Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the implicit association of concepts and attributes

Similar documents
Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

The frontal cortex comprises a third of

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe

Supporting Information

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Event-related fmri analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Classification of Inhibitory Function

Selective attention to emotional stimuli in a verbal go/no-go task: an fmri study

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations

Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval

Magnetic Resonance Angiography

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

General and task-speci c frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: A fmri investigation of taskswitching

Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the. attentional network

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) Received 19 July 2001; received in revised form 7 January 2003; accepted 7 January 2003

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

SPECIAL ISSUE: ORIGINAL ARTICLE BINDING OF WHAT AND WHERE DURING WORKING MEMORY MAINTENANCE

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

Repetition priming and the time course of object recognition: an fmri study

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Behavioural Brain Research

Chapter 5. Working memory segregation in the frontal cortex

Mathematical models of visual category learning enhance fmri data analysis

Transient Activation of Superior Prefrontal Cortex during Inhibition of Cognitive Set

EXPLORATION OF THE NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING BY FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety

Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fmri

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function

The hippocampus operates in a threshold manner during spatial source memory Scott D. Slotnick a and Preston P. Thakral b

The Function and Organization of Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Test of Competing Hypotheses

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX. Connections. Dorsolateral FrontalCortex (DFPC) Inputs

Main points. Neuropsychology. Neuropsychological methods OCD. The neuropsychology of obsessivecompulsive. Neuropsychology OCD

Primacy and recency effects as indices of the focus of attention

Dissociation of Mnemonic and Perceptual Processes During Spatial and Nonspatial Working Memory Using fmri

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Dissociation of Verbal Working Memory System Components Using a Delayed Serial Recall Task

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

An fmri investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The functional anatomy of the McCollough contingent colour aftereffect

Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fmri Study of the Stroop Task

Functional specializations in lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the integration and segregation of information in working memory

Chantal E. Stern,*, Adrian M. Owen, Irene Tracey,*, Rodney B. Look,* Bruce R. Rosen,* and Michael Petrides

Goal-directed human cognitive processes can be characterized

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Flexible adaptation to changing environments is one of the

Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers

Object and spatial alternation tasks with minimal delays activate the right anterior hippocampus proper in humans

Functional Topography of a Distributed Neural System for Spatial and Nonspatial Information Maintenance in Working Memory

Working Memory: A View from Neuroimaging

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Problemas conceptuales de la memoria del trabajo

FUNCTIONAL MRI IN EPILEPSY December 6 th 2013

The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: Effects of memory load and individual differences

Cognitive Neuroscience and Addiction

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1.

Selective Tuning of the Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Response during Simple Target Detection Dissociates Human Frontoparietal Subregions

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting

PET Evidence for an Amodal Verbal Working Memory System

performance of verbal working memory tasks

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients

Supporting Information

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment.

Importance of Deficits

Contribution of Working Memory to Transient Activation in Human Inferior Prefrontal Cortex during Performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Brain Regions Associated with the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests

11/14/2017 SUPPORT FOR A MULTI STORE MODEL TEMPORARY MEMORY: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY INFORMATION PROCESSING MODEL & THE MODAL MODEL OF MEMORY

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Transcription:

