FOR PROOFREADING ONLY

Similar documents
Activity Avoidance and Function in Persons with Chronic Back Pain

Measures of anxiety: is there a difference in their ability to predict functioning at three-month follow-up among pain patients?

DR. GATCHEL HAS NO CONFLICTS OF INTEREST TO DISCLOSE. Gatchel

The Development of a Japanese Version of the Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire

Social context and acceptance of chronic pain: the role of solicitous and punishing responses

DEPARTMENT <EXPERIMENTAL-CLINICAL AND HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY... > RESEARCH GROUP <.GHPLAB.. > PSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION. Geert Crombez

Pain in the Context of Family: A Study on Factors Contributing to Marital Satisfaction among Couples Suffering from Chronic Pain

Pain Catastrophizing Predicts Pain Intensity, Disability, and Psychological Distress Independent of the Level of Physical Impairment

Coping responses as predictors of psychosocial functioning amongst individuals suffering from chronic pain

Functional Tools Pain and Activity Questionnaire

Acceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment method

Correction of Pain Expectancies Following Exposure to Movement in Chronic Back Pain. A thesis presented to. the faculty of

Chapter 3. Psychometric properties of the Pain Self-efficacy Questionnaire: Dutch validation, prediction and discrimination quality

Body Mass Index and Chronic Health Conditions as Predictors of Exercise Fear-Avoidance Beliefs

Exposure to Physical Movements in Low Back Pain Patients: Restricted Effects of Generalization

From the Orton Orthopaedic Hospital, Invalid Foundation, Helsinki, Finland

Learning to live with the pain: acceptance of pain predicts adjustment in persons with chronic pain

ASQ (MANOVA)

British Journal of Pain. Understanding the link between feelings of mental defeat, self-efficacy and the experience of chronic pain

PSYCHOLOGY, PSYCHIATRY & BRAIN NEUROSCIENCE SECTION

Validation of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire-8 in an Australian Pain Clinic Sample

A prospective study of acceptance of pain and patient functioning with chronic pain

SPECIAL ISSUE. Correcting Abnormal Lumbar Flexion Surface Electromyography Patterns in Chronic Low Back Pain Subjects. Randy Neblett, LPC, BCIA-C

Panic Control Therapy (PCT) 1 Barlow model

Psychosocial Factors and Functional Capacity Evaluation Among Persons With Chronic Pain

Is it useful for chronic pain patients? Therapeutic Patient Education (TPE) Psycho-education Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) Françoise LAROCHE, MD

Development and Validation of the Athlete Fear Avoidance Questionnaire. Geoffrey Dover, PhD, CAT(C), ATC; Vanessa Amar, MSc, CAT(C) Online First

Factors Affecting Children's Attitudes Toward Health Care and Responses to Stressful Medical Procedures 1

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results

Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure

Self-Discrepancy in Chronic Low Back Pain: Relation to Pain, Depression, and Psychological Distress

Treatment Intervention Inventory Reliability, Validity and Accuracy

Psychosocial and psychological

JOMP. Reliability of the Korean Version of Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia for Temporomandibular Disorders INTRODUCTION

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults

Biographical, pain and psychosocial data for a South African sample of chronic pain patients

Mindfulness And Relationship Health In Couples With Chronic Pain

Family Medicine: Managing Chronic Pain on

Acognitive-behavioral model of chronic pain emphasizes

HOW TO DESIGN AND VALIDATE MY PAIN QUESTIONNAIRE?

Chapter 3 - Does Low Well-being Modify the Effects of

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

INVESTIGATION OF THE START BACK SCREENING TOOL IN OUTPATIENT PHYSICAL THERAPY SETTINGS

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

Changes in Beliefs, Catastrophizing, and Coping Are Associated With Improvement in Multidisciplinary Pain Treatment

A new scale (SES) to measure engagement with community mental health services

Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1998, 26, Cambridge University Press. Printed in the United Kingdom

From Confounders to Suspected Risk: The Role of Psychosocial Factors Michael Feuerstein, Ph.D., MPH

Osteopathic Medicine Unit, School of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Victoria University, Melbourne

Additional resources from Lunch and Learn: The Good, Bad, and the Ugly: Treatment of Complex Regional Pain Syndrome in Pediatrics

Psychologic variables hold a prominent place in current theorizing

In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), The International Encyclopedia of Depression (pp ). New York: Springer (2009). Depression and Marital Therapy

The Relationship between Self-Belief, Belief in Continuity, Self-Blame, Stability, Mysterious Pain and Intensity of Chronic Pain Disorder

