Optimal compensation for changes. in effective movement variability

Similar documents
Optimal Compensation for Changes in Task-Relevant Movement Variability

Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements

Limits to Human Movement Planning in Tasks with Asymmetric Gain Landscapes

The Expected Utility of Movement

Trading Off Speed and Accuracy in Rapid, Goal Directed Movements

Questions Without Words: A Comparison Between Decision Making Under Risk and Movement Planning Under Risk

THE EXPECTED UTILITY OF MOVEMENT

Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning

SPEEDED REACHING MOVEMENTS AROUND INVISIBLE OBSTACLES

Sub-Optimal Allocation of Time in Sequential Movements

Dynamic Estimation of Task-Relevant Variance in Movement under Risk

QUESTIONS WITHOUT WORDS: A COMPARISON BETWEEN DECISION MAKING UNDER RISK AND MOVEMENT PLANNING UNDER RISK

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology

Bayesian Integration in Force Estimation

Decision theory, motor planning, and visual memory: deciding where to reach when memory errors are costly

Probabilistic Inference in Human Sensorimotor Processing

7 Grip aperture and target shape

Speeded Reaching Movements around Invisible Obstacles

Near Optimal Combination of Sensory and Motor Uncertainty in Time During a Naturalistic Perception-Action Task

Statistical decision theory and trade-offs in the control of motor response

Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events

Risk sensitivity in a motor task with speed-accuracy trade-off

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Fractionation of the visuomotor feedback response to directions of movement and perturbation

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Task goals influence online corrections and adaptation of reaching movements

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Psychological Research

Variability and Location of Movement Endpoint Distributions: The Influence of Instructions for Movement Speed and Accuracy

Risk-Sensitive Optimal Feedback Control Accounts for Sensorimotor Behavior under Uncertainty

Behavioural Brain Research

Complexity and specificity of experimentally induced expectations in motion perception

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

Visual-Shift Adaptation Is Composed of Separable Sensory and Task-Dependent Effects

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

Testing Whether Humans Have an Accurate Model of Their Own Motor Uncertainty in a Speeded Reaching Task

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Postural Constraints on Movement Variability

Bimanual Interference Associated With the Selection of Target Locations

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Sensory Cue Integration

Dynamic Integration of Reward and Stimulus Information in Perceptual Decision-Making

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action

An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs

Integration Mechanisms for Heading Perception

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science

Visual and motor constraints on trajectory planning in pointing movements

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Somatosensory Plasticity and Motor Learning

Multiple motor memories are learned to control different points on a tool

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant?

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression

Report. Responsibility Assignment in Redundant Systems

Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in Trajectory-Based Tasks with Temporal Constraint

T he integration of cues from different modalities or from different sources of information is a topic of growing

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

Statistical decision theory for human perception-action cycles

Steven A. Cholewiak ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Choice adaptation to increasing and decreasing event probabilities

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Parallel specification of competing sensorimotor control policies for alternative action options

Impedance control is selectively tuned to multiple directions of movement

Early Learning vs Early Variability 1.5 r = p = Early Learning r = p = e 005. Early Learning 0.

Feedback-Controlled Parallel Point Process Filter for Estimation of Goal-Directed Movements From Neural Signals

Sources of uncertainty in intuitive physics

MODELLING CHARACTER LEGIBILITY

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS.

Supporting Information

Edited by Avinash K. Dixit, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved February 23, 2009 (received for review January 19, 2009)

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

6. Unusual and Influential Data

Supplementary Materials

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be-

Supporting Information

Detection Theory: Sensory and Decision Processes

STRATEGIC INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN HIGH-SPEED HUMAN PERFORMANCE

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

the human 1 of 3 Lecture 6 chapter 1 Remember to start on your paper prototyping

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Supplemental Material

Transcription:

Optimal compensation for changes in effective movement variability Julia Trommershäuser 1, Sergei Gepshtein, Laurence T. Maloney 1, Michael S. Landy 1 & Martin S. Banks,3 1 New York University, Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, 6 Washington Place, New York, New York 10003, USA; University of California, Berkeley, Vision Science Program, School of Optometry, Berkeley, California 9470-00, USA; 3 University of California, Berkeley, Department of Psychology and Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, Berkeley, California 9470, USA Abbreviated Title: Compensation for changes in movement variability Number of Words in Abstract: 136 Number of Words in Introduction, Results and Discussion: 937 Correspondence should be addressed to: Julia Trommershäuser Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science 6 Washington Place, Room 96 New York, NY 10003 USA Email: trommer@cns.nyu.edu Acknowledgments: Supported by Grant EY0866 from the National Institute of Health; Grant RG0109/1999-B from the Human Frontiers Science Program; Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Emmy-Noether-Programm); AFOSR Research Grant F4960 Key Words: Visuo-motor control, movement planning, optimality, statistical decision theory, movement under risk, decision making. 1

Abstract Effective movement planning should take into account the consequences of possible errors in executing a planned movement. These errors can result from either sensory uncertainty or variability in movement production. We examined humans ability to compensate for variability in sensory estimation and movement production when variability is increased by the experimenter. Subjects rapidly pointed at a target and adjacent penalty region. Target and penalty hits yielded monetary rewards and losses. We manipulated effective movement variability by perturbing visual feedback of finger position during the movement. Rewards and penalties were based on the perturbed, visually specified finger position. Subjects adjusted their aim point to this manipulation appropriately. Their performance did not differ significantly from the predictions of an optimal movement planner that maximizes expected gain. Thus, humans compensate for externally imposed changes in their effective movement variability.