BRAIN IMAGING NEUROREPORT Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the implicit association of concepts and attributes Michael W. L. Chee, 1,CA N. Sriram, 2 Chun Siong Soon 1,2 and Kok Ming Lee 1 1 Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Singapore General Hospital, 20 College Road, Singapore 169856; 2 Department of Social Work and Psychology, National University of Singapore 119260, Singapore CA Corresponding author Received 28 September 1999; accepted 3 November 1999 Acknowledgements: This work was supported by the National Medical Research Council of Singapore, Grant NMRC 98/00270. We are grateful to Michael I Posner Ph.D. for helpful critique of the manuscript and to Jia Jia Hoon and Marilyn Chee, who contributed to running the experiments and processing the data. The Implicit Association Test (IAT) examines the differential association of two object categories (e.g. ower and insect) with attribute categories (e.g. pleasant and unpleasant). When items from congruent categories (e.g. ower pleasant) share a response key, performance is faster and more accurate than when items from incongruent categories (e.g. insect pleasant) share a key. Performing incongruent word classi cation engages inhibitory processes to overcome the prepotent tendency to map emotionally congruent items to the same response key. Using fmri on subjects undergoing the IAT, we show that the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and to a lesser extent the anterior cingulate cortex, mediate inhibitory processes where manipulation of word association is required. NeuroReport 11:135±140 & 2000 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Key words: Executive function; fmri; Implicit association; Inhibition; Prefrontal cortex INTRODUCTION The need to inhibit habitual or ingrained responses arises in situations where our implicit or habitual tendencies are inappropriate. The capacity to override these habitual or ingrained responses is the function of a supervisory system [1] that includes lateral prefrontal and midline frontal areas. Within the lateral prefrontal cortex, the ventral prefrontal cortex is thought to be critical in mediating inhibitory functions. Imaging studies using the go/no-go task [2,3], an implementation of a Stroop experiment [4] and a verbal, letter-memory inhibition task [5] support this notion. In the context of the present study, inhibition refers to the process of making a choice contrary to an inherently preferred response. Two well-known tests of frontal lobe function, go/no-go and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST) employ newly learned and arbitrary associations between items and responses. In order to generate response selection competition, bias is built up by repeatedly presenting one type of stimulus prior to a shift or change of stimulus type. As such, a novelty or oddball detection effect cannot be excluded as a component process in such experimental designs. In the verbal memory inhibition task used by Jonides et al.; [5] a target item was sometimes used later as a source of interference. Stimulus±response mapping was not held constant. As a result of incidental memory encoding, interference effects could cumulate in an unpredictable fashion as the task progressed. In the present study, we sought to characterize the areas activated by inhibitory processing by engaging a task that utilizes items with ingrained (as distinct from arbitrary) associations and one which does not increment working memory load in order to generate interference. We hypothesized that such a task might preferentially recruit dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) on the basis that manipulation of semantic knowledge is required in performing such a task [6]. The IAT [7] examines the differential association of two object categories (e.g. ower and insect) with two attribute categories (e.g. pleasant and unpleasant). In addition to the knowledge of physical and functional characteristics of items contained in the object categories, we also harbor ingrained associations coding emotional valence. These may not be apparent on casual introspection and are thus termed implicit associations [8]. In the IAT, subjects are instructed to make dichotomous choices in two distinct classi cations on alternating trials. In the rst classi cation, the subject decides if a word belongs to one or the other category (e.g. a rose is a ower; a beetle is an insect). In the following trial, the subject judges an attribute of a word 0959-4965 & Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Vol 11 No 1 17 January 2000 135