Reliability and validity of the International Spinal Cord Injury Basic Pain Data Set items as self-report measures

Psychometric Properties of an Arabic Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale-20 (PASS-20) in Healthy Volunteers and Patients Attending a Physiotherapy Clinic

Extension of the Children's Perceptions of Interparental Conflict Scale for Use With Late Adolescents

Razieh Shahni a*, Mohammad Reza Shairi b, Mohammad Ali AsghariMoghaddam c, Maryam zarnaghash a

Predicting Complaints of Impaired Cognitive Functioning in Patients with Chronic Pain

Using the STIC to Measure Progress in Therapy and Supervision

Factor Analysis of Gulf War Illness: What Does It Add to Our Understanding of Possible Health Effects of Deployment?

Critical Evaluation of the Beach Center Family Quality of Life Scale (FQOL-Scale)

Author: John Baranoff Stephanie J. Hanrahan Jason P. Connor. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport

An examination of the relationship between chronic pain and post-traumatic stress disorder

Sun Ah Choi 1, ChongNak Son 2*, Jang-Han Lee 1* and Sungkun Cho 3*

Treatment Outcomes Vary by Coping Styles in Rehabilitation Settings

Heavy drinking among college students is in uenced by anxiety sensitivity, gender, and contexts for alcohol use

By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach

EVALUATION OF CHANGES IN OCCUPATIONAL PERFORMANCE AMONG PATIENTS IN A PAIN MANAGEMENT PROGRAM

ISPUB.COM. S Cheng, C Cheung, V Ng, H Lim, K Leung, A Chan, G Hui INTRODUCTION

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida

Key words: State-Trait Anger, Anger Expression, Anger Control, FSTAXI-2, reliability, validity.

DOES RELATIONSHIP STATUS AND QUALITY MODERATE DAILY RESPONSES TO PAIN IN WOMEN WITH CHRONIC PAIN?

Validation of Self-Report Measures of Emotional and Physical Distress Tolerance

Exploring differences in Veterans and Non-veterans at the Chronic Pain Management Unit

International Journal of Health Sciences and Research ISSN:

An activity pacing scale for the chronic pain coping inventory: development in a sample of patients with bromyalgia syndrome

Childhood Healthcare Experience, Healthcare Attitudes, and Optimism as Predictors of Adolescents Healthcare Behavior

Anxiety sensitivity and affect regulatory strategies: Individual and interactive risk factors for anxiety-related symptoms

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module

Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John s, Newfoundland, Canada

Examination of Problem-Solving Related Metacognitions and Worry in an American Sample

Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Illness Factors and Child Behavior Before and During Pediatric Hospitalization

Metacognitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: An open trial

CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS OF THE ANXIETY SENSITIVITY INDEX-3. A Thesis by LACI L. ZAWILINSKI

Abstract. Background. Purpose

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

Functional Capacity Evaluations in Persons With Spinal Disorders: Predicting Poor Outcomes on the Functional Assessment Screening Test (FAST)

Individuals experiencing pain differ in many ways from

Acceptance and Values-Based Action in Chronic Pain: A Study of Treatment Effectiveness and Process

Cognitive-Motivational Influences on Health Behavior Change in Adults with Chronic Pain

Family Assessment Device (FAD)

Running head: ANXIETY SENSITIVTY AND RISK-TAKING BEHAVIORS Dowd 1. Anxiety Sensitivity and Risk-Taking Behaviors. Correy Dowd

Investigation of the Effect of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy on Chronic Pain in the Elderly

FACTOR VALIDITY OF THE MERIDEN SCHOOL CLIMATE SURVEY- STUDENT VERSION (MSCS-SV)

Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L.


Components of Mindfulness in Patients with Chronic Pain

THE PSYCHOLOGY OF CHRONIC PAIN

Transcription:

