LOW DOSE OF ETHANOL INDUCES CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN RATS AFTER REPEATED EXPOSURES TO ETHANOL OR SALINE INJECTIONS

Similar documents
DEVELOPMENT OF ALCOHOL DEPRIVATION EFFECT IN RATS: LACK OF CORRELATION WITH SACCHARIN DRINKING AND LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

Effects of Alprazolam and Fluoxetine on Morphine Sensitization in Mice

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

The Biology of Addiction

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place

America is a drugged society

A history of morphine-induced taste aversion learning fails to affect morphine-induced place preference conditioning in rats

Reference place conditioning procedure with cocaine: Increased sensitivity for measuring associatively motivated choice behavior in rats

Methamphetamine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference In Adolescent Male and Female Mice of Two Strains

Pentobarbital-induced place aversion learning

Drugs, The Brain, and Behavior

Aberrant approach-avoidance conflict resolution following repeated cocaine pre-exposure

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

Dopaminergic Signaling Mediates the Motivational Response Underlying the Opponent Process to Chronic but Not Acute Nicotine

Running head: ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT AND ALCOHOL ADDICTION 1

A single cocaine exposure enhances both opioid reward and aversion through a ventral tegmental area-dependent mechanism

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Effects of Norharmane and Nicotine on the Conditioned Place Preference of Mice

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

At a Glance. Background Information. Lesson 3 Drugs Change the Way Neurons Communicate

Deprivation state determines the motivational effects of neuroleptics in rats

processes in the central nervous system (CNS), affecting many of the during the course of ethanol treatment. Ethanol stimulates the release of

NICOTINE ENHANCES THE EXPRESSION OF A CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN ADULT MALE RATS. Nana Marfo. Submitted to the Undergraduate Faculty of

Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions

Competition Between the Conditioned Rewarding Effects of Cocaine and Novelty

Inhibition of the Acquisition of Conditioned Place Aversion by Dopaminergic Lesions of the Central Nucleus of the Amygdala in Morphine-Treated Rats

Psychoactive Drugs & The Brain

Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain. SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO

Rimonabant attenuates sensitization, crosssensitization and cross-reinstatement of place preference induced by nicotine and ethanol

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur

Cogs 107b Systems Neuroscience lec9_ neuromodulators and drugs of abuse principle of the week: functional anatomy

The effects of alcohol and nicotine pretreatment during adolescence on adulthood responsivity to alcohol

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Supplementary Online Content

morphine-dependence and tolerance in the rat

Drugs, addiction, and the brain

ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN

The Effects of Benzodiazepines on Ingestive Behavior. Hannah Dinnen & Ivy Far

General introduction. Chapter 1


Conditioned taste aversion produced by the oral ingestion of ethanol in the rat

Neurobiology of Addiction

Via Barbagia, Quartu Sant Elena, Cagliari

nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727

Many substances that relay signals among neurons

Ghrelin mediates stressinduced. behavior in mice. Chuang et al 2011 L3: Love, Lust, Labor

PIOTR POPIK and WOJCIECH DANYSZ

Basic and Clinical Spring 2012, Volume 3, Number 3

Evaluation of rat ultrasonic vocalizations as predictors of the conditioned aversive effects of drugs

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1

Localization of brain reinforcement mechanisms: intracranial self-administration and intracranial place-conditioning studies

Kappa Opioid Receptor Activation Potentiates the Cocaine-Induced Increase in Evoked Dopamine Release Recorded In Vivo in the Mouse Nucleus Accumbens

MeCP2 and psychostimulantinduced behavioral adaptations. Anne E. West, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurobiology Duke University Medical Center

Sensitization of Salt Appetite Is Associated With Increased Wanting but Not Liking of a Salt Reward in the Sodium-Deplete Rat

The Biological Perspective. Jørg Mørland Senior researcher, Norwegian Institute of Public Health Professor em of Medicine University of Oslo

Contribution of the Opioid System to Alcohol Aversion and Alcohol Drinking Behavior 1

The study of drugs. Pharmacology

The selective dopamine D 3 receptor antagonist SB A reduces nicotine-enhanced brain reward and nicotine-paired environmental cue functions

C81ADD Psychology of Addiction. Alcohol. Ethyl alcohol (ethanol) School of Psychology. Tobias Bast.