The outcome of any planned movement is governed by the movement plan itself, but it is also subject to sensory and motor variability. Thus, if you intend to reach across your desk quickly to pick up a pencil, you may spill your cup of coffee instead. The mover s own variability (sensory uncertainty, execution of the motor command) and deviations in the motor trajectory due to extrinsic sources of noise (unreliability of feedback, externally imposed perturbations) contribute to the outcome of the movement. This variability must be taken into account to maximize the probability of reaching targets while minimizing the probability of hitting other objects. Experiments show that the human movement planning system uses an estimate of sensorimotor variability in selecting a movement plan. For example, as a target is made small and rapid pointing becomes difficult, people sacrifice speed to increase pointing accuracy. This speed-accuracy trade-off allows the subject to hit the target with constant reliability despite changes in task difficulty. 1,,3,4,5,6 In keeping with this observation, models of motor control have emphasized that planning takes movement variability into account. 5,7,8,9 A movement plan is chosen that minimizes task-relevant variance while not constraining task-irrelevant variance. 9 For example, in moving the eye or finger to a target, the variance of the final position is minimized while the variance of mid-trajectory position is not. 8-9 Similarly, if an obstacle is present, the variance of finger position while passing the obstacle is minimized. 7,10 Although the results just described are consistent with the minimum-variance idea, it has been difficult to test these models explicitly because it has not been possible to alter the mover s variability and calculate precisely what changes in the movement plan should occur in response to these changes. We have developed a task that allows us to calculate the optimal movement strategy for a variety of situations. 11-1 Here we explicitly test whether human movement planners can compensate for changes in their effective movement variability. We impose such changes on subjects and measure the corresponding adjustment in their movement. In the task, subjects earned money by rapidly hitting targets that carry a known monetary reward (100 points) while avoiding nearby penalty regions carrying known losses (0, 00, or 500 points). Subjects were instructed to earn as many points as possible. Fig. 1B shows the target-penalty configurations. The center of the penalty region 3

(red) was 9, 13.5 or 18 mm left or right from the center of the reward region (green). Subjects were required on each trial to complete the finger movement within 650 ms of the presentation of the stimulus; if they did not, they incurred a timeout penalty of 700 points. Because of the time constraint, responses were variable. We showed in previous reports that subjects in this task follow an optimal movement strategy that maximizes expected gain. 11-1 In the experiment reported here, we manipulated task-relevant effective movement variability by perturbing the visually specified position of the finger unpredictably during the movement and by scoring responses on the perturbed representation of the finger (Fig. 1A). Three amounts of isotropic perturbation were added: none ( σ = 0mm; meaning no perturbation), medium ( σ = 4.5 mm ), and high pert ( σ pert = 6mm). Subjects first underwent a training session with no perturbation to learn the speeded pointing task including its time constraints. They were then presented with the three amounts of perturbation in different experimental sessions (ordered randomly). With each new amount, they carried out training trials to learn the new effective movement variability. After that, they were presented experimental trials with the three penalties (0, 00, and 500 points) and the three target-penalty configurations (near, medium, and far); both the penalties and target-penalty configurations were presented in random order. An optimal movement strategy corresponds to choosing an aim point that yields an optimal balance between the probability of winning points by hitting the target region and the probability of losing points by hitting the penalty region. The optimal strategy depends on the penalty and the distribution of the subject s end points. When the penalty is zero, the optimal aim point (and hence the mean end point) is the center of the target region. When the penalty is non-zero, the optimal aim point shifts away from the penalty region and, therefore, away from the center of the target (Fig. ). The shift is larger for greater penalties, for penalty regions closer to the target, and for larger perturbations. Therefore, an optimal plan must simultaneously take into account the properties of the stimulus (penalty values and target-penalty configuration, both of which varied randomly from trial to trial) and his/her own effective movement variability (which was fixed within an experimental session). For all conditions, we compared subjects mean end points to pert 4

those of an optimal movement planner that maximizes expected gain by taking into account its own movement variability. Once we measured the effective variability for each subject and for each level of effective movement variability, our model yielded parameterfree predictions of optimal behavior for all the experimental conditions. Results The data of most interest are the shifts in mean end points with changes in penalty value, target-penalty configuration, and effect movement variance, and how those observed shifts correspond with the shifts of an optimal planner. Before describing the shifts, however, we present evidence that subjects behavior was stable before the experimental sessions began. Then we examine the statistical properties of the subjects end points. Finally, we compare the observed and optimal shifts. Homogeneity of subject movements after training Before we started to collect data for a given perturbation, subjects practiced with that perturbation for 70 trials with penalties of 0 and 00. In all cases, the variances of the actual end points for each subject reached a stable value within the first 5 blocks (i.e., the first 10 trials) of the learning session (Fig. 3A). We also examined reaction times and movement times and found that neither differed significantly across conditions (p > 0.05 for each subject in all cases). Reaction and movement times remained constant for the duration of the experiment. This indicates that the timing of movements was the same across experimental conditions. Homogeneity and isotropy of variance across target-penalty configurations and penalty amounts Movement variability differed significantly across subjects, so we analyzed the data separately for each subject. In analyzing the data, it is useful to distinguish three distributions. 1) We refer to the distribution of finger-tip positions when the finger contacted the surface as PDF finger (where PDF stands for probability distribution function); ) The distribution of perturbations of the visual representation of the finger tip is PDF pert. As stated earlier, the perturbation was added to the actual finger position and was an 5