Journal of Behavioral Medicine, Vol. 27, No. 1, February 2004 ( C 2004) Pain-Related Anxiety in the Prediction of Chronic Low-Back Pain Distress Kevin E. Vowles, 1,4 Michael J. Zvolensky, 2 Richard T. Gross, 3 and Jeannie A. Sperry 3 Accepted for publication: May 17, 2003 This study evaluated the relation of particular aspects of pain-related anxiety to characteristics of chronic pain distress in a sample of 76 individuals with low-back pain. Consistent with contemporary cognitive behavioral models of chronic pain, the cognitive dimension of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS; McCracken, Zayfert, and Gross, 1992, Pain 50: 67 73) was uniquely predictive of cognitive-affective aspects of chronic pain, including affective distress, perceived lack of control, and pain severity. In contrast, the escape and avoidance dimension of the PASS was more predictive of behavioral interference in life activities. Overall, the findings are discussed within the context of identifying particular pain-related anxiety mechanisms contributing to differential aspects of pain-related distress and clinical impairment. FOR PROOFREADING ONLY KEY WORDS: chronic pain; pain-related anxiety; low-back pain. INTRODUCTION Chronic low-back pain is perhaps the most common and disabling of the pain conditions, resulting in significant personal, social, health, and occupational impairment (Nachemson, 1992). In an illustrative example, the incidence of low-back pain in North America alone is approximately 10%, 1 Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, P.O. Box 6040, Morgantown, West Virginia 26506-6040. 2 Department of Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington, Vermont 05405-0134. 3 Oasis Occupational Rehabilitation and Pain Management, P.O. Box 4013, Morgantown, West Virginia 26504-4013. 4 To whom correspondence should be addressed; e-mail: kvowles@mix.wvu.edu. 77 0160-7715/04/0200-0077/0 C 2004 Plenum Publishing Corporation

78 Vowles, Zvolensky, Gross, and Sperry with a substantial percentage of these persons being unresponsive to traditional medical interventions (Frymoyer, 1993). Perhaps not surprisingly, the cost to the health care system is immense, resulting in over $50 billion in direct and indirect costs each year (Frymoyer, 1993; Hoffman et al., 1993; Nachemson, 1992; Tollison et al., 1985). Historically, low-back pain conceptualizations have been dominated by the medical model in which pain is proportional to identified pathology (e.g., tissue damage). Unfortunately, such a perspective has not contributed to a better understanding of the complex nature of chronic pain nor the underlying processes involved with pain-related disability (Turk, 1990). Accordingly, to better account for the highly varied individual expression of chronic lowback pain, contemporary cognitive behavioral models portray the condition as a sensory-affective response, involving physiological, cognitive, and behavioral components (Waddell, 1987). Within this context, researchers have begun to focus their scientific efforts to understanding the nature of chronic low-back pain at the psychological level of analysis, and pain-related anxiety has been highlighted as one of the most disabling aspects of the chronic pain experience (Lethem et al., 1983; Turk and Okifuji, 2002; Vlaeyen and Linton, 2000). Specifically, heightened levels of anxiety about pain are believed to contribute to avoidance of activities that are perceived to promote pain, which in turn, often lead to physical deconditioning, secondary behavioral problems (e.g., weight gain), and reduced social contact (Hadjistavropoulos and LaChapelle, 2000). Moreover, this pattern of responding is likely to become cyclical in nature, such that emotional responsivity and physical deconditioning lead to greater levels of severe pain, behavioral interference, perceived lack of control over life activities, and affective distress (Asmundson, 1999; Asmundson et al., 1997; McCracken, 1997). In this model, then, anxiety about pain is a critical psychological factor involved with the production of maladaptive responding, behavioral interference, and emotional distress. Despite the general theoretical promise of fear-avoidance conceptualizations of chronic pain (Arntz and Peters, 1995; Craig, 1994; Jensen et al., 1994), researchers have only relatively recently began to systematically explore the association between anxiety about pain and clinically relevant dimensions of chronic low-back pain. This research has been facilitated by the development of valid and reliable methodologies that can assess various aspects of anxiety about and fear of pain (e.g., Kori et al., 1990; McNeil and Rainwater, 1998; Vlaeyen et al., 1999; Waddell and Main, 1984). The Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS; McCracken et al., 1992), which assesses four dimensions of pain-related anxiety, including fearful pain appraisals,

