UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

Similar documents
Classical Conditioning

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

The Contribution of Neuroscience to Understanding Human Behaviour

PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015

Two Process Account of Aversive Classical Conditioning: Amygdala Modulation of Cerebellar-dependent Eyeblink Conditioning

Psychological Foundations of Curriculum. Kevin Thompson

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

Self-Awareness SEL. Social and Emotional Learning (SEL) at MPS: Social and Emotional Learning is a priority area for Minneapolis

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm

On the Time of Peripheral Sensations and Voluntary Motor Actions. Text

2. Hull s theory of learning is represented in a mathematical equation and includes expectancy as an important variable.

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

NCERT Solutions Class 11 Psychology. Chapter - The Bases of Human Behaviour

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AB Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科目簡介

KINE 4500 Neural Control of Movement. Lecture #1:Introduction to the Neural Control of Movement. Neural control of movement

Associative learning

LEARNING. Learning. Type of Learning Experiences Related Factors

COMMENTARY. Mechanism Through Methodology: No Madness to the Method

Taken From The Brain Top to Bottom //

Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness

From Cells to Concepts

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

Sensory Processing Disorder

GRADE LEVEL AND SUBJECT: ADVANCED PLACEMENT PSYCHOLOGY (11 TH AND 12 TH )

Psychology Formative Assessment #2 Answer Key

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

PSYCHOLOGY CONTENT STANDARDS

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

Motivation represents the reasons for people's actions, desires, and needs. Typically, this unit is described as a goal

Clinical Reasoning for Western Acupuncture

V. List the major objectives of the proposed minor and describe its chief features briefly.

INDIAN HILL EXEMPTED VILLAGE SCHOOL DISTRICT Social Studies Curriculum - May 2009 Psychology Semester Course

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

4/15/2018. Neuroplasticity: Is it for Real? Neuroplasticity Research. Neuroplasticity Research. You can wire and re-wire your brain!

CHAPTER 15 SKINNER'S OPERANT ANALYSIS 4/18/2008. Operant Conditioning

Running head: FREEDOM, CHOICE & RESPONSIBILITY 1

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction

Next Level Practitioner

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

Learning & Language Development

Study of the Brain. Notes

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien.

Why trace and delay conditioning are sometimes (but not always) hippocampal dependent: A computational model

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

EDUCATE ME INCLUDE ME. Learning conversations about epilepsy. A parent and teacher s resource

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

BRAIN PLASTICITY. Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

Degree of freedom problem

Oscillatory Neural Network for Image Segmentation with Biased Competition for Attention

Complementarity and the Relation Between Psychological and Neurophysiological Phenomena

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence

Seminar: Skinner s Analysis of Verbal Behavior PRIVATE EVENTS

Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Error Correction in Classical Conditioning and Human Category Learning

Basic definition and Classification of Anhedonia. Preclinical and Clinical assessment of anhedonia.

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

What is Learned? Lecture 9

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

Chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Practical Brain-Focused Strategies for Working with Depression

Concluding Remarks. Mental illness is ubiquitous. Mental illnesses exist at the extremes of normal behavior

Do Generosity and Kindness Change Circuits in the Brain? Robert Brooks, Ph.D.

Lecture 2.1 What is Perception?

5.8 Departure from cognitivism: dynamical systems

A neurocomputational model of classical conditioning phenomena: A putative role for the hippocampal region in associative learning

Frequently Asked Questions

Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses

Integrating Cognition, Emotion and Autonomy

Education Options for Children with Autism

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

Lesson 14. The Nervous System. Introduction to Life Processes - SCI 102 1

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

Study Plan: Session 1

Hoboken Public Schools. Advanced Placement Psychology Curriculum

Chapter 7 - Learning

Psychology, Ch. 6. Learning Part 1

The Emotional Nervous System

To understand AD, it is important to

THOUGHTS, ATTITUDES, HABITS AND BEHAVIORS

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Transcription:

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title A Continuum of Learning and Memory Research Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9q3031wx Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18(1) ISSN 2168-3344 Authors Sokoloff, Greta Steinmetz, Joseph E. Publication Date 2005-12-31 Peer reviewed escholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California

