. Adding the ascertainment parameters c and d improved the fit of the ACE model substantially, and there was not a significant worsening 499

Table 1. Probandwise Concordance for Unipolar (UPD) and Bipolar (BPD) Affective Disorder (AD) Co-Twin Diagnosis Monozygotic Dizygotic Proband Diagnosis N UPD, % BPD, % Total AD, % N UPD, % BPD, % Total AD, % BPD 30 26.7 40 66.7 37 13.5 5.4 18.9 UPD 68 44.1 1.5 45.6 109 20.2 0.0 20.2 Table 2. Univariate Model Fitting on Twins Ascertained via Bipolar Probands* Diagnosis Model A C E df 2 P Value AIC Broad ACE 0.89 0 0.11 NA 2.70 NA NA AE 0.89 [0.0] 0.11 1 2.70.52 0.695 CE [0.0] 0.64 0.36 1 21.02.001 19.02 E [0.0] [0.0] 1 2 73.61.001 69.61 Narrow ACE 0.85 0 0.15 NA 0.03 NA NA AE 0.85 [0.0] 0.15 1 0.03.86 1.97 CE [0.0] 0.69 0.31 1 12.75.001 10.75 E [0.0] [0.0] 1 2 103.03.001 99.03 Abbreviations: A, C, and E, proportions of variance explored by additive genetic, common environmental, and specific environmental effects, respectively; AIC, Akaike information criterion; NA, not applicable. *Parameters in brackets are constrained. Pairs were classified as concordant if the co-twin had unipolar or bipolar disorder. Pairs were classified as concordant if the co-twin had bipolar disorder. Table 3. Within-Pair Concordances and Correlations in Liability, Within and Across Disorders Diagnosis No. (%) of Subjects Total Correlation in Liability (95% CI) Proband Co-twin Monozygotic Dizygotic Monozygotic Dizygotic Depression Depression 43 (48)/89 27 (20)/134 0.76 (0.65-0.85) 0.37 (0.23-0.50) Mania Mania 12 (40)/30 2 (5)/37 0.85 (0.73-0.93) 0.41 (0.12-0.63) Depression Mania 8 (10)/79 2 (1)/134 Mania Depression 15 (50)/30 7 (19)/37 0.56 (0.44-0.66) 0.23 (0.08-0.37) Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval. Table 4. Fitting the Correlated Liability Model of Depression and Mania* Depression Mania Model 2 (df ) P Value AIC r g r e A C E A C E ACE c=d=1 145.2 (15).001 115.2 0.76 0.54 0.66 0.04 0.30 0.81 0.07 0.12 c and d Free 14.5 (13).24 11.5 0.69 0.45 0.67 0.05 0.28 0.80 0.05 0.15 AE c and d Free 14.6 (16).44 17.4 0.58 0.47 0.71 [0.0] 0.29 0.85 [0.0] 0.15 c = 1; d Free 36.9 (17).004 2.9 0.33 0.10 0.72 [0.0] 0.28 0.84 [0.0] 0.16 c Free; d=1 14.6 (17).62 19.4 0.65 (0.58-0.75) 0.59 (0.15-0.84) 0.72 (0.60-0.81) [0.0] 0.28 (0.19-0.40) 0.85 (0.74-0.93) [0.0] 0.15 (0.07-0.26) Abbreviations: A, C, and E, proportions of variance explored by additive genetic, common evironmental, and specific environmental effects, respectively; AIC, Akaike information criterion; c and d, ascertainment parameters (see the Methods section for explanation); r g and r e, correlations for genetic and common environment, respectively. *Parameters in brackets are constrained. The best-fitting model, for which 95% confidence intervals around the parameter estimate are given. of fit when the family environment, C, was constrained to be 0, suggesting again that additive genetic factors are sufficient to explain the familiality and covariance of mania and depression. We next tested the effects of fixing the ascertainment parameters. The results indicate that the c parameter is significantly different from 1, whereas d is not. This suggests that probands with only depression are less likely to be ascertained than those who have both depression and mania. We also fitted models not presented in Table 4 con- 500

straining r e and r g. Fixing the environmental correlation, r e, at 0 resulted in a significant decline in fit ( 2 1=7.1; P.001), and there was a very marked worsening of fit when the genetic correlation, r g, was constrained to be 0 ( 2 1=77.9; P.001). This suggests that the observed correlation between mania and depression results both from overlapping genetic influences and overlapping nonfamilial environmental effects. We can conclude that the AE model with the ascertainment parameter d fixed at 1 and c estimated best explains the data on the grounds of parsimony and goodness of fit. The heritability estimate for mania based on this model was 0.85 (95% CI, 0.74-0.93) and that for depression was 0.72 (95% CI, 0.6-0.81). The genetic variance of mania explained by common genetic influences with depression was estimated to be 0.25, and the variance explained by specific genetic influences was estimated to be 0.60. The genetic variance in liability to mania is determined by the effect of a common additive genetic factor, A c, which influences the first and second disorder (represented by the path a c ), and an additive genetic factor specific to mania, A s (with the corresponding path a s ). The proportion of variance in liability to mania explained by common genetic influences with depression (a c /a c +a s +e c +e s ) was estimated to be 0.25 (2.499/ 9.971), and the proportion of variance explained by specific genetic influences was 0.60 (a s /a c +a s +e c +e s =5.887/9.971). The sum of the proportions of variance in liability to mania explained by both specific and common A factors sum to the heritability estimate (0.85). Therefore, 0.60/0.85, or approximately 71%, of the genetic variance in liability to mania is estimated to be specific to mania and not shared with depression. COMMENT We reported the concordance rates of a series of twins ascertained through 67 probands with BPD. This is the largest twin study of BPD since that of Bertelsen et al, 6 and it has a very similar pattern