Pain-Related Anxiety 79 cognitive symptoms, physiological symptoms, and escape and avoidance behavior, represents one of these methods. Consistent with fear-avoidance models of chronic pain (e.g., Asmundson et al., 1999; Lethem et al., 1983; Vlaeyen and Linton, 2000), research using the PASS has found that patients with chronic pain disorders demonstrate greater pain-related anxiety relative to matched comparison groups, over predict the intensity of pain, cope poorly with pain sensations (e.g., avoidance of physical activity), and evidence greater somatic reactivity in anticipation of pain-eliciting physical activity (McCracken et al., 1993a, 1998; Zvolensky et al., 2001). The PASS total score is also positively correlated with measures of general anxiety, pain, and self-reported disability (Crombez et al., 1999; McCracken et al., 1992), as well as nonspecific physical complaints (McCracken et al., 1998). Furthermore, Burns and colleagues reported that physical ability, as indexed by one s ability to lift or carry certain amounts of weight, was negatively related to scores on the PASS (Burns et al., 2000). Taken together, the PASS has proven to have strong relations with many aspects of the chronic pain experience, especially cognitive aspects of fear and pain (cf. physiological, Crombez et al., 1999). The importance of PASS scores is further supported by their strong relation to treatment outcomes in chronic pain, independent of other theoretically relevant factors such as depressive symptoms and physical ability (McCracken et al., 2002; McCracken and Gross, 1998). Given the existing literature supporting the utility of the PASS, it remains important to evaluate whether specific components of the PASS predict the clinically- and theoretically-relevant dimensions of pain severity, behavioral interference, perceived lack of control over life activities, and affective distress explicitly highlighted in chronic pain models (e.g., Arntz and Peters, 1995; Asmundson, 1999; Vlaeyen et al., 1999). Although the total score has been widely used as an index of fear of pain, the utility of the subscale scores and their relation to aspects of chronic pain is relatively unexplored. To address this issue, this study evaluated the relation of the different subscales of the PASS with established indices of chronic pain distress (i.e., behavioral interference, perceived lack of control, affective distress, and pain severity; Coft et al., 1995), as well as other theoreticallyrelevant variables of pain duration, surgical history, lumbar range of motion, sensory experience of pain, and demographic variables. Consistent with contemporary cognitive-behavioral models of chronic pain (Asmundson, 1999) and anxiety-related disorders (Clark et al., 1989; Eifert et al., 1999), it was hypothesized that cognitive symptoms dimension of the PASS would be uniquely predictive of cognitive-affective aspects of chronic pain. In contrast, the escape and avoidance dimension of the PASS would be more

80 Vowles, Zvolensky, Gross, and Sperry predictive of behavioral interference in life activities because of the pain condition. METHOD Participants Participants were 76 consecutive chronic low-back pain patients. In total, 33 (43.4%) females and 43 (56.6%) males, ranging in age from 21 to 58 (Mean age = 40.4 years, SD = 8.7 years). All participants were Caucasian and had a prolonged history of chronic pain (Mean duration of pain chronicity = 21.3 months, SD = 12.1 months). Additionally, 100% of the patients were occupationally disabled due to their low-back pain problem. Approximately 75% of the sample had completed at least the equivalent of a high school education. The marital status of the participants consisted of 55.3% (n = 42) being married, 30.2% (n = 23) being separated or divorced, 14.5% (n = 11) being single. Finally, 52.6% (n = 37) of the patients had previously received surgery for their presenting low-back problem. As part of an intake appointment, each participant completed a number of self-report questionnaires, a general medical history, and a physical therapy evaluation. Measures Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS) The PASS (McCracken et al., 1992) is a 40-item self-report measure in which respondents indicate anxiety related to pain on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (never) to 5 (always). The summation of individual items allows the derivation of a total score and four subscale scores. The content of the four subscales includes (a) cognitive anxiety symptoms related to the experience of pain, (b) escape and avoidance responses related to reducing pain, (c) fearful appraisals of pain, and (d) physiological anxiety symptoms related to pain. The measure has demonstrated good internal consistency across items, with reported alpha s ranging from 0.74 to 0.94, and excellent 2-week test retest reliability, all r s 0.93, with the exception of the escape/avoidance subscale, which had an r = 0.77 across different administrations (McCracken et al., 1992, 1993b). Finally, the validity of the four subscale scores is supported by significant positive correlations with measures of general anxiety, pain, and disability (McCracken et al., 1992).