isotropic Gaussian with mean = 0 and variance = σ pert (upon contact). 3) The distribution of the visual representation of the finger tip when it contacted the surface is PDF visual. We found that the variances of finger position (PDF finger ) in the x- and y-directions were independent of conditions and isotropic (p>0.05 in all cases). In particular, we found no evidence of a correlation between the x- and y-directions. Gaussian distribution of movement end points We asked what the form of PDF finger was. Fig. 3B compares PDF finger to a Gaussian distribution. In constructing the figure, the x- and y-coordinate of each end point were treated identically, as if they were drawn from the same distribution. For each quantile of this combined set of x and y end points, Fig. 3B plots a point with ordinate value equal to the z-score of the corresponding end-point position, and abscissa value equal to the quantile for a normal distribution with mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1. The close correspondence between the resulting data points and the solid diagonal line is strong evidence that the distribution is Gaussian. This was the case for all three amounts of effective response variance σ pert, including zero. Because the distributions of the x- and y-coordinates of the end points were identical (i.e., isotropic), in the rest of the paper we compute estimates of observers end-point variability by averaging the variance of the x- and y-coordinates of the end points. Additivity of motor and visually imposed response variability We next tested whether subjects compensated for the experimentally imposed perturbation (PDF pert ) by altering finger position during the movement. When asked about their experience during the experiment, subjects reported that they had noted a decrease of pointing accuracy and a drop in score (in the conditions in which we added a perturbation), but were unable to explain the cause of this effect. Consistent with these reports, two pieces of evidence show that subjects did not compensate for the added perturbation during movement. First, we examined the variance of PDF finger for different amounts of perturbation σ pert. The white bars in Fig. 4 represent σ finger for each value of σ pert for each subject. σ finger did not vary as a function of σ pert (p>0.05 for all subjects in 6

all cases). (Also, σfinger did not vary with target-penalty configuration, nor with the amount of the penalty; p>0.05 for all subjects in all cases.) Second, we looked for evidence of covariation between finger end points and the imposed perturbation. If the two distributions did not covary, the sum of their variances ( σ finger σpert + ) should equal σ visual, the variance of the visually specified end points. The white and black bars in Fig. 4 represent σ finger and σ pert, respectively. Their sum is indeed approximately equal to σ visual, which is represented by the red bars (F-test, all p>0.05). Thus, PDF visual, the distribution of the effective variance, is well characterized by the sum of two independent variables with distributions PDF finger and PDF pert. Furthermore, because PDF pert and PDF finger are both Gaussian, PDF visual must also be Gaussian. These observations justify our computation of an estimate of the actual end-point variance σ finger by averaging over the x- and y-directions, all spatial configurations, and all penalty values. Optimal compensation for changes in effective movement variance Because the distribution of movement end points (PDF finger ) was symmetric with respect to the y-axis, we collapsed the data across the left-right symmetric configurations. The optimal movement planner (described in detail in the Methods) exhibits a different horizontal shift of the aim point (and therefore the mean end point) away from the penalty region for each spatial configuration, penalty level, and amount of perturbation; the predicted shifts for one subject are represented by the white and orange squares in Fig.. The optimal shift is larger for target positions closer to the penalty region, for higher penalty values, and for greater perturbations. No predicted shift occurs when the penalty is zero and when the penalty region is far from the target. The predicted shifts, when they occur, are always horizontal because the target was displaced horizontally from the penalty region. Fig. 5A shows the observed shifts in mean end points (the actual positions of the finger at the end of the movements) away from the center of the target (averaged over trials and subjects). We plot only the horizontal component of the shifts because the end 7

points did not shift vertically. Mean movement end points did not shift horizontally in the zero-penalty condition, nor did they shift when the target and penalty regions were widely separated. They did shift when the penalty was not zero and the penalty and target were not widely separated. End points shifted farther from the target center for higher penalties, closer target positions, and larger perturbations ( σ pert ). In general, the observed shifts are similar to those of the ideal planner. Fig. 6A plots the observed shifts of the actual finger positions versus the shifts predicted by the optimal planner. The correspondence between observed and predicted shifts is excellent with one exception: in the near, high-penalty condition, subjects should have aimed several millimeters outside of the target region (whose outer edge is represented by the vertical and horizontal dashed lines). Subjects did shift their end points away from the target center, but not far enough to fall consistently outside the target region. However, this non-optimal choice of end point had little effect on the score in the high-variance conditions because the gain landscape in these conditions is shallow. Fig. 5B shows the average score per condition averaged across subjects. Scores were lower with high penalties, closer penalty regions, and larger perturbations ( σ ). These changes in scores are consistent with the behavior of the optimal planner. The figure plots the observed scores (average points per trial) against the optimal scores. The correspondence between observed and predicted scores is excellent (see also Table 1) except for one subject in one condition (CAL, in the highest variance condition). Scores were otherwise statistically indiscriminable from optimal. In general, performance did not differ significantly from optimal, indicating that subjects compensated for visually imposed changes in their effective movement variance by appropriately adjusting their movement end points. pert Discussion Our results extend the evidence that humans estimate statistical regularities in motor tasks to improve their performance. For example, Baddeley and colleagues 13 examined how movement planners accumulate information across recent trials to 8