Effects of Caffeine on Memory in Rats

HYPERACTIVITY AND SENSITIZATION IN DIFFERENTIALLY REARED RATS ROSEMARY HA. B.S., Kansas State University, 2005 A THESIS MASTER OF SCIENCE

The role of β-phenylethylamine in behavioral sensitization

INTRODUCTION. Ninglei Sun 1, Steven R Laviolette*,1,2,3 and Addiction Research Group

Effect of MS-153, a glutamate transporter activator, on the conditioned rewarding effects of morphine, methamphetamine and cocaine in mice

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP

The Neurobiology of Drug Addiction

- Neurotransmitters Of The Brain -

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

Supporting Online Material for

Dependence Potential of Tramadol: Behavioral Pharmacology in Rodents

Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive effects of opiate withdrawal in rats

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

Abstract. Introduction. R.N. Takahashi 1, O. Berton 2,3, P. Mormède 2 and F. Chaouloff 2

Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward?

The Neurobiology of Addiction

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Pentylenetetrazol Produces a State-Dependent Conditioned Place Aversion to Alcohol Withdrawal in Mice

James M. Murphy, Ting-Kai Li, Lawrence Lumeng and William J. McBride

The Effects of Clozapine on Methylphenidate-Induced Conditioned Place Preference Tiffany Wilkins, a Robin McGovern a

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats

Nicotine Decreases Ethanol-induced Dopamine Signaling and Increases Self-administration via Steroid Hormones

If you give any person a prescription of something like Valium and have them take it on

Differential sensitivity to the acute and sensitizing behavioral effects of methylphenidate as a function of strain in adolescent and young adult rats

EXTERNAL AND INTERNAL STIMULI have signaling

A psychobiological framework of the substrates that mediate nicotine use during adolescence

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Alcohol Clin Exp Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 April 01.

Preclinical Psychopharmacology: From Mechanisms & Molecules to Medicines

Psychoactive drugs Drugs which affect mental processes. Legal but restricted (by prescription only)

Relationship Between Stress and Substance Use Disorders: Neurobiologic Interface

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Outline of the presentation

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

Serotonin System May Have Potential as a Target for Cocaine Medications

Nervous System, Neuroanatomy, Neurotransmitters

Rimonabant for treating tobacco dependence

Transcription:

Alcohol < Alcoholism Vol. 31, No. 6, pp. 547-553, 1996 LOW DOSE OF ETHANOL INDUCES CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN RATS AFTER REPEATED EXPOSURES TO ETHANOL OR SALINE INJECTIONS PRZEMYSLAW BIENKOWSKI 12, PAWEL KUCA 1, JERZY PIASECKI 1 and WOJCIECH KOSTOWSKI 1-2 * 'Department of Pharmacology and Physiology of the Nervous System, Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology, Sobieskiego 1/9, PL-02957 Warsaw and ^Department of Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, Medical High School, Warsaw, Poland (Received 5 February 1996; in revised form 22 July 1996; accepted 30 July 1996) Abstract Using the place conditioning paradigm (biased design), we have shown that five conditioning sessions with ethanol (0 5 or 1.0 g/kg, i p ) did not result in place conditioning Similarly, 15 conditioning sessions with ethanol (1 0 g/kg) did not produce significant place conditioning response In contrast, rats that received 20 injections of ethanol (0 5 g/kg) or saline, before the conditioning procedure, showed significant place preference to the companment paired with 0.5 g/kg ethanol (but not I Og/kg). INTRODUCTION Conditioned place preference (CPP) has been used widely to study the rewarding effects of various drugs of abuse (Barr et al., 1985; Shoaib et al., 1994; Shippenberg and Heidbreder, 1995). This procedure measures drug reward by assessing the association developed between certain environmental stimuli and the drug effects. After several conditioning sessions, animals increase the amount of time spent in the compartment associated with the effects of rewarding drugs such as cocaine (Shippenberg and Heidbreder, 1995) or morphine (Mucha and Iversen, 1984; Barr et al., 1985). Studies of the effect of ethanol (EtOH) on the conditioned place preference in rats have generated conflicting results. The most commonly observed outcome is a failure to establish a CPP after lower ethanol doses (Asin et al., 1985) or a conditioned place aversion (CPA) after higher (> 1.0 g/kg) doses of the drug (Cunningham, 1979; van der Kooy et al., 1983). These results taken together with some self-administration studies (Numan, 1981; Numan et al., 1984) suggest that EtOH possesses rather weak primary reinforcing 'Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. properties in rats. Some authors, however, were able to show EtOH-induced CPP or reduction in EtOH-induced CPA if the rats had previously been exposed to ethanol (Reid et al., 1985; Gauvin and Holloway, 1991; Holloway et al., 1992). Bozarth (1990) reported EtOH-induced CPP after 15 drugcompartment pairings (biased design, without saline injections on alternate days), suggesting that a longer conditioning period might be necessary for associating weak positive reinforcing properties of EtOH with a distinct compartment. Recently, we observed EtOH-induced CPP after repeated injections of EtOH. Animals injected i.p. with 0.5 g/kg of EtOH during pre-exposure period (20 days) and CPP procedure showed significant CPP to the compartment paired with EtOH. On the other hand rats receiving higher EtOH dose (1.0 g/ kg) failed to develop the CPP (Bienkowski et al., 1995). These results suggested a development of sensitization to EtOH rewarding properties after administration of lower doses. Alternative explanations, e.g. development of tolerance to aversive properties of EtOH, might be considered since repeated injections of EtOH (usually higher dose) are known to attenuate subsequent conditioned taste aversion (CTA) (for review see Baker and Cannon, 1982). However, we had considered these results with caution since a non-specific effect of 547 1996 Medical Council on Alcoholism