Pain-Related Anxiety 81 Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI) The MPI (Kerns et al., 1985) is a well-established three-part measure that assesses pain appraisals, pain severity, impact of pain on specified life domains (e.g., work), and particular behaviors that are performed as a result of the pain. The MPI has demonstrated adequate reliability and validity in chronic pain populations (Kerns et al., 1985). As is typical of research in this area, in the present study we utilized Section I of the MPI. This segment of the instrument assesses the conceptually- and clinically-relevant domains of pain severity, life control, affective distress, and behavioral interference due to pain. Within each subscale, reported interitem reliabilities have ranged from 0.68 to 0.90 (Ferrari et al., 2000; Kerns et al., 1985), and 2-week test retest reliabilities range from 0.62 to 0.91 (Kerns et al., 1985). McGill Pain Questionnaire-Short Form (MPQ-SF) The MPQ-SF (Melzack, 1987) is a self-report measure that assesses sensory and affective aspects of a pain problem. Ratings are made on a 4- point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (none) to 3(severe); items are added together to derive a total score, as well as sensory and affective subscales, which assess specific components of the pain experience. The psychometric properties of the MPQ-SF are well established (see Melzack and Katz, 2001 for a review); reported 15- and 30-day test retest reliabilities range from 0.58 to 0.92 (Burckhardt and Bjelle, 1993; Georgoudis et al., 2001) and internal A1 consistency is adequate (Cronbach s α = 0.71; Georgoudis et al., 2000). In the present study, we used the MPQ-SF sensory scale (total score) rather than a combined sensory and affective total score because the latter may confound the measurement of the sensory experience of pain with pain-related anxiety (Asmundson and Taylor, 1996). Lumbar Range of Motion To provide an index of actual physical movement capacity in the low back, a physical therapist assessed lumbar flexion during a routine physical examination using an inclinometer (possible range: 0 360 ). Although the interrater reliability of physical therapy examinations has been generally low, lumbar flexion has been identified as one of the most reliable measurements with reported interrater reliabilities from 0.87 to 0.93 (Waddell et al., 1992).

82 Vowles, Zvolensky, Gross, and Sperry RESULTS Descriptive Data and Zero-Order Relations Between Predictor and Criterion Variables Means and standard deviations for the psychological variables are presented in Table I. Zero-order correlations were computed between each of the predictor variables and each of the dependent measures to determine the relation between these theoretically relevant constructs. As can be seen in Table II, the results generally indicated that the PASS subscales significantly correlated in a content-specific fashion with the various MPI subscales. In a representative example, the PASS cognitive subscale correlated in the most robust manner with the MPI subscales tapping cognitive-affective aspects of pain-related distress (i.e., affective distress, perceived life control, and pain severity) and to a lesser extent with the MPI subscale tapping behavioral interference. Additionally, divergent validity was demonstrated with the PASS subscales tapping visceral arousal (physiological) and behavioral activation (i.e., escape and fear dimensions) showing a nonsignificant positive relation with cognitive-affective aspects of pain distress. The indices of surgical history, lumbar flexion, and duration of pain problem showed nonsignificant relations with all of the MPI dimensions, reflecting independence of medical/physical parameters associated with functional disability. Table I. Means and Standard Deviations of Predictor and Criterion Psychological Variables Measure Mean (SD) PASS Cognitive anxiety 25.6 (9.9) Escape and avoidance 23.8 (9.2) Fearful appraisals 18.3 (8.9) Physiological anxiety 18.2 (9.8) MPI Affective distress 48.1 (9.1) Behavioral interference 57.8 (8.4) Life control 50.3 (7.9) Pain severity 50.3 (8.2) MPQ-SF 11.8 (7.7) Note. N = 76. PASS: Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (McCracken et al., 1992); MPI: Multidimensional Pain Inventory (Kerns et al., 1985); MPQ-SF: McGill Pain Questionnaire-Short Form, sensory scale (Melzack, 1987).

Pain-Related Anxiety 83 Table II. Zero-Order Relations Between Predictor and Criterion Variables Instrument 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 1. PASS (Cognitive) 0.41 0.69 0.58 0.38 0.27 0.51 0.23 0.05 0.18 0.13 0.20 0.01 0.07 0.09 2. PASS (Escape) 0.70 0.48 0.01 0.31 0.00 0.25 0.07 0.13 0.11 0.29 0.08 0.07 0.06 3. PASS (Fear) 0.61 0.21 0.26 0.17 0.23 0.13 0.15 0.08 0.13 0.07 0.08 0.12 4. PASS (Physiological) 0.20 0.21 0.11 0.21 0.05 0.12 0.07 0.30 0.19 0.05 0.01 5. MPI (Affective distress) 0.39 0.40 0.27 0.16 0.16 0.05 0.06 0.06 0.10 0.03 6. MPI (Interference) 0.08 0.26 0.09 0.14 0.00 0.02 0.01 0.08 0.05 7. MPI (Life control) 0.29 0.03 0.14 0.08 0.04 0.08 0.05 0.04 8. MPI (Pain severity) 0.04 0.22 0.26 0.02 0.01 0.06 0.10 9. MPQ-SF 0.04 0.07 0.08 0.10 0.04 0.03 10. Age (years) 0.09 0.02 0.09 0.02 0.11 11. Education (years) 0.09 0.07 0.19 0.06 12. Gender (1 = male; 2= female) 0.07 0.20 0.06 13. Pain duration (months) 14. Surgical history (frequency) 15. Low-back flexion (deg.) 0.24 0.14 0.04 Note. N = 76. PASS: Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (McCracken et al., 1992); MPI: Multidimensional Pain Inventory (Kerns et al., 1985). p < 0.05; p < 0.01.