of findings; we found a broad concordance of 67% in monozygotic twins and 19% in dizygotic twins compared with a broad proband concordance of 79% in monozygotic twins and 19% in dizygotic twins in the study by Bertelsen et al. 6 However, our study includes several methodological advances; we made the diagnoses according to reliable operational criteria (DSM-IV), the diagnostic information on co-twins was assessed blind to zygosity and status (ie, whether the subject was a proband or co-twin), and modern model fitting procedures were applied. Univariate model fitting resulted in estimates of heritability in excess of 80% (with a lower confidence limit of more than 70%) whether a broad or narrow diagnostic perspective was taken, suggesting that all of the familiality of BPD could be accounted for by additive genetic effects with no contribution from family environment. We next examined the relationship between UPD and BPD by combining the 67 pairs in the BPD series with 177 twins ascertained via probands with UPD from the same register. We first applied a classic 2-threshold model to test the assumption that BPD and UPD occupy the same continuum of liability, whereby all affected individuals lie beyond the first threshold for being affected but only those who exceed a second, more extreme threshold are diagnosed with BPD. Applying the isocorrelational test, as suggested by Reich et al, 19 we rejected this model. We were also able to reject a model in which BPD and UPD are etiologically distinct. It could be argued that an independent model is, in any case, implausible because in our 2-threshold analysis we classified subjects using conventional diagnostic rules, according to which individuals receive a single diagnosis and BPD trumps UPD. However, we then explored a correlated liability model, in which subjects were classified as having no disorder, 1 disorder (either mania or depression), or 2 disorders (mania and depression). Fitting a model of this type necessitated applying an ascertainment correction to take into account the fact that we ascertained our twins through probands affected either by mania or depression. 21 We also included estimation of ascertainment parameters in our model, which, perhaps unsurprisingly, suggested that the probability of ascertainment is higher for individuals with both mania and depression than for those with depression alone. The genetic correlation between mania and depression was substantial, 0.65, and could not be dropped from the model without worsening the fit. The heritability estimate for mania, 0.85, was similar to that obtained in the univariate model fitting of BPD. This left an estimated 15% of variance in liability to mania explained by individual specific or nonfamilial environmental effects. Given this comparatively modest estimate of environmental effects, it is of interest that the correlation between the environmental influences for mania and depression were estimated to be substantial (0.59, albeit with a large confidence interval), and environmental correlation could not be dropped from the model without worsening the fit. We were also able to estimate the size of the specific genetic influence for mania and found that, although the genetic effects on mania and depression are correlated, most of the genetic influence on liability to mania, approximately 71%, is distinct from the genetic liability to depression. The main limitation of this study is the sample size. Our analyses were carried out on 67 twin pairs ascertained via probands with BPD and 177 pairs ascertained via probands with UPD, giving us a total of 244 pairs, which constitutes the largest clinically ascertained series of twins with affective disorder yet reported. However, the sample size is small by modern twin study standards, especially when compared with studies of other behaviors and disorders based on population-derived samples. 22 This means that we are able to say with some confidence that BPD is substantially heritable. However, that we did not find any evidence of common environmental effects is not conclusive evidence that such effects do not exist; typically, much larger samples are required to detect small familial environmental effects. 23 Although most subjects underwent a structured or semistructured interview, a substantial minority were not interviewed, so we relied on a combination of written material, such as case notes, and information provided by informants. Nevertheless, we were able to achieve high 501

levels of interrater agreement when data from all sources were combined. Finally, our morbid risk estimates were extrapolated from local case register data rather then being based directly on population estimates. Nevertheless, the pattern of results and the estimates of heritability were similar to those of Kendler et al 7 from their population-based twin study. Kendler et al also assumed somewhat higher morbid risks of mania or mania plus hypomania. We therefore checked the effect of assuming a higher population risk of BPD of 1%. This resulted in a slight reduction in the estimate of heritability in the univariate analysis to 0.80 but did not change the pattern of results otherwise. In particular, the fit of the 2-threshold isocorrelational model became even poorer ( 2 7=40.89; P.001). CONCLUSIONS Our study confirms that there is a large genetic