548 P. BIENKOWSKI el al. repeated handling procedure and stressful stimuli associated with daily i.p. injections had not been ruled out in our preliminary report (Bienkowski et al., 1995). In the present paper, we report the results of place conditioning studies with morphine (as a reference compound), ethanol and ethanol after an additional pre-exposure or conditioning period. MATERIALS AND METHODS Animals Adult male Wistar rats (240-280 g at the start of each study) were used. The rats were housed 7>-A per cage, in the room with controlled environmental conditions (temperature of 21 ± 1 C, 50% relative humidity and 12: 12 h light: dark cycle, light on at 0600). Standard laboratory food (Bacutil, Poland) and tap water were available ad libitum. All procedures were carried out between 1300 and 1800 h. Apparatus and conditioning procedure The CPP apparatus contained five rectangular, wooden boxes. Each box consisted of two end compartments (25 x 34 x 38 cm) and a middle area (25 x 11 x 38 cm). One of the end compartments had brown-painted walls and the floor made of black Plexiglas, whereas the second end compartment had its walls painted white and the floor made of rough naturally coloured plank. The middle area was all painted grey. Removable guillotine doors separated the middle area from the end compartments. The apparatus was enclosed in the temperature-controlled, dimly-lit room. A video camera and video tape recorder were used for acquisition of data. The data were subsequently scored by a 'blind' experimenter. The CPP procedure consisted of three phases and lasted for 13 consecutive days. During phase 1 (pre-conditioning phase; 2 days), the animals were allowed to explore freely the entire box for 10 min. On day 2 (pre-test), the time spent by the rat in each of the two end compartments was recorded and this provided a measure of pre-conditioning preference for the white or black compartment. In phase 2 (conditioning phase; 10 days) the animals were injected daily with ethanol (0.5 or l.og/kg, 10% v/v) or saline i.p., and after 5 min they were then confined to the non-preferred or preferred compartment, respectively. The conditioning sessions (five with the drug and five with saline) lasted for 20 min. In the place conditioning study with morphine, the rats received 2.0 mg/kg of morphine or saline and after 10 min were confined to the non-preferred or preferred compartment, respectively. The conditioning session lasted 30 min. The order of injections (drugsaline-drug-saline... or saline-drug-saline-drug...) was counterbalanced across all groups. Saline-treated control groups received 0.9% NaCl every day. During phase 3 (post-test; 1 day), the animals were allowed to explore the entire box for 10 min and the lime spent in each of the two end compartments was recorded. Experiment 1: place conditioning with morphine In experiment 1, morphine sulfate (Polfa, Warsaw, Poland) was used to show that place preference conditioning might be obtained in our apparatus after five morphine-non-preferred compartment pairings. Morphine (2.0 mg/kg; salt form) was dissolved in saline and given s.c. in a volume of 1.0 ml/kg. The naive animals were randomly assigned to two experimental groups: a saline-treated control group (n = 10) and a morphine-treated group (n = 10). The procedure of conditioning and testing was as described above. Experiment 2: place conditioning with ethanol In this experiment, the ability of ethanol to induce place conditioning was assessed in completely naive animals. The rats were randomly assigned to three experimental groups: a salinetreated control group, a 0.5 g/kg ethanol-treated and a l.og/kg ethanol-treated group (n = 8 rats per group). The procedure of conditioning and testing was as described above. Experiment 3: prolonged conditioning with ethanol In experiment 3, lasting for 35 days, after the pre-conditioning phase (2 days; phase 1), three conditioning phases, 10 days each, ended by posttests, were conducted (post-test 1, 2, 3). The rats were randomly assigned to two experimental groups: a saline-treated control group (n - 8) and a l.og/kg EtOH-treated group (n = 8). The procedure of conditioning and testing was as described above.

ETHANOL AND PLACE PREFERENCE 549 Experiment 4: conditioning with ethanol after a pre-exposure period In experiment 4, lasting for 33 days, the conditioning procedure (described above) started after the pre-exposure period. During the preexposure period, the animals received 20 daily injections of saline or EtOH. The rats were randomly assigned to five groups differing in the treatment during the pre-exposure period and the conditioning phase: saline-saline, saline-0.5 g/kg EtOH, saline-1.0 g/kg EtOH, 0.5 g/kg EtOH- 0.5 g/kg EtOH and 1.0 g/kg EtOH-1.0 g/kg EtOH (n = 8-10 rats per group). PRETEST SALINE POST-TEST MORPHINE GROUP Experiment 5 In this experiment, designed to replicate some findings of experiment 4, larger groups (n = 12-15 rats each) treated with saline during the preexposure period were studied. The rats were randomly assigned to three groups differing in the treatment during the conditioning phase (0.5 or 1.0 g/kg of EtOH, or saline). All groups received 20 daily injections of saline during the preexposure period. The conditioning procedure (described above) started after the pre-exposure period. Statistics All the results [i.e. mean time in seconds (±SEM) spent in the non-preferred compartment] were analysed by two-way ANOVA (group x phase) with repeated measure, and by Newman- Keuls test for individual post-hoc comparison. RESULTS Experiment 1 Five conditioning sessions with morphine resulted in a significant conditioned place preference to the compartment paired with the drug (Fig. I). Two-way ANOVA showed significant effect of group: F(1.18) = 5.95, P = 0.025; phase: F( 1.18) = 14.87, P<0.001; and group x phase interaction: F(I.I8) - 8.64, P<0.0l. Post-hoc analysis showed that the morphine-treated group differed significantly (P < 0.01) from the salinetreated group during post-test (phase 3). Experiment 2 A short period of conditioning (five sessions) Fig. I. Results of place conditioning with 2.0mg/kg of morphine (experiment 1). by the rats in the non-preferred compartment during preand post-test. Numbers in parentheses indicate size of the groups *P < 0.01 vs saline-saline group. with ethanol (0.5 or 1.0 g/kg) was not sufficient to induce any significant place conditioning (Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA did not show significant effect of group, phase or group x phase interaction (all F values < 1). Experiment 3 Fifteen pairings of 1.0 g/kg EtOH dose with the O UJ 2 250-200 - PRETEST _i POST-TEST 1.0 ElOH Fig. 2 Results of place conditioning with 0.5 or 1 0 g/kg of ethanol (experiment 2). by the rats in the non-preferred compartment during preand post-test Numbers in parentheses indicate size of the groups.