NEUROREPORT M. W. L. CHEE ET AL. (e.g. corpse is unpleasant; heaven is pleasant). The items in the alternating classi cations are mutually exclusive. Flower/insect names do not appear in the pleasant/unpleasant task. Previous work has shown that most persons regard owers as pleasant and insects as unpleasant [7]. When items from congruent categories (e.g. ower pleasant) share a response key, performance is faster and more accurate than when items from incongruently associated categories (e.g. insect pleasant) share a key. In the congruent task, categories with similar valences are mapped to the same response key. In the incongruent task, categories with discordant valences are mapped to the same response key. In order to complete the incongruent task successfully, the subject has to remember the counterintuitive assignment of response keys and inhibit the prepotent tendency to assign items with implicitly linked attributes to the same response key. The alternating sequence of the two classi cations enhances the inhibitory demands of this task. In contrast to most other tests exploring inhibition, the change of action schema is predictable in the IAT. The IAT uses the memory of the action taken in the preceding trial to interfere with the current trial and the basis for interference lies in implicit or pre-programmed associations between words and the concepts they allude to. These features enable us to look at the effects of increasing inhibitory processing requirements without changing memory load or incurring novelty effects. MATERIALS AND METHODS Implicit associations related to verbal items were evaluated. Eight right-handed undergraduates who gave informed consent participated in the study. In a practice run prior to scanning, subjects began by classifying 7-letter consonant strings (perceptual control) as either upper or lower case using a two-button mouse. Next they classi ed 20 words as either pleasant or unpleasant. After another block of perceptual controls, subjects classi ed a further 20 items as ower or insect. This sequence was repeated once. In the congruent task subjects pressed the left mouse in response to ` ower' and `pleasant' (Fig. 1). Subjects were thus familiarized with the response mappings immediately prior to undergoing fmri scans. While being scanned, subjects made alternating ower/insect and pleasant/ unpleasant decisions every 1.2 s. Each block of word classi cation lasted 30 s and was alternated with 20 s of upper case/lower case discrimination. Each run comprised CONGRUENT four word classi cation blocks and four blocks of consonant strings. After two fmri runs, another practice was performed for the Incongruent task. This practice was identical in all respects to the congruent task except that the mapping of response keys was switched such that the pleasant words and insect words shared the same response key. Two further fmri runs were then performed. The order of presentation of the congruent and incongruent tasks was counterbalanced across subjects; four subjects began with the congruent task and the other four subjects with the incongruent task. A total of 200 images per task were obtained per subject. Imaging and image analysis: Scanning was performed in a 2.0 T Bruker Tomikon S200 system (Bruker, Karlsruhe, Germany) tted with a 30 mt/m gradient system. A gradient-echo EPI sequence with the following parameters was used: effective TE 40 ms, TR 2000 ms, FOV 23 3 23 cm, a 128 3 64 pixel matrix (128 pixels in the A-P direction). Seventeen contiguous oblique axial slices approximately parallel to the AC-PC line 4 mm thick (with a 2 mm gap) were acquired. High-resolution, T1-weighted anatomical reference images were obtained as a set of 128 contiguous axial slices with a reformatted matrix of 256 3 56 pixels, using a three-dimensional spoiled gradient-echo sequence. Head motion was reduced using a bite bar system (Biomat, Singapore). Methodology related to image pre-processing has been described in previous work from our laboratory [9]. Functional images were separated into two sets for further analysis: congruent and control; incongruent and control. A direct comparison between incongruent and congruent tasks was not possible because these tasks were not interleaved in our design. Images were shifted 4 s to allow for a time lag in the rise of BOLD signal following presentation of word stimuli. Unpaired t-tests were then applied to the groups of images. A Z-score threshold of 3.5 was used to generate the activation maps. These were transformed into Talairach space and co-registered with the high resolution anatomical images. Averaging Talairach-transformed statistical and structural images from each subject separately and then re-computing the statistical map resulted in pooled activation maps. Voxels above the speci ed threshold in the prefrontal, midline frontal and parietal regions were counted. The DLPFC was distinguished from the ventral prefrontal cortex by de ning it as INCONGRUENT (happy) PLEASANT (torture) UNPLEASANT (happy) PLEASANT (torture) UNPLEASANT (carnation) FLOWER (mosquito) INSECT (mosquito) INSECT (carnation) FLOWER Fig. 1. L R L Congruent and incongruent mapping of object and attribute categories to responses. R 136 Vol 11 No 1 17 January 2000

IAT AND PREFRONTAL CORTEX NEUROREPORT the prefrontal cortex above Talairach co-ordinate z ˆ 28 mm. This corresponds to the average level of the inferior frontal sulcus. ANOVA was computed on log-transformed values of pixel counts in the prefrontal regions taking into account laterality of activation, location (dorsal vs ventral), task (incongruent or congruent) and order of task performance (congruent followed by incongruent or vice versa). A region of interest in the DLPFC located at z ˆ 32 mm was de ned in each subject who performed the congruent task rst. Pixels surrounding the activation maxima (approximate location 40,18,32) were included in the region of interest. The region was identical across congruent and incongruent tasks. Signal time courses from individuals and tasks were averaged and the signal intensities normalized to obtain percentage change plots of activations. RESULTS Word classi cation in the incongruent task was associated with increased reaction times A(F(1,6) ˆ 13.4; p ˆ 0.01) as well as reduced accuracy (F(1,6) ˆ 10.3; p ˆ 0.021) compared with the congruent task and control. Repeated presentation of the same stimuli in the incongruent task resulted in some performance improvement, both in terms of reaction time and accuracy (Fig. 2). However, compared to the congruent task, reaction times for the incongruent task were always more delayed. In the pooled data, word classi cation of either type compared to case determination resulted in activation of the left prefrontal and superior parietal regions (Fig. 3a,b). Parietal activations were present in an area ( 32, 58,46) corresponding to BA7 occupying the superior end of the inferior parietal lobule and the posterior portion of the superior parietal lobule. Comparing activations from incongruent and congruent tasks, more extensive activation in the incongruent task was noted in the prefrontal and midline frontal areas as well as the superior parietal areas bilaterally (Fig. 3a, Fig. 4a). Activation maxima in the frontal regions were (a) (b) 1100 1.00 0.90 1000 0.80 0.70 900 0.60 0.50 800 0.40 0.30 700 0.20 0.10 600 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 0.00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Block Block Incongruent Congruent Control Incongruent Congruent Control Fig. 2. (a) Mean reaction time data from eight subjects plotted as a function of time. The numbers on the x-axis denote the block number. (b) Accuracy data from the same eight subjects segregated by block. RT/msec Accuracy (b) 2.00% 1.50% 1.00% I C L DLPFC 0.50% 1 6 11 16 21 26 31 36 41 46 Fig. 3. (a) Axial slices from Talairach transformed, averaged images derived from eight subjects' data showing prefrontal, anterior cingulate and parietal activations. The numbers above the images indicate the distance above the intercommisural plane in millimeters. (b) Averaged time course across task blocks and subjects collapsed into two task-control cycles. Voxel intensities during the incongruent (I; solid line) and congruent (C; shaded line) tasks from a region of interest in the left prefrontal cortex corresponding to z ˆ 32 mm are shown. The rst 10 images relate to the control task and the next 15 images to the task. 0.00% 0.50% Vol 11 No 1 17 January 2000 137