contribution to the variance in liability for DSM-IV BPD and showed substantial genetic overlap with UPD. However, a liability threshold model in which UPD and BPD occupy the same continuum, but with BPD being the more severe of the two, does not fit. The relationship between BPD and UPD is better described by a correlated liability model. Fitting such a model shows us that there are significant genetic and nonfamilial environmental correlations between mania and depression but that most of the genetic variance in liability to mania is specific to the manic syndrome. Submitted for publication June 27, 2002; final revision received October 3, 2002; accepted November 8, 2002. This study was supported by grant MH44359 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md, and grants from the Medical Research Council, London, and the Stanley Foundation, Bethesda. This work was carried out while Dr Cardno was a Medical Research Council Clinical Training Fellow. We thank Jane Marshall, MD, Bina Coid, PhD, Alison Macdonald, PhD, Tracy Ribchester, PhD, Nadia Davies, PhD, Piero Venturi, MD, Lisa Jones, PhD, Shôn Lewis, MD, Joan Rutherford, MD, Irving Gottesman, PhD, Anne Farmer, PhD, Adrianne Reveley, MD, and Robin Murray, MD, for their work on this study Corresponding author: Peter McGuffin, MD, PhD, FRCP, FRCPsych, Social, Genetic, and Developmental Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, England (e-mail: p.mcguffin@iop.kcl.ac.uk). REFERENCES 1. Jones I, Kent L, Craddock N. The genetics of affective disorders. In: McGuffin P, Owen MJ, Gottesman II, eds. Psychiatric Genetics and Genomics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2002. 2. McGuffin P, Katz R. The genetics of depression and manic-depressive disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1989;155:294-304. 3. Mendlewicz J, Rainer JD. Adoption study supporting genetic transmission in manicdepressive illness. Nature. 1977;268:327-329. 4. Wender PH, Kety SS, Rosenthal D, Schlusinger F, Ortmann J, Lunde I. Psychiatric disorders in the biological and adoptive families of adopted individuals with affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43:923-929. 5. Von Knorring AL, Cloninger CR, Bohman M, Sigvardsson S. An adoption study of depressive disorders and substance abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40: 943-950. 6. Bertelsen A, Harvald B, Hauge M. A Danish twin study of manic-depressive disorders. Br J Psychiatry. 1977;130:330-351. 7. Kendler KS, Pedersen N, Johnson L, Neale MC, Mathe AA. A pilot Swedish twin study of affective illness, including hospital- and population-ascertained subsamples. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:699-706. 8. Cardno AG, Marshall EJ, Coid B, Macdonald AM, Ribchester TR, Davies NJ, Venturi P, Jones LA, Lewis SW, Sham PC, Gottesman II, Farmer AE, McGuffin P, Reveley AM, Murray RM. Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders: the Maudsley Twin Psychosis Series. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:162-168. 9. Kringlen E. Heredity and Environment in the Functional Psychoses. London, England: William Heinemann; 1967. 10. Allen MG, Cohen S, Pollin W, Greenspan SI. Affective illness in veteran twins: a diagnostic review. Am J Psychiatry. 1974;131:1234-1239. 11. Torgersen S. Genetic factors in moderately severe and mild affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43:222-226. 12. Leonhard K. The Classification of the Endogenous Psychoses. 5th ed. Berman R, trans. Robins E, ed. New York, NY: Wiley & Sons; 1959. 13. McGuffin P, Katz R, Watkins S, Rutherford J. A hospital-based twin register of the heritability of DSM-IV unipolar depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53: 129-136. 14. Gottesman II, Shields J. Schizophrenia and Genetics: A Twin Study Vantage Point. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1972. 15. McGuffin P, Katz R, Aldrich J. Past and present State Examination: the assessment of lifetime ever psychopathology. Psychol Med. 1986;16:461-465. 16. Spitzer RL, Endicott J. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia Lifetime Version. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1978. 17. McGuffin P, Farmer A, Harvey I. A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:764-770. 18. Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH. Mx: Statistical Modelling. 5th ed. Richmond: Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University; 1999. 19. Reich T, Rice J, Cloninger CR, Wette R, James J. The use of multiple thresholds and segregation analysis in analyzing the phenotypic heterogeneity of multifactorial traits. Ann Hum Genet. 1979;42:371-390. 20. Thapar A, Harrington R, McGuffin P. Examining the comorbidity of ADHDrelated behaviours and conduct problems using a twin study design. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:224-229. 21. Cardno AG, Rijsdijk FV, Sham PC, Murray RM, McGuffin P. A twin study of genetic relationships between psychotic symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159: 539-545. 22. Martin N, Boomsma D, Machin G. A twin-pronged attack on complex traits. Nat Genet. 1997;17:387-392. 23. Rutter M, Silberg J, O Conner T, Simonoff E. Genetics and child psychiatry, I: advances in quantitative and molecular genetics. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999; 40:3-18. 502