84 Vowles, Zvolensky, Gross, and Sperry Prediction of Pain-Related Distress Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed with each of the primary dependent measures. Independent variables were divided into three levels in the hierarchy: (a) demographic variables of gender, age, and education were at level one, (b) medical and physical variables of duration of chronic pain problem, lumbar flexion, MPQ-SF sensory score, and surgery history were included at level two, and (c) PASS subscales at level three. Although demographic, medical, and physical variables were generally unrelated to the subscales of MPI in the present sample, previous investigations have indicated their importance in both chronic pain (Miaskowski, 1999) and pain responding (Carter et al., 2002); therefore, they were included in the present analyses in order to assess the independent value of each of the PASS subscales. First- and second-level variables were entered into the analysis, and the stepwise method was used for third-level variables. For all analyses, partial correlations (Sr) were included to represent effect sizes only for those variables retained in the equation. In terms of the MPI affective distress, the results indicated a significant overall effect, F(1, 76) = 9.9, p.01 (r 2 = 0.12; adjusted r 2 = 0.11). As expected, the PASS cognitive subscale significantly predicted variance in MPI affective distress (β = 0.35, p 0.01, Sr = 0.28), whereas none of the other independent variables added a significant amount of variance in this equation. For the perceived life control dimension of the MPI, there was an overall significant effect, F(1, 76) = 22.2, p 0.001. The r 2 for this equation was 0.24 (adjusted r 2 = 0.23). As hypothesized, the PASS cognitive subscale significantly predicted variance in MPI perceived control (β = 0.48, p 0.001, Sr = 0.54). The PASS fear appraisal subscale was the only other variable to significantly contribute to this equation (β = 0.29, p 0.01, Sr = 0.18), adding an additional 5% of variance (adjusted r 2 change 0.04). For the behaviorally-oriented domain of life interference on the MPI, the results indicated a significant effect, F(1, 76) = 9.2, p 0.01 (r 2 = 0.11; adjusted r 2 = 0.10). As hypothesized, the PASS escape subscale was the only variable to significantly predict variance in behavioral interference (β = 0.34, p 0.01, Sr = 0.22). Finally, for pain severity dimension of the MPI, the results of the regression indicated an overall significant effect, F(1, 76) = 6.4, p 0.01 (r 2 = 0.09; adjusted r 2 = 0.08). As hypothesized, the cognitive subscale of the PASS predicted a significant amount of variance (β = 0.28, p 0.01, Sr = 0.17). No other independent variables added a significant amount of variance in predicting pain severity.

Pain-Related Anxiety 85 Regression equations were also conducted separately for the two genders and resulted in similar findings. In general, the cognitive subscale of the PASS explained a significant amount of variance in the more cognitive aspects of the MPI and the escape/avoidance subscale was more useful in the prediction of the MPI-Interference scale across both genders. In women, however, the cognitive subscale of the PASS did not explain a significant amount of variance in either affective distress or pain severity. DISCUSSION Persons with chronic low-back pain often demonstrate clinically significant levels of anxiety about pain. Such heightened pain-related anxiety is associated with maladaptive coping responses, medication use, and occupational disability (McCracken et al., 1992). Yet, it remains unknown to what extent specific aspects of the global pain-related anxiety construct predict clinically relevant dimensions of chronic low-back pain distress. To address this issue, the present study was designed to evaluate specific pain-related components as differential predictors of established indices of chronic pain distress (Coft et al., 1995) relative to other theoretically relevant variables of pain duration, surgical history, lumbar flexion, sensory experience of pain, and demographic variables. As expected, consistent with contemporary cognitive behavioral models of chronic pain (Asmundson, 1999; Craig, 1994; Vlaeyen et al., 1999), the cognitive dimension of the PASS was uniquely and significantly predictive of affective distress, perceived lack of control, and pain severity. The fear appraisal subscale added a significant, albeit small amount of variance, only for perceived life control. In contrast, the escape and avoidance PASS dimension was significantly predictive of behavioral interference due to pain. This same pattern of findings also was visible when inspecting the zero-order relations between variables. Specifically, there was a greater level of response concordance between cognitive variables and psychological-affective responding to pain, and greater levels of response discordance between psychologicalaffective distress and historical and physical aspects of the pain problem (i.e., surgical history, duration of pain problem, low-back flexion). Such relative response concordance-discordance is a well-established finding in the study of emotion generally (Rachman, 1991) and in studies of pain responding specifically (Craig, 1994). The present study s results extend this result to the fear-avoidance model of chronic pain. Indeed, the results lend further (correlational) support to the perspective that emotional experiences such as pain are best understood as partially independent response systems rather than a more unified construct (Barlow, 1991; Zinbarg, 1998).