550 P. BIENKOWSKI et al. O PRE-TEST POST-TEST 1 250 * r^t, H H POST-TEST 2 200-150 100 50 - B4al POST-TEST 3 1 SALINE (8) l.oetoh GROUP Experiment 4 Two-way ANOVA showed significant effect of group: F(4,39) = 3.93, P<0.0l; phase: F(l,39) = 6.28, P<0.00l; and group x phase interaction: F(4,39) = 2.66, P < 0.05 (Fig. 4A). Lower dose of EtOH (0.5 g/kg) was capable of inducing significant CPP in the group receiving 0.5 g/kg EtOH during pre-exposure period (P < 0.01 vs saline-saline group during post- PRETEST IZH POST-TEST 350-280- 140" 70-IL Llil o- SAL-SAL SAL-.5EI SAL-lEi.5Et-.5Et 1 El-1 Et GROUP PRETEST POST-TEST 300- B Fig. 3. Results of long-time (15 drug-compartment pairings) place conditioning with l.og/kg of ethanol (experiment 3) by the rats in the non-preferred compartment during preand post-test. Numbers in parentheses indicate size of the groups. non-preferred compartment (experiment 1) did not result in a place preference conditioning (Fig. 3). Two-way ANOVA did not show significant effect of group, phase or group x phase interaction (all F values <1). The time spent in the non-preferred compartment during the subsequent post-tests remained relatively stable for every group. SAL-lEi (15) Fig. 4. Results of experiment 4(A) and 5(B). by the rats in Ihe non-preferred compartment during preand posl-test. Symbols describing experimental groups refer to the treatment received by the animals during preexposure period and conditioning phase, respectively. Numbers in parentheses indicate size of the groups. *P < 0.01 vs saline-saline group. SAL: saline; 0.5 Ef 0.5 g/kg EtOH; 1 Et-l.Og/kg EtOH. test). Post-hoc analysis did not reveal any other significant differences vs saline-saline group, though marked increase in preference to the drug-paired compartment was observed in saline-0.5 g/kg EtOH group. Experiment 5 Two-way ANOVA revealed significant effect of phase: F(l,38) = 5.20, P< 0.001; and interaction: F(2,38) = 4.29, P < 0.025. The effect of