NEUROREPORT M. W. L. CHEE ET AL. (b) Activated voxels 1400 1200 1000 800 C-I I-C 0 Congruent Incongruent Fig. 4. (a) Axial slices from Talairach transformed, averaged images derived from subjects performing the incongruent task rst (I-C), upper panel and the congruent task rst (C-I) lower panel. The arrows indicate the order of task performance. (b) Plot showing mean voxel counts taken from a region of interest in the left prefrontal region segregated by order of task performance and task. 600 400 200 Table 1. Location of activation maxima in left prefrontal region (Talairach coordinates) Subject t-statistic Incongruent t-statistic Congruent x y z x y z 1 7.69 40 16 34 4.57 42 16 34 2 11.0 42 12 28 3.70 42 8 32 3 8.47 40 26 32 7.87 40 18 30 4 10.2 38 10 36 6.26 34 8 36 8.25 44 18 30 5.04 36 20 32 5 6.08 34 28 32 3.53 30 28 32 6 5.24 34 20 30 4.94 34 20 42 4.09 34 20 40 4.61 34 20 30 7 7.44 36 18 28 5.17 36 20 28 6.29 44 14 40 8 8.82 30 24 40 4.53 30 24 40 6.90 28 14 32 3.87 28 14 32 located in the left ventral prefrontal cortex corresponding to BA 47 (Talairach co-ordinates 40,34,8), the DLPFC ( 40,16,32; Table 1) in the middle frontal gyrus corresponding to BA9 and the pre-sma/anterior cingulate region ( 2,10,48). There was an increase in the spatial extent as well as magnitude of activations in the incongruent task vs control (Fig. 3a,b). The increase in prefrontal activations was signi cantly greater in left prefrontal region than the corresponding right prefrontal region (Table 2; F(1,6) ˆ 157; p ˆ 0.00001). Modulation of activation across tasks was greater in the DLPFC compared to the ventral prefrontal cortex (F(1,6) ˆ 7.5; p ˆ 0.03). This effect involved the left but not right prefrontal activations (F(1,6) ˆ 8.8; p ˆ 0.021). It is important to point out that the DLPFC is in fact contiguous with parts of the ventral prefrontal cortex that lie below it. Our intention in making the distinction between ventral and dorsal areas is to merely to highlight a functional difference in the more dorsal parts of the prefrontal cortex. Across individuals, there was consistent activation in the dorsal prefrontal, posterior ventral prefrontal and parietal regions in each subject. However, the spatial extent of activation was very variable (Table 2), necessitating a log transform of pixel counts (all counts were incremented by 1 to set zero as the minimum value in the log transformed data) in the prefrontal region to facilitate analysis by ANOVA. Activations in BA 47 and the anterior cingulate were less robust and tended to appear in subjects who underwent the congruent±incongruent version of the test. When time courses from individual subjects were pooled, we noted that there was no decrement in the magnitude of activations from block to block in the incongruent task. This was in contrast to the improvement in reaction time and accuracy shown in the behavioral data (Fig. 2) and serves to illustrate that imaging and behavioral data can provide independent information about cognitive processes. The modulation of prefrontal and anterior cingulate activations across congruent and incongruent tasks was greater when the congruent task was performed before the incongruent task (F(1,6) ˆ 20.7; p ˆ 0.004; Fig. 4a,b). This may stem from reinforcement of implicit associations when subjects performed the congruent task rst. 138 Vol 11 No 1 17 January 2000