compensate for visual displacements of the hand. On a given trial, their subjects pointed at a target while the visually specified position of the finger tip was shifted by a fixed amount. On subsequent trials this shift changed according to a random walk plus a noise component that varied from trial to trial. Baddeley and colleagues varied the balance between drift rate of the random walk and the magnitude of the independent trial-by-trial noise. Subjects were nearly 73% efficient relative to an optimal model (a Kalman filter). A trial-by-trial analysis of those data suggested that subjects gave most weight to errors from the most recent trials to distinguish shifts due to the random walk from the trial-bytrial noise. Other studies have shown that subjects also estimate the prior probabilities of occurrence of different targets in planning movements. 14-15 Our results extend this picture. In previously studied tasks, optimal performance corresponded to separating reliable information from variability (which could then be ignored). In contrast, optimal performance in our task required an estimate of the variability. An optimal movement strategy has to take into account not only the consequences of the intended movement, but also the consequences of unintended ones (i.e., errors). To achieve that, the optimal strategy is affected by the subject s effective movement variability in addition to the stimulus configuration and the relative rewards and costs associated with the target and penalty regions. Our results demonstrate that the human movement planning system both takes this movement variability into account and compensates for changes in variability if these changes interfere with task goals. Our model is complementary to a recent model of motor coordination based on stochastic optimal feedback control. Todorov and Jordan 9 introduced a minimal intervention principle which assumes that deviations from the average trajectory are corrected only when they interfere with task performance. Variance is not eliminated, but rather it is allowed to accumulate in task-irrelevant dimensions. In our experiments, task relevance is defined explicitly for the subject by the payoffs and penalties associated with different outcomes. Therefore, optimality requires more than minimizing variability. Subjects must also reach the target within a specified time-out period; otherwise, they incur a large penalty. To meet the time constraint, they accept an increase in movement variability. In our task, minimal intervention means that movement variance should be reduced as much as possible by using all the time available. Our subjects learned to time 9

their movements such that they hit the screen just before the end of the timeout. As a result, about 75% of the arrival times fell between 500 ms and the 650 ms time limit, in all subjects. Subjects hardly ever hit the screen later than 650 ms (less than 10 timeouts per subject in a total of 148 trials). Under different task constraints, subjects will choose a different strategy. They may endure higher biomechanical costs to improve the stability of their movements. For example, when moving in the presence of externally applied force fields, subjects maintain a constant level of movement variance by increasing the stiffness of the arm. 16-17 In other words, every task comes with its own cost function, based on the explicit gains and losses associated with the possible outcomes of the movement, biomechanical gains, and the gains associated with the time limits imposed on the mover (Eq. 1). Behavior can only be classified as optimal or sub-optimal with respect to this pre-specified cost function. Subjects may deviate from optimality for a variety of reasons. In our experiment, subjects were generally quite close to optimal with one statistically significant exception (subject CAL in the large-perturbation condition). It is interesting to note that all subjects demonstrated a risk-seeking behavior in this condition: When the optimal planner predicted an end point outside the target region, subjects mean end points were closer to the penalty region than predicted (although only two subjects, CAL and SSG, exhibited efficiencies below 90%; Table 1), as if subjects were reluctant to aim consistently outside the target region. We conclude that these small deviations from optimality do not indicate that subjects failed to update the estimate of their own effective movement variance, but rather that they may have decided to ignore this estimate in some conditions, and aimed within the target even though aiming outside the target would have yielded a larger gain. In summary, our subjects compensated for visually imposed increases in variance, and their performance did not differ significantly from optimal. Our results suggest that humans take their effective movement variance into account in planning movements and that they update their estimates of movement variance in response to externally imposed changes in effective variance. 10

Materials and Methods Apparatus The apparatus has been described by Ernst and Banks. 18 Visual stimuli were displayed on a CRT suspended from above. Subjects viewed the stereoscopically displayed visual stimulus in a mirror using CrystalEyes liquid-crystal shutter glasses. A head-and-chin rest limited head movements. A lightly textured, frontoparallel plane was presented in front of the subject and the stimuli were presented on this plane. A PHANToM force-feedback device tracked the 3d position of the right index finger. The hand itself was not visible, but the fingertip was represented visually by a small cursor. The apparatus was calibrated to assure that the visual and haptic stimuli were superimposed in the workspace. In some conditions, the visual representation of the fingertip was displaced from its actual position thereby perturbing the visual feedback (see Procedure). When the finger reached the visually rendered frontal plane, haptic feedback was provided by the PHANToM : the finger hit the plane. Stimuli The stimuli consisted of a target region and a penalty region (Fig. 1). The target region was a filled green circle and the penalty region was an unfilled red circle. Overlap of the target and penalty was readily visible. The target and penalty regions had radii of 9 mm. The target region was displaced left or right from the penalty region by one of three amounts: near, medium, and far (Fig. 1B). The position of the penalty region was selected randomly on each trial to prevent subjects from using pre-planned movements; the position was chosen from a uniform distribution with a range of ±44 mm relative to screen center. A central 00 100-mm frame indicated the area within which the target and penalty regions could appear. Procedure The appearance of a fixation cross indicated the start of the next trial. The subject moved the right index finger to the starting position, represented by a 4-mm sphere. He or she was required to stay at the starting position until the stimulus appeared (otherwise, 11