ETHANOL AND PLACE PREFERENCE 551 group was not significant: F(2,38) = 3.01, P- 0.061. Post-hoc analysis showed that only saline-0.5 g/kg EtOH group differed significantly (P<0.01) from the saline-saline control group during post-test (Fig. 4B). Marked individual differences were again observed. DISCUSSION The results of experiment 1 indicate that, using our apparatus, it is possible to produce significant place preference conditioning with a standard dose of morphine. These results are in line with many previous studies demonstrating conditioned place preference after morphine injections (Mucha and Iversen, 1984; Barr et al., 1985). In accordance with many other reports (Cunningham, 1979; Van Der Kooy et al., 1983; Asin et al., 1985), ethanol did not induce place preference conditioning after five conditioning sessions (experiment 2). The CPA reported in several studies with higher ethanol doses (Cunningham, 1979; Van Der Kooy et al., 1983) was not observed in the present study. The procedure used by us (the biased design) may be a possible explanation of an apparent lack of aversive conditioning with 1.0 g/kg dose of ethanol. Furthermore, three conditioning phases [i.e. 15 ethanol (1.0 g/kg)-compartment pairings] did not result in any significant place conditioning effect (experiment 3). Our results thus do not support a previous finding (Bozarth, 1990), but an obvious procedural difference makes direct comparison between the studies difficult. Data from experiments 4 and 5 suggest a development of sensitization to EtOH rewarding effects, detected in biased CPP procedure, after prolonged administration of relatively small dose (0.5 g/kg), but not after prolonged pre-exposure and conditioning with a higher dose (1.0 g/kg). The results of the present study are in accordance with our previous findings (Bienkowski et al., 1995) and, in addition, suggest a role for repeated non-specific handling and injections, as the animals treated daily with saline showed CPP after administration of lower but not higher, dose of EtOH. Thus, prolonged pre-exposure to low EtOH dose as well as repeated stress induced by handling and i.p. injections were capable of sensitizing the rats to the rewarding effects of a 0.5 g/kg dose of EtOH. In addition, our results are in line with previous findings that prolonged preexposure to EtOH was necessary for producing CPP response (Reid et al., 1985; Gauvin and Holloway, 1991), facilitating i.v. self-administration (Numan, 1981) and attenuating CTA induced by EtOH (Baker and Cannon, 1982; Stewart et al., 1991). Recently, similar findings were reported for another weak primary reinforcer in rats, nicotine. Only the rats with a history of nicotine exposure showed CPP to the compartment paired with the drug (Shoaib et al., 1994). The higher dose of EtOH (1.0 g/kg) did not produce CPP either after prolonged conditioning (experiment 3) or pre-exposure (experiment 4). These results are in contrast with findings by Bozarth (1990), though strain differences in aversive threshold for EtOH and procedural variations (lack of saline injections on the alternated days in the Bozarth study) may partially explain the discrepancy. Davies and Parker (1990) reported lack of effect of nine EtOH injections (2.0 g/kg) on subsequent place conditioning, suggesting that lower, less aversive, doses of EtOH might be more appropriate. Although behavioural sensitization has been well described for psychostimulants (Shippenberg and Heidbreder, 1995) or nicotine (Shoaib et al., 1994), the literature on sensitization to EtOH's behavioural or rewarding effects is sparse. Cunningham and Noble (1992) demonstrated that repeated injections of EtOH produce sensitization to its activating effect in DBA/2J inbred mice during conditioning trials that produce preference for EtOH-paired environmental stimuli. Other work demonstrated positive effects of nicotine or amphetamine injections on EtOH drinking behaviour in rats, and reported the potentiation of EtOH-induced locomotor stimulation and dopamine turnover after nicotine pre-exposure in mice (Soderpalm et al., 1993; Fahlke et al., 1994; Johnson et al., 1995). There is general agreement that the mesolimbic dopaminergic system is the anatomical locus for reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse (Di Chiara, 1995). EtOH, like other abused drugs, has been consistently reported to stimulate dopamine (DA) neurotransmission in this brain region (Gessa et al., 1985; Yoshimoto et al., 1982). Although the neuropharmacological mechanisms underlying the sensitization of EtOH rewarding effects by repeated stressful stimuli remain unknown, the