International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2005, 18, 38-41. Copyright 2004 by the International Society for Comparative Psychology COMMENTARY A Continuum of Learning and Memory Research Greta Sokoloff and Joseph E. Steinmetz Indiana University, U.S.A. History has revealed time and time again that science is moved forward by revolutions that pit one point of view, theory, or methodology, against an opposing view. During calmer times, however, we as researchers are left to our own devices and settle into our work with little thought to the world around us. The field of learning and memory has been privy to many such revolutions in the past but has yet to form a cohesive, modern message. Grau and Joynes suggest that our strong ties to the past are to blame for a lack of progression in the field. We agree and add that the focus of the field on two extreme ends of a continuum has also held us back; suggesting that research that goes on in the middle of the continuum may be the key to leading the field out of its rut. Grau and Joynes (2005) posit that there are two core assumptions that maintain and nourish research in the field of learning and memory. The first assumption, which is not problematic to the authors (nor to us), is that learning is essential. The second assumption, however, is where Grau and Joynes assert that the field has been led down a problematic path (p. 2) that the interpretation of the assumption of generality in learning has led the field astray. Specifically, the study of phenomena with traditional methodologies that manipulate relationships between stimuli and responses, such as Pavlovian, operant, or instrumental conditioning paradigms, has resulted in the methodologies and procedures themselves taking precedence over the phenomena being studied. Grau and Joynes have pointed out an important problem in the field of learning and memory. The way in which many in the field of learning cling to traditions established long ago is troublesome not only for the research we perform and the data we obtain but also for the dissemination of the field, and our findings, to others. This problem is especially evident in the university classroom. Few would argue the fact that when we construct a curriculum for a course on learning and memory, we perpetuate the very problem in the field that Grau and Joynes identify. Not yet has a comprehensive text been written that has even come close to tearing down the traditional house (p. 2) thereby reinforcing a segregation of theory with no attempt at synthesis. Although it is quite evident that the way in which we teach learning to students is in desperate need of modernization it is also important for a synthesis across research programs. Many of us, however, do strive to achieve this goal. In fact, the incursion of the problem of generality in research, as emphasized by Grau and Joynes, arises from a blurring of two extreme ends of a continuum. To this extent, the authors criticism of using particular methodologies setting up shop in a single room (p. 2) although important, may be a bit overstated. Please address correspondence to Joseph E. Steinmetz, Department of Psychology, Indiana University, 1101 E. 10 th Street, Bloomington, IN 47405-7007, U.S.A. (steinmet@indiana.edu).

- 39 - Researchers who study behavioral and biological underpinnings of learning and memory, like researchers involved in many scientific disciplines, have research interests that can be placed on a continuum. At one end of the continuum there are researchers interested in describing and uncovering the laws of behavior based primarily on a history of traditional learning theory and animal behavior. Not surprisingly, these researchers use traditional methodologies to achieve that goal. At the other end of the continuum, researchers may have little concern for learning in and of itself, instead being interested in understanding the laws of neurobiology that are associated with the behavior. These researchers also use traditional methodologies because the resultant changes in behavior that are known from previous work provide them with a means to assess neural and molecular substrates of learning. The point on the continuum that one decides to set up shop, therefore, depends on the specific interests, goals and training of the individual researcher. The dichotomy that establishes the endpoints of such a continuum is not unique to the study of learning and memory. It has spurred on many important scientific revolutions; evolution, developmental biology, and psychology, and of course in learning as well God vs. natural selection, nature vs. nurture, behaviorism vs. cognitive psychology. While these extreme positions frequently act to garner our attention and make us think more deeply about our work, the majority of us do not see the world in quite so black and white terms. It may be argued that much of the work that moves us forward and fills in the gaps arises from the gray areas, the middle of the continuum. A good example of work at the middle of the continuum is the use of fear conditioning procedures to study the role of structures like the amygdala and hippocampus in emotional learning (e.g., Fanselow, 2001; Lamprecht and LeDoux, 2004). Many laboratories have adopted variations of fear conditioning procedures to study how brain systems interact and encode this form of learning in a number of difference species (including humans). Much of this work has advanced our understanding of acquired fear behaviors while at the same time advancing our understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of the behavioral change. It is true, as Grau and Joynes point out, that the majority of researchers utilize a rather small set of traditional methodologies (i.e., Pavlovian, operant, or instrumental conditioning). Obviously at one end of the continuum the interest is in the methodology itself, while at the other end the methodology serves as a tool for looking at other events (i.e., neural activity, protein synthesis, receptor populations). The use of traditional paradigms/methodologies as research tools, however, is not necessarily the result of seeking a safe course as Grau and Joynes suggest (p. 3). Many of us utilize traditional learning paradigms because they allow us to control a larger number of experimental variables so that underlying neural function can be assessed. Our own work in studying how structures like the cerebellum and hippocampus are involved in classical eyeblink conditioning provides an example of this use of a traditional paradigm (see Steinmetz, 2000, for review). Due to the elegant and comprehensive behavioral work of Isadore Gormezano, Allan Wagner, James Kehoe, Berny Schreurs, and others, we know what to expect behaviorally when manipulations such as those involving interstimulus intervals, intertrial intervals, and stimulus intensity are performed. Being able to predict what happens behaviorally when variations of this basic, traditional learning procedure