the trial was aborted). The frame was then displayed, followed 500 ms later by the target and penalty regions. Subjects were required to touch the stimulus plane within 650 ms or they would incur a timeout penalty of 700 points. The point where the observer touched the plane is the end point of the movement, denoted ( x, y ). If the subject touched the plane at a point within the target or penalty region, the region exploded visually. The points awarded for that trial were then shown, followed by the total accumulated points for that session. A target hit was always worth 100 points. A penalty hit cost 0, 00, or 500 points, which was constant during a block of trials. If the stimulus plane was touched in the region where the target and penalty overlapped, the reward and penalty were both awarded. If a subject moved from the starting position before or within 100 ms after stimulus presentation, the trial was abandoned and repeated later during that block. Visually imposed increase in effective movement variance At the beginning of each trial, the visually specified position of the fingertip the cursor was in the same 3d location as the fingertip itself. The actual end point where the finger hit the stimulus plane is ( x, y ). On perturbation trials, the cursor was displaced smoothly relative to the true, but invisible, location of the fingertip during the second half of the movement (Fig. 1A; see caption for more details). The displacement on each trial ( x, y ) was chosen from a bivariate Gaussian distribution with mean ( ) 0,0 and variance σ pert ; displacements greater than 1 mm were not presented. The cursor hit the plane at the visually specified end point ( x xy, y) + +. Rewards and penalties were scored based on the perturbed (and more variable) visually specified finger position, forcing subjects to estimate their new effective response variance to optimize performance. Each subject ran the experiment with three different amounts of perturbation (which in turn affected the effective response variance): σ pert = 0, 4.5, and 6 mm. Subjects ran a total of 10 sessions. The first was a practice session during which the subject learned the timing of the task. In the practice session, subjects first ran 30 trials (five repeats of each of the six spatial configurations) in the zero-penalty condition 1

with no time limit. This was followed by four blocks of 4 trials (i.e., four repeats) with a moderate time limit of 850 ms, followed by six blocks of 4 trials with a 650-ms time limit. Then three consecutive sessions were run with each amount of perturbation. The first of the three sessions was a learning session in which the subject learned the new effective variance. In the learning session, the subject first ran a warm-up block of 30 trials with zero penalty. Then the cumulative score was reset to zero and 10 more blocks of 4 trials were run (five blocks with penalty zero and five with a penalty of 00, penalty level alternating between blocks). The learning session was followed by two experimental sessions of 37 trials each. Experimental sessions consisted of 1 warm-up trials followed by 1 blocks of 30 trials (four blocks for each of the three penalty levels, five repetitions per target location per block) in random order. Sessions with different amounts of perturbation were run on different days to facilitate learning of the new effective variance. The order of exposure to the different amounts of perturbation was counterbalanced across subjects. Each session lasted about 45 min. Subjects and instructions Six subjects participated. Four were unaware of the experimental purpose; the other two were authors. The four naïve subjects were paid for their participation; they also received bonus payments determined by their cumulative score (5 cents per 1000 points). All subjects used their right index finger for the pointing movement. Subjects were told the payoffs and penalties before each block of trials. All subjects but one were right-handed and all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Subjects gave informed consent before testing. Model of optimal movement planning In previous work, we developed a model of optimal movement planning based on statistical decision theory. 11-1 We assume that the goal of movement planning is to select an optimal visuo-motor movement strategy (i.e., a movement plan) that specifies a desired movement trajectory, method for using visual feedback control, and so on. In this model, called MEGaMove (Maximize Expected Gain for Movement planning), the optimal movement strategy is the one that maximizes expected gain. The model takes into account explicit gains associated with the possible outcomes of the movement, the 13

mover s own effective movement variance, biomechanical gains, and gains associated with the time limits imposed on the mover. Here we summarize the model briefly. The scene is divided into a number of possibly overlapping regions, R i. For the conditions of our experiment, the regions associated with non-zero gains are the circular target and penalty regions. An optimal visuo-motor strategy S on any trial maximizes the subject s expected gain N Γ ( S) = Gi P( Ri S) + GtimeoutP( timeout S) + λb( S), (1) i = 1 where G i is the gain the subject receives if region R i is reached on time. P(R i S) is the probability, given a particular choice of strategy S, of reaching region R i before the time limit t = timeout has expired, where PRS ( ) = P( τ S) dτ, () i timeout R i timeout R i is the set of trajectories τ that pass through R i at some time after the start of the execution of the visuo-motor strategy and before time t = timeout. The expected biomechanical costs associated with the selected movement trajectory are represented in Eq. 1 by a gain function B(S), which is typically negative (a cost or penalty associated with the trajectory). Because the task involves a penalty for not responding before the time limit, Eq. 1 contains a term for this timeout penalty. The probability that a visuo-motor strategy S leads to a timeout is P(timeout S) and the associated gain is G timeout. The parameter λ characterizes the trade-off the subject will tolerate between physical effort and expected reward. In our experiments, subjects win and lose points by touching a stimulus configuration displayed on a plane. As long as the plane is hit before the timeout, penalties and rewards depend only on the position of the end point in this plane. In this effectively two-dimensional task, a strategy S may be identified with the mean end point on the plane (x,y) that results from adopting strategy S. We assume that the movement end points ( x, y ) are Gaussian distributed. Because we found that subjects movement variance was the same in the vertical and horizontal directions, 14