86 Vowles, Zvolensky, Gross, and Sperry Overall, these findings are theoretically important to chronic pain models in that they empirically demonstrate a high degree of specificity with the pain-related anxiety construct in a low-back pain population. In particular, the higher the level of correspondence between the particular pain-related anxiety domain and aspects of the pain problem that closely match that fear, the better particular pain indices of distress can be predicted. It is important to note that these findings converge with a larger body of literature in the area of anxiety-related disorders, whereby elevated anxiety is primarily produced when triggered by cues that closely match the object/event of concern (e.g., Clark et al., 1989; Cox, 1996; Eifert et al., 1999; McNally and Eke, 1996). Accordingly, it is becoming increasingly apparent that the specific tendency to respond in an anxious and fearful manner to pain-related events should be more predictive of certain cognitive and behavioral dimensions of chronic pain problems than others. In this way, particular pain-related anxiety components may function as distinct pathways to different aspects of pain-related distress. At a more general level, the results provide additional evidence that psychological variables are important in the prediction of the problematic aspects of chronic low-back pain (Craig, 1994). Indeed, whereas specific pain-related anxiety components predicted pain-related distress, there was no evidence that the duration of chronic pain, surgical history, lumbar flexion, sensory experience of pain, or relevant demographic variables functioned in the same way. Thus, focusing scientific and clinical attention on anxiety and fear of pain is critical to understanding the complex nature of chronic lowback pain. Furthermore, although changes in pain-related anxiety appear to be important in the prediction of multidisciplinary treatments for chronic pain (McCracken et al., 2002; McCracken and Gross, 1998; Vowles and Gross, 2003), it is not yet clear whether reductions in particular pain-related anxiety components are differentially related to other indices of change (e.g., physical capacity functioning). It also is important to note that from a psychometric perspective, the results of this study provide support for the discriminative validity of painrelated anxiety construct as being composed of specific dimensions, as several distinct PASS dimensions varied in their association with chronic pain distress and disability. These findings converge with other research in the area of anxiety pathology, identifying the differential predictive power of specific pain-related anxiety dimensions (Schmidt, 1999). We also found that the PASS and MPI generally share a low, significant (positive) relation with one another, providing convergent validity data for the PASS in a clinical population of low-back pain patients. There are a number of interpretative-related caveats that warrant consideration in this study. First, because the psychological variables were

Pain-Related Anxiety 87 assessed with the same type of methodology, it is possible that the observed findings were, at least in part, due to shared method variance. Future research therefore could employ experimental cognitive methodologies (e.g., Stroop task) to explore more automatic types of processes in which anxiety about pain contributes to emotional distress in low-back pain patients (Mogg and Bradley, 1998). Second, the correlational design while useful at this stage of research development does not permit causal explanations, leaving the results open to a number of alternative explanations in terms of type and direction of the observed relations. Finally, we did not provide a prospective assessment of how pain-related anxiety relates to various aspects of pain distress. Given the present results, in conjunction with other studies in this general area (Asmundson and Taylor, 1996), researchers are in a good position to evaluate the relative stability of the observed findings over time. Taken together, this study evaluated pain-related anxiety, along with other theoretically relevant variables, as predictors of pain severity, affective distress, perceived lack of control, and behavioral interference. Overall, the findings reiterate the importance of anxiety about pain in understanding distress produced by chronic pain problems, and suggest that the higher level of correspondence between the particular pain-related domain and events that closely match that fear, the better problematic aspects of the pain problem can be predicted, and subsequently addressed. REFERENCES Arntz, A., and Peters, M. (1995). Chronic low back pain and inaccurate predictions of pain: Is being too tough a risk factor for the development and maintenance of chronic pain? Behav. Res. Ther. 33: 49 53. Asmundson, G. J. G. (1999). Anxiety sensitivity and chronic pain: Empirical findings, clinical implications, and future directions. In Taylor, S. (Ed.), Anxiety Sensitivity: Theory, Research, and Treatment of the Fear of Anxiety, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 269 285. Asmundson, G. J. G., Kuperos, J. L., and Norton, G. R. (1997). Do patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information? Preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fear. Pain 72: 27 32. Asmundson, G. J. G., Norton, P. J., and Norton, G. R. (1999). Beyond pain: The role of fear and avoidance in chronicity. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 19: 97 119. Asmundson, G. J. G., and Taylor, S. (1996). Role of anxiety sensitivity in pain-related fear and avoidance. J. Behav. Med. 19: 573 582. Barlow, D. H. (1991). Disorders of emotion. Psychol. Inq. 2: 58 71. Burckhardt, C. S., and Belle, A. (1993). A Swedish version of the Short Form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Scand. J. Rheumatol. 23: 77 81. Burns, J. W., Mullen, J. T., Higdon, L. J., Wei, J. M., and Lansky, D. (2000). Validity of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS): Prediction of physical capacity variables. Pain 84: 247 252. Carter, L. E., McNeil, D. W., Vowles, K. E., Sorrell, J. T., Turk, C., Ries, B., and Hopko, D. R. (2002). Effects of emotion on pain reports, tolerance and physiology. Pain Res. Manage. 7: 21 30.