552 P. BIENKOWSKI etal. most probable explanation involves ihe changes in DA neurotransmission. Release of DA occurs after exposure to different kinds of stimuli including those possessing negative properties. Importantly, not only strong negative stimuli (e.g. the footshock), but also mild stressors such as restraint of conscious rats enhances the release of DA from the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex (for review, see Salamone, 1994). Thus, it may well be that DA mechanisms underlie the sensitization of reinforcing effects of ElOH after prolonged preexposure to EtOH as well as chronic injection stress. Recently, Persico et al. (1995) showed that pre-exposure to repeated i.p. saline injections profoundly changes transcription factor gene expression, DA turnover and locomotor activity, particularly in a novel environment. In addition, by comparing data from their different studies (Persico et al., 1993, 1995), the above authors suggested development of 'cross-sensitization' between amphetamine and injection stress. Another plausible mechanism that may underlie the stress-induced sensitization for rewarding properties of EtOH may relate to GABA-ergic neurotransmission. Stress decreases the function of chloride channel coupled to the GABA-A receptor due to reduction in density of benzodiazepine receptors and increases in the density of socalled inverse benzodiazepine agonist binding (e.g. Mennini et al., 1988). Notably, these latter authors reported the down-regulation of [ 3 H]- flunitrazepam binding sites in the rat cortex following repeated handling. With regard to the dosage, it seems that the higher dose of EtOH (l.og/kg) was unable to produce CPP because of its aversive properties. It is noteworthy that low doses of EtOH have been consistently reported as ineffective in inducing the CTA in rats, whereas a higher dose produced clear-cut aversive effects (Cannon and Carrell, 1987; Froehlich et al., 1988). Thus, low EtOH dose is likely to be a critical factor in producing the CPP, as higher doses may evoke a variety of unpleasant effects which can mask the true rewarding effect (Nadal et al., 1992). We cannot offer, at present, a more precise explanation of the differences in effects of the two doses of EtOH used in this study. Given that background, the present findings suggest that factors such as drug pre-exposure or stress might be necessary to bring out the rather weak primary reinforcing properties of EtOH in the CPP paradigm. Acknowledgements This study was partially supported by a gram from KBN. Warsaw (GP 20701907). The authors wish (o thank Dr Mariusz Papp for helpful comments and advice REFERENCES Asm, K. E., Wirtshafter, D. and Tabakoff, B. (1985) Failure to establish a conditioned place preference wilh ethanol in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 22, 169-173. Baker, T. B. and Cannon, D. S. (1982) Alcohol and taste-medialed learning. Addictive Behaviors 7, 211 230. Barr, G. A., Paredes, W. and Bridger, W. A. (1985) Place conditioning with morphine and phencychdine; dose dependent effects. Life Sciences 36, 363-368. Bienkowski, P., Kuca, P. and Kostowski, W. (1995) Conditioned place preference after prolonged preexposure to ethanol. Polish Journal of Pharmacology 47, 185-187. Bozarth, M. A. (1990) Evidence for the rewarding effects of ethanol using the conditioned place preference method. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 35, 485^t87. Cannon, D. S. and Carrell, L. E. (1987) Rat strain differences in ethanol self-adminislration and laste aversion learning. Pliarmacologv, Biochemistry and Behavior 28, 57-63. Cunningham, C. (1979) Flavor and location aversion produced by ethanol. Behavioral and Neural Biology 27, 362-367. Cunningham, C. and Noble, D. (1992) Conditioned activation induced by ethanol: role in sensitization and conditioned place preference. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 43, 307-313. Davies, B. T. and Parker, L. A. (1990) Novel versus familiar ethanol: a comparison of aversive and rewarding properties. Alcohol 7, 523-529. Di Chiara, G. (1995) The role of dopamine in drug abuse viewed from the perspective of its role in motivation. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 38, 95-137. Fahlke, C, Hansen, S., Engle, J. and Hard, E. (1994) Effects of ventral striatal 6-OHDA lesions or amphetamine sensitization on ethanol consumption in the rat. Pharmacology, Biocliemistry and Behavior 47, 345-349. Froehlich, J. C, Harts, J. Lumeng, L. and Li, T.-K. (1988) Differences in response to the aversive properties of ethanol in rats selectively bred for oral ethanol preference. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 31,215-222. Gauvin. D. V. and Holloway, F. A (1991) Historical factors in the development of EtOH-conditioned place preference. Alcohol 9, 1-7.