IAT AND PREFRONTAL CORTEX NEUROREPORT Table 2. Activated voxels tabulated by region in congruent (C) and incongruent (I) tasks Subject L ventral frontal (BA 47,44; 40,34,8; 38,12,20) R ventral frontal (BA 47,44; 40,34,8; 38,12,20) L DLPFC (BA 9,44; 40,16,32) R DLPFC (BA 9,44; 40,16,32) Anterior cingulate/pre- SMA (BA 32; 2,10,48) C I C I C I C I C I C-I 1 0 91 0 0 19 217 0 41 2 8 2 122 159 1 301 33 984 0 350 1 283 3 61 36 1 30 0 344 0 25 0 107 4 45 198 0 41 52 353 0 122 0 124 I-C 5 23 130 1 29 32 210 0 8 7 17 6 2 13 0 0 32 61 0 0 6 9 7 12 36 0 0 74 168 0 10 0 4 8 2 4 0 0 0 102 0 0 0 0 Data from eight individual subjects segregated according to order of task performance: C-I indicates that the congruent task was performed rst. DISCUSSION Our results suggest that within the frontal regions, inhibition of implicit word associations involve prefrontal and the anterior cingulate cortex. The prefrontal activations observed were strongly lateralized to the left hemisphere, dorsal to where activations related to inhibitory processing have previously been demonstrated [3±5,10], and were robustly reproduced across subjects. The organizing principles underlying memory and executive processes in the prefrontal lobe are subjects of considerable interest (see [11] for a review). While much work has been channelled to the study of working memory, relatively less attention has been paid to inhibitory processes despite their involvement in operations considered executive or supervisory. On the basis of primate studies, it was long held that the ventral prefrontal cortex had an exclusive role in mediating inhibition [12,13]. This view has prevailed despite evidence from patient studies showing impaired performance in the WCST following damage to the DLPFC [14]. Damage to this region as a result of phenylketonuria results in a loss of ability to act counter to a pre-programmed initial tendency [15]. New lesion [16] and computational studies have strengthened the notion that the suppression of inappropriate prepotent responses may be a property of the entire prefrontal cortex [17,18]. Despite these points, functional imaging studies to date have imputed inhibitory function only to the ventral prefrontal cortex [2,3,5,10]. What underlies the dorsal extension of prefrontal activations in the present study? Previous experiments involving mnemonic processing where the DLPFC was activated, relate to situations where there is increasing item load [19,20], where manipulation of the contents of working memory is required [21] and where task monitoring is required [22]. Activation of the left middle and inferior frontal gyrus in the DLPFC occurs where the need to select a semantically appropriate response from competing responses increases [6]. Given that short-term retention of semantic contents is an essential part of context-based control processes mediated by the frontal lobes [18], one interpretation of the present data is to posit that the DLPFC activation is driven by the need to access semantics and then manipulate that information. In the present experiment, a subject seeing the word `termite' in the incongruent task realizes that this is an insect. However, pressing the left key is interfered with because the same key maps to pleasant in the previous classi cation schema. Suppressing the implicit tendency to map `termite' to the unpleasant response key constitutes a type of inhibition not studied in prior investigations that uncovered ventral prefrontal activation [2±5]. N-back tasks activate the DLPFC and the prevailing notion is that this is because manipulation of working memory is required [11,21]. As N increases, the task becomes more dif cult, arguably because of an increasing load on working memory as well as the increase in competing, task-irrelevant responses. Inhibitory as well as mnemonic processes are involved in N-back tasks; the latter providing the context for the former to act on. Since increasing memory load itself can result in DLPFC activation [20], imputing inhibitory function on the DLPFC requires a task that dissociates memory load from inhibitory requirements. In the IAT, the constancy of stimulus± response mappings and the use of identical items in the congruent and incongruent tasks ensure a constant working memory load and allows us to relate DLPFC activations to inhibitory demands. The lateralization of prefrontal activation in the present experiment was striking and is opposite to the pattern seen with the go/no-go task. The present ndings are inconsistent with the notion that the right inferior frontal area has universal utility in performing inhibitory operations [3,23]. Compared with the dorsoventral organization of working memory, there appear to be few or no organizing principles for lateralization of inhibitory processes. Human case studies suggest that left frontal damage is associated with memory de cits involving verbal items and right frontal damage with non-verbal items [13]. The go/no-go task, which involves attention to action using non-verbal items activates the right prefrontal region when one only considers responses related to the inhibition of the `go' Vol 11 No 1 17 January 2000 139