{ ( ) ( ) } 1 px (, y xy, ) = exp x x + y y σ. πσ The probability of hitting region R i is then, (3) PR ( xy, ) = px (, y xy, ) dxdy. (4) i Ri In our experiments, the probability of a timeout and the biomechanical gains are effectively constant over the limited range of relevant screen locations, so in finding the optimal solution of Eq. 1, we can ignore the timeout and biomechanical gain terms. Thus, finding an optimal movement strategy corresponds to choosing a strategy with mean aim point (x,y) that maximizes, ( ) Γ ( x, y) = GP R x, y. (5) i No analytical solution could be found for maximizing Eq. 5 for our stimulus configurations, so the integral was solved by integrating Eq. 4 numerically 19 and using the results to maximize Eq. 5. Data analysis For each trial, we recorded reaction time (the interval from stimulus display until movement initiation), movement time (the interval from leaving the start position until the screen was touched), the movement end position, and the score. Trials in which the subject left the start position less than 100 ms after stimulus display or hit the screen after the time limit were excluded from the analysis. Data format. Each subject contributed approximately 160 data points; i.e., 80 repetitions per condition (with data collapsed across left-right symmetric configurations). On each trial, the actual end-point positions ( ) 1,...,80 i j j i x,y,j= were recorded relative to the center of the target circle. The corresponding effective end-point positions were ( x j xj, yj yj) + +. Tests of homogeneity and isotropy of variance of movement end points. For each amount of perturbation and each subject, we tested whether the variances of the finger end points ( σ finger ) in the x- and y-directions were affected by manipulations of target 15

location and penalty value. Levene tests 0 were performed to test for the homogeneity of the variances in the x- and y-directions across spatial and penalty conditions and across amounts of perturbation. We found no significant differences in variance across stimulus configurations, penalty amounts, and perturbation amounts. We also found that the distribution of end points (PDF finger ) was isotropic. We computed one estimate of each subject s end-point variability ( σ ˆfinger ) by averaging variances across spatial and penalty conditions and across the x- and y-directions. Test of additivity of variance. From the end-point data, we estimated the subject s ˆfinger actual end-point variance σ for each level of perturbation ( σ pert ). For each σ pert, we computed ˆfinger σ for each of the 18 spatial and penalty conditions in the x- and y- directions. We then averaged the resulting 36 variance estimates for that σ pert. If the subject s actual end-point variance is unaffected by the imposed perturbations, ˆfinger σ should be constant across conditions and equal to the expected value σ finger, the subject s unperturbed end-point variance. The subject s effective movement variance σ visual was estimated similarly, using ( x j xj, yj yj) + + in place of ( j, j ) x y. Effective movement variance should increase with increasing perturbation σ pert and, if the subject s actual end-point variance was independent of the amount of perturbation in the visual representation of the end point, it should be equal to the expected value of visual = finger pert σ σ + σ. We found this to be the case (Fig. 4). Responses in symmetric configurations. The target was displaced leftward from the penalty region in half the trials and rightward in the other half. We asked whether the movement end points had the same properties in the two types of stimuli, and found that there were no significant differences. Thus, we averaged data across the leftward and rightward target displacements for each condition. Reaction times and movement times. We also looked for changes in reaction and movement times across conditions. We analyzed both measures for each subject in a 3- factor, repeated-measures ANOVA. The factors were target position, penalty level, and 16

amount of perturbation. We found no significant differences in reaction or movement time across these variables. Effect of spatial and penalty conditions. To determine whether subjects shifted their movement end points in response to changes in perturbation amount (i.e., effective movement variance), we analyzed the end points for each subject in a 3-factor, repeatedmeasures ANOVA. The factors were target position (averaged over symmetric configurations), penalty level, and amount of perturbation. The data are displayed in Figs. 5 and 6. Comparison to model predictions. Mean movement end points for each condition were compared with the end points predicted by our model of optimal movement planning. We calculated the optimal end points (x opt, y opt ) based on each subject s estimated effective movement variance ˆvisual σ for each level of effective response variability σ finger σpert +. Note that y opt = 0 for all conditions. The comparisons are displayed in Fig. 6. Efficiency. Each subject s performance the total points scored in the experiment was compared to optimal performance by computing efficiency. Each subject s cumulative score was computed across the conditions of primary interest, which are those in which the model predicts measurable differences in mean movement end points. These conditions were the near and medium target-penalty configurations for penalty values of 00 and 500. Efficiency is the subject s cumulative score divided by the optimal score predicted by the model. The optimal scores were computed in a Monte Carlo simulation consisting of 100,000 runs of the optimal movement planner performing the experiment with each subject s variance. We calculated the efficiency ratio for each condition and each subject and expressed the ratio as a percentage. Efficiencies were statistically indiscriminable from 100% except in one condition with one subject (Table 1). 17