- 40 - are performed has greatly aided our ability to elucidate neural processes associated with the learning. In other words, it is not so much that we are suspicious of other paradigms, rather, we use what works optimally and predictably for elucidating mechanisms underlying the phenomena we are studying. The importance of mechanism is well-established by Grau and Joynes, and the authors suggest that mechanism should be the focus of the field as opposed to methodology. We too appreciate the importance of mechanism(s) and it is what drives our research program. Mechanism, however, is not immune from the problems suffered by methodology. Mechanism may vary due to methodology at a behavioral level (e.g., operant conditioning vs. instrumental conditioning) or at a neural level (e.g., what and how brain structures are engaged during learning). An example from the classical eyeblink conditioning literature may serve to illustrate this point. From a behavioral standpoint, evidence for the acquisition of the classically conditioned eyeblink response is the appearance of anticipatory conditioned responses (CRs), which we can call the behavioral mechanism. A number of studies have shown that a small lesions placed in the interpositus nucleus of the cerebellum can abolish this type of learning (e.g., Steinmetz, Logue, & Steinmetz, 1992). From these (and other) data it has been established that the cerebellum is critically involved in the acquisition and performance of the classically conditioned eyeblink response (see Steinmetz, 2000, for review). This is an example of a neural mechanism. Several years ago, Kelly, Zou, and Bloedel (1990) reported the appearance of eyeblink CRs in rabbits that had cerebellum and cerebral cortex removed (i.e., essentially a brainstem preparation). Do these data show that the cerebellum is not critical for classical eyeblink conditioning? Probably not. These data more likely demonstrate the uncovering of plasticity processes in the brain stem after cerebral cortical input is eliminated, a second neural mechanism that is capable of supporting the appearance of the behavioral mechanism (the conditioned eyeblink). Importantly, this second neural mechanism cannot independently support conditioned responding in the intact preparation where the cerebral cortical input is present. This example illustrates the point that a given behavioral mechanism may be accounted for by a number of different neural mechanisms that play different roles in producing the behavior depending on the components of the nervous system that are available to the organism. Indeed, many neural mechanisms may in fact be overlooked or under-appreciated because of traditional mechanisms that hold sway in the field (i.e., cortical inhibition, LTP, LTD, PKC, etc.). Again we see that the problem of generality of neural mechanisms to be just as potentially troublesome here as it is when traditional learning methodologies or phenomena are compared (e.g. Is latent inhibition mechanistically, both behaviorally and neurally, the same during appetitive and aversive forms of instrumental learning?). Grau and Joynes should be applauded for their efforts to regroup and revitalize the field of learning and memory. Their caution of leaning too heavily on old theories, both in our research and in our classrooms, is well warranted. Their position of neural functionalism, focusing on motivational and operational behaviors and the underlying neural mechanisms, is sound. In fact, many of us would purport to already be converts or followers. In the end, our goal as researchers in the field (regardless of our location on the continuum) should encompass an understanding of learning that can explain phenomena across methodologies, in different species,

- 41 - and across development. When we get to this point maybe we will have learned something. References Fanselow, M. S. (2001). Toward a neurobiology of functional behavior systems: Contrasting Pavlovian emotional and motor learning. In J. E. Steinmetz, M. A. Gluck, & P. R. Solomon (Eds.), Model systems and the neurobiology of associative learning (pp. 379-393). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Grau, J. W., & Joynes, R. L. (2005). A neural-functionalist approach to learning. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 1-22. Kelly, T. M., Zou, C.-C., & Bloedel, J. R. (1990). Classical conditioning of the eyeblink reflex in the decerebrate-cerebellate rabbit. Behavioural Brain Research, 38, 7-18. Lamprecht, R., & LeDoux, J. (2004). Structural plasticity and memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 45-54. Steinmetz, J. E. (2000). Brain substrates of classical eyeblink conditioning: A highly localized but also distributed system. Behavioural Brain Research, 110, 13-24. Steinmetz, J. E., Logue, S. F., & Steinmetz, S. S. (1992). Rabbit classically conditioned eyelid responses do not reappear after interpositus nucleus lesion and extensive post-lesion training. Behavioural Brain Research, 51, 103-114. Received April, 19, 2004. Revision received June 22, 2004. Accepted June 22, 2004.