NEUROREPORT M. W. L. CHEE ET AL. response [3]. The suppression of prepotent responses to recently shown letters [5,10] and the suppression of word reading in a color naming Stroop task [4] activate the left ventral prefrontal region. Before considering a mediaspeci c effect (words vs non-words), we note that tasks that utilize shapes or objects and ostensibly recruit inhibitory processing [23,24] activate both prefrontal cortices, not preferentially the right. This could re ect a tendency to use verbally based strategies whenever a task lends itself to such a possibility [25]. However, another consideration is that inhibitory processes are different in all of these experiments. For example, the go/no-go and WCST involve detection of a change in the experimental milieu, inhibiting the tendency to use the current action schema and replacing it with a new one. The color word naming Stroop test involves selective attention to word color while suppressing an innate tendency to read words. While a direct comparison of different inhibitory tasks has not been performed, a double dissociation between inhibitory processes involving affect and higher order attentional factors has been demonstrated in a lesion study involving marmosets [16]. This supports the notion that spatially distinct areas within the prefrontal region can mediate different inhibitory processes. As such our ndings complement rather than contradict existing ndings. The appearance of anterior cingulate activations in the Incongruent task is consonant with the view that this area mediates con ict resolution and is activated when there is an increased likelihood of making an erroneous response [26]. Compared with the increased prefrontal activations seen in all subjects when the incongruent and congruent tasks were compared, an increase in cingulate activation was less robust. Of interest is the observation that cingulate activation was more prominent when the congruent task was preformed rst. A plausible explanation for this is that the reinforcement of an inherent bias enhances expectations of congruent responses and leads to greater cingulate activation [11]. CONCLUSION The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and, to a lesser extent, the anterior cingulate cortex are activated during the inhibition of prepotent responses to implicit word associations. REFERENCES 1. Norman D and Shallice T. Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. In: Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE and Shapiro D, eds. Consciousness and Self Regulation. New York: Plenum Press, 1986: 1±18. 2. Kawashima R, Satoh K, Itoh H et al. Brain Res 728, 79±89 (1996). 3. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I et al. Brain 122, 981±91 (1999). 4. Taylor SF, Komblum S, Lauber EJ et al. Neuroimage 6, 81±92 (1997). 5. Jonides J, Smith EE, Marshuetz C et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95, 8410± 8413 (1998). 6. Thompson-Schill SL, D'Esposito M, Aguiffe GK and Farah MJ. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94, 14792±14797 (1997). 7. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE and Schwartz JL. J Pers Soc Psychol 74, 1464±1480 (1998). 8. Greenwald AG and Banaji MR. Psych Rev 102, 4±27 (1995). 9. Chee MW, Caplan D, Soon CS et al. Neuron 23, 127±137 (1999). 10. D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Jonides J and Smith EE. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96, 7514±7519 (1999). 11. Smith EE and Jonides J. Science 283, 1657±1661 (1999). 12. Iversen SD and Mishkin M. Exp Brain Res 11, 376±386 (1970). 13. Fuster JM. Human neuropsychology. In: Fuster JM, ed. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven, 1997: 150±184. 14. Milner B. Arch Neurol 9, 90±100 (1963). 15. Diamond A. Evidence for the role of dopamine for prefrontal cortex functions in early life. In: Roberts AC, Robbins TW and Weiskrantz L, eds. The Prefrontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998: 144±164. 16. Dias R, Robbins TW and Roberts AC. J Neurosci 17, 9285±9297 (1997). 17. Roberts AC, Robbins TW and Weiskrantz L. Discussions and conclusions. In: Roberts AC, Robbins TW and Weiskrantz L, eds. The Prefrontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998: 221±242. 18. Cohen JD, Braver TS and O'Reilley RC. A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges. In: Roberts AC, Robbins TW and Weiskrantz L, eds. The Prefrontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998: 195±220. 19. Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE et al. Neuroimage 9, 216±26 (1999). 20. Rypma B and D'Esposito M. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96, 6558±6563 (1999). 21. Smith EE, Jonides J, Marshuetz C and Koeppe RA. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95, 876±882 (1998). 22. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Meyer E and Evans AC. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90, 878±882 (1993). 23. Nagahama Y, Fukuyama H, Yamaaauchi H et al. Brain 119, 1667±1675 (1996). 24. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I et al. Nature Neuroscience 1, 80±84 (1998). 25. Kelley W, Buckner RL, Miezin F et al. Neuroimaging 9, S937 (1999). 26. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM et al. Science 280, 747±749 (1998). 140 Vol 11 No 1 17 January 2000