References 1) Bohan, M., Longstaff, M. G., van Gemmert, A. W., Rand, M. K. & Stelmach, G. E. Effects of target height and width on D pointing movement duration and kinematics. Motor Control 7, 78-89 (003). ) Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E. & Smith, J. E. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol. Rev. 95, 340-370 (1988). 3) Murata, A. & Iwase, H. Extending Fitts law to a three-dimensional pointing task. Hum. Mov. Sci. 0, 791-805 (001). 4) Plamondon, R. & Alimi, A. M. Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. Behav. Brain Sci. 0, 79-349 (1997). 5) Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S. & Quinn, J. T. Motor output variance: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol. Rev. 86, 415-451 (1979). 6) Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Constantinidi, T. S. & Kastrinakis, G. Speedaccuracy trade-offs in the performance of pointing movements in different directions in two-dimensional space. Exp. Brain Res. 134, 1-31 (000). 7) Hamilton, A. F. C. & Wolpert, D. M. Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 434-440 (00). 8) Harris, C. M. & Wolpert, D. M. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780-784 (1998). 9) Todorov, E. & Jordan, M. I. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 16-135 (00). 18

10) Sabes, P. N. & Jordan, M. I. Obstacle avoidance and perturbation sensitivity in motor planning. J. Neurosci. 17, 7119-718 (1997). 11) Trommershäuser, J., Maloney, L. T. & Landy, M. S. Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 0, 1419-1433 (003). 1) Trommershäuser, J., Maloney, L. T. & Landy, M. S. Statistical decision theory and trade-offs in the control of motor response. Spat.Vis. 16, 55-75 (003). 13) Baddeley, R. J., Ingram, H. A. & Miall, R. C. System identification applied to a visuomotor task: near-optimal human performance in a noisy changing task. J. Neurosci. 3, 3066-3075 (003). 14) Körding, K. P. & Wolpert, D. M. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 47, 44-47 (004). 15) Vetter, P. & Wolpert, D. M. Context estimation for sensorimotor control. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 106-1034 (000). 16) Burdet, E., Osu, R., Franklin, D. W., Milner, T. E. & Kawato, M. The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance. Nature 414, 446-449 (001). 17) Franklin, D. W., Osu, R., Burdet, E., Kawato, M. & Milner, T. E. Adaptation to stable and unstable dynamics achieved by combined impedance control and inverse dynamics model. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 370-38 (003). 18) Ernst, M. O. & Banks, M. S. Human integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415, 49-433 (00). 19) Press, W. P., Teukolsky, S. A., Vetterling, W. T. & Flannery, B. P. (eds.) Numerical Recipes in C. The Art of Scientific Computing nd edn. (Cambridge U. Press, Cambridge, UK, 199). 19

0) Howell, D. C. Statistical Methods for Psychology 5 th edn. (Duxbury, Australia, 00). 1) Gnanadesikan, R. Methods for Statistical Data Analysis of Multivariate Observations nd edn. (John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1997). ) Rencher, A. Methods of Multivariate Analysis nd edn. (John Wiley & Sons, New York, 00). 0

Figure captions Fig. 1: Experimental stimuli. A) Manipulation of visual feedback. Subjects viewed a virtual scene on a computer display reflected in a mirror using liquid-crystal shutter glasses. The scene consisted of a gaze-normal plane and a small sphere (cursor) representing the unseen fingertip. The green target and red penalty circles appeared on the plane, and subjects attempted to hit the target, avoiding the penalty, within 650 ms. The position of the cursor (red line) was perturbed away from the actual trajectory of the fingertip (blue line) beginning halfway along the trajectory. The direction of the perturbation was chosen randomly on each trial, but within a trial the direction was fixed (within a plane parallel to the target plane). The perturbation increased linearly in size until reaching a value of ( x, y ) as the screen was hit. The perturbation depended on spatial position of the fingertip and not on the time at which the finger reached a position. Observers received haptic feedback when they hit the plane. The feedback depended on the apparent (i.e., the perturbed), rather than the actual, position of the fingertip. Two trials are shown. In the first, the perturbation drove the cursor away from the penalty and target (no points scored). In trial, with a new target/penalty configuration and new perturbation, the red cursor landed in the penalty region even though the fingertip did not, resulting in a penalty of 00 points. B) Stimulus configurations. The green target and red penalty areas were circular with diameters of 18 mm. The target was displaced leftward or rightward relative to the penalty region by one of three amounts: near, medium, and far (9, 13.5, and 18 mm, respectively). Fig. : Simulations of an optimal movement planner. 00 simulated responses are shown per condition for two amounts of perturbation ( σ = 0 and 6 mm) and four combinations of penalty value and target position. Squares indicate the optimal mean end points. The red and green circles represent the penalty and target regions (centers indicated by the vertical lines). The two levels of effective movement variability (blue: σ visual = 3.48 mm, orange: σ visual = 6.19 mm) correspond to subject AAM s estimated pert 1