ETHANOL AND PLACE PREFERENCE 553 Gessa. G. L.. Muntoni, F., Collu. M.. Vargiu. L. and Mereu. G. (1985) Low doses of ethanol activate dopaminergic neurones in the ventral tegmental area Bram Research 348, 201-203. Holloway, F. A., King, A., Bedingfield, J. G. and Gauvin. D. V. (1992) Role of context in ethanol tolerance and subsequent hedonic effects. Alcohol 9, 109-116. Johnson. D. H.. Blomqvist, O., Engel, J. A. and Soderpalm. B. (1995) Subchronic intermittent nicotine treatment enhances ethanol-induced locomotor stimulation and dopamine turnover in mice. Behavioral Pharmacology 6, 203-207. Mennini, T.. Gobbi, M.. Perin, L. and Salmona, M. (1988) Rapid internahzation of benzodiazepine receptors in the rat cortex induced by handing. In Chloride Cliannels and Their Modulation by Neurotransmitters and Drugs, Biggio, G. and Costa, E. eds, pp. 263-274. Raven Press, New York. Mucha. R. F. and Iversen. S. D. (1984) Reinforcing properties of morphine and naloxone revealed by conditioning place preference: a procedural examination. Psvchophannacology 82, 241-247. Nadal, R., Pallares. M. and Ferre, M. (1992) Conditioned place preference for ethanol and individual differences in rats. Personality and Individual Differences 13, 287-294. Numan, R. (1981) Multiple exposures to ethanol facilitate intravenous self-administration of ethanol in rats. Phannacologv, Biochemistry and Behavior 15, 101-108. Numan, R., Naparzewska, A. M. and Adler, C. M. (1984) Absence of reinforcement with low dose intravenous ethanol self-administration in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 21, 609-615 Persico, A. M.. Schindler, Ch. O'Chara, B. F., Brannock, M. T. and Uhl, G. R. (1993) Brain transcription factor expression: effect of acute and chronic amphetamine and injection stress. Molecular Bram Research 20,91-100. Persico. A M. Schindler. Ch. W., Zaczek, R.. Brannock, M. T. and Uhl, G. R. (1995) Brain transcription factor gene expression, neurotransmitter levels and novelty response behaviors: alterations during rat amphetamine withdrawal and following chronic injection stress. Synapse 19, 212-227. Reid, L. D., Hunter, G. A., Beaman, C. M. and Hubbell. C. L. (1985) Toward understanding ethanol's capacity to be reinforcing: a conditioned place preference following injections of ethanol. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 22, 483-487. Salamone, J. D. (1994) The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation. Behavioral Brain Research 61, 117-133. Shippenberg, T. S. and Heidbreder, Ch. (1995) Sensitization to the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine: pharmacological and temporal characteristics. Journal of Phannacologv and Experimental Therapeutics 273, 808-815. Shoaib, M., Stolerman, 1. P. and Kumar, R (1994) Nicotine-induced place preferences following prior nicotine exposure in rats. Psvchopharmacologv 113, 445^152. Soderpalm, B, Blomqvist, O., Johnson, D. and Engel, J. A. (1993) Involvement of central nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the mesolimbic DA-activating and reinforcing properties of ethanol. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 19, 610. Stewart, R. B., McBride, W. J., Lumeng, L, Li, T.-K. and Murphy, J. M. (1991) Chronic alcohol consumption in alcohol-prefernng P rats attenuates subsequent taste aversion produced by ethanol injections. Psychopharmacology 105, 530-534. Van der Kooy, D., O'Shaughnessy, M., Mucha, R. F. and Kalant, H. (1983) Motivational properties of ethanol in naive rats as studied by place conditioning. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Beliavior 19,441-445. Yoshimoto, K., McBnde, W. J., Lumeng, L. and Li, T.- K. (1992) Alcohol stimulates the release of dopamine and serotonin in the nucleus accumbens. Alcohol 9, 17-22.