effective variability in the conditions with no perturbation ( σ = 0) and the largest perturbation ( σ = 6 mm). The optimal movement planner aims at the center of the pert target circle in no-penalty conditions (not shown), but aims farther away from the penalty pert circle as penalty value increases, distance to the penalty circle decreases, and σ pert increases. Fig. 3: Stability and distribution of effective movement variability. A) Effective ( σ visual ) and actual ( σ finger ) movement variability (expressed as standard deviations) as a function of block number. Open symbols indicate learning blocks, and closed symbols indicate subsequent data collection blocks. In the plots corresponding to conditions with added noise ( σ pert 0 ), the black symbols indicate σ finger and the blue and red symbols indicate σ visual. B) Quantile-quantile plots 1- of subject MID s effective movement end points (PDF visual ). Black corresponds to end points with no movement perturbation ( σ pert = 0 ) and red to end points with the largest perturbation ( σ pert = 6 mm) (70 data points per condition; data for no perturbation collected in session, 3, and 4, data for largest perturbation collected in sessions 8, 9, and 10). In each condition, the x and y values were treated as if they were independent draws from the same distribution. Each quantile of that distribution is plotted versus the corresponding quantile for a Gaussian distribution with mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1. The close correspondence of the data with the diagonal line (indicating the equality of quantiles) is strong evidence that the end-point distribution is Gaussian. Fig. 4: Observed end-point variances. A) Effect of varying the perturbation for each subject. Red bars represent the measured variance σ visual of the perturbed cursor position. The white portion of the other bars represents the measured variance of the actual finger position σ finger (averaged across the x- and y-directions). The black portion of those bars represents the variance of the perturbation ( σ pert ). The sum of the two

variances, σ finger σpert +, is similar to the values of σ visual, indicating that subjects did not compensate for the added perturbation. Estimates of σ visual are an average of 70 data points per level of perturbation. Fig. 5: Observed shifts and points scored. A) Horizontal shift of end points averaged across subjects as a function of added perturbation σ pert for the near (left), medium (middle), and far (right) conditions. The red squares, blue circles, and black triangles represent average shifts when the penalty value was 500, 00, and 0, respectively. The shift increases with increasing penalty, increasing variance, and nearer penalty region. B) Points per trial averaged across subjects. Average points scored as a function of added perturbation σ pert for the near (left), medium (middle), and far (right) conditions. Average points per trial decreased with increasing penalty, increasing variance, and nearer penalty region. Fig. 6: Comparison of observed and optimal performance. A) Horizontal shift of mean end points from target center plotted against the optimal shift predicted by our model. Model predictions were computed based on each subject s variance. Solid diagonal lines indicate perfect correspondence of observed and predicted end points. Vertical and horizontal dashed lines indicate the edge of the target circle. Symbol color represents different amounts of perturbation: black = 0, blue = 4.5, and red = 6 mm. Symbol type represents data from different subjects (from left to right in the key: AAM, CAL, JJT, MID, SSG, and VVF). B) Average number of points earned per trial plotted against optimal number of points per trial as predicted by our model. Model predictions were computed based on each subject s variance. 3

Table Efficiency Subject no noise medium noise high noise AAM 9.% 103.5% 10.5% CAL 107.0% 9.8% 8.4%* JJT 96.6% 9.% 93.0% MID 100.1% 93.% 93.6% SSG 114.7% 99.7% 88.8% VVF 93.8% 109.4% 113.3% Table 1: Efficiency. Data are reported for the six subjects and three levels of effective movement variability. Efficiency is the cumulative score in the penalty = 00 and 500 conditions in the near and medium configuration divided by the corresponding expected score of an optimal movement planner with the same movement variance. Significant deviations from optimality (outside the 95% confidence interval) are marked by an asterisk. 4

A B 18 mm Dy Dx trial near medium far trial 1 Fig. 1

near middle -00-00 optimal mean end point, s pert = 0 mm optimal mean end point, s = 6 mm pert -500-500 s s visual visual = 3.48 mm = 6.19 mm Fig.

A End Point Variability, σ (mm) 8 Subject MID 6 4 0 8 5 10 15 0 6 4 8 6 4 0 5 10 15 0 0 5 10 15 0 Block Number B Quantiles of Normalized End Point Distribution Fig. 3 1 0-1 - Subject MID - -1 0 1 Standard Normal Quantiles learning session s pert = 0 mm s pert = 4.5 mm s pert = 6 mm s pert = 0 mm pert = 6 mm

End-Point Variance (mm ) 50 40 30 0 10 0 50 40 30 0 10 0 AAM CAL JJT MID SSG VVF 0 4.5 6 0 4.5 6 0 4.5 6 s s pert finger visual Visually Imposed Noise s (mm) pert Fig. 4

A B Points per Trial Observed Shift (mm) 8 6 4 0 100 80 60 40 0 near medium far penalty = 0 penalty = 00 penalty = 500 0 4.5 6 0 4.5 6 0 4.5 6 0 0 4.5 6 0 4.5 6 0 4.5 6 s Visually Imposed Noise (mm) Fig. 5 pert

A B Observed Shift (mm) Observed Points per Trial 10 5 100 50 0 near, 500 0 0 5 10 0 50 100 near, 00 medium, 500 0 5 10 0 5 10 Optimal Shift (mm) 0 50 100 0 50 100 Optimal Points per Trial medium, 00 0 5 10 : spert = 0 mm : spert = 4.5 mm : spert = 6 mm 0 50 100 Fig. 6