88 Vowles, Zvolensky, Gross, and Sperry Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., and Brown, G. (1989). Cognitive mediation in general psychiatric outpatients: A test of the content-specificity hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56: 958 964. Coft, P. R., Papageorgio, A. C., Thomas, E., Jayson, M. I., and Silman, A. J. (1995). Psychologic distress and low back pain. Evidence from a prospective study in the general population. Spine 15: 2731 2737. Cox, B. J. (1996). The nature and assessment of catastrophic thoughts in panic disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 34: 363 374. Craig, K. D. (1994). Emotional aspects of pain. In Wall, P. D. and Melzack, R. (Eds.), The Textbook of Pain, Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, pp. 261 274. Crombez, G., Vlaeyen, J., W. S., Heuts, P. H. T. G., and Lysens, R. (1999). Pain-related fear is more disabling than pain itself: Evidence on the role of pain-related fear in chronic back pain disability. Pain 80: 329 339. Eifert, G. H., Zvolensky, M. J., Sorrell, J. T., Hopko, D. R., and Lejuez, C. W. (1999). Predictors of self-reported anxiety and panic symptoms: An evaluation of anxiety sensitivity, suffocation fear, heart-focused anxiety, and breath-holding duration. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 21: 293 305. Ferrari, R., Novara, C., Sanavio, E., and Zerbini, F. (2000). Internal structure and validity of the Multidimensional Pain Inventory, Italian language version. Pain Med. 1: 123 130. Frymoyer, J. W. (1993). Quality: An international challenge to the diagnosis and treatment of the disorders of the lumbar spine. Spine 18: 2147 2152. Georgoudis, G., Oldham, J. A., and Watson, P. J. (2001). Reliability and sensitivity measures of the Greek version of the short form of the McGill Pain Questionnaire. Eur. J. Pain 5: 109 118. Georgoudis, G., Watson, P. J., and Oldham, J. A. (2000). The development and validation of a Greek version of the short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Eur. J. Pain 4: 275 281. Hadjistavropoulos, H. D., and LaChapelle, D. L. (2000). Extent and nature of anxiety experienced during physical examination of chronic low back pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 38: 13 29. Hoffman, R. M., Wheeler, K. J., and Deyo, R. A. (1993). Surgery for herniated discs: A literature synthesis. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 8: 487 496. Jensen, M. P., Turner, J. A., and Romano, J. M. (1994). Correlates of improvement in multidisciplinary treatment for chronic pain. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 62: 172 179. Kerns, R. D., Turk, D. C., and Rudy, T. E. (1985). The West Haven Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory. Pain 23: 345 356. Kori, S. H., Miller, R. P., and Todd, D. D. (1990). Kinesiophobia: A new view of chronic pain behavior. Pain Manage. 3: 35 64. Lethem, J., Slade, P. D., Troup, J. D. G., and Bentley, G. (1983). Outline of a fear-avoidance model of exaggerated pain perception I. Behav. Res. Ther. 21: 401 408. McCracken, L. M. (1997). Attention to pain in persons with chronic pain: A behavioral approach. Behav. Ther. 28: 271 284. McCracken, L. M., Faber, S. D., and Janeck, A. S. (1998). Pain-related anxiety predicts nonspecific physical complaints in persons with chronic pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 36: 621 630. McCracken, L. M., and Gross, R. T. (1998). The role of pain-related anxiety reduction in the outcome of multidisciplinary treatment for chronic low back pain: Preliminary results. J. Occup. Rehabil. 8: 179 189. McCracken, L. M., Gross, R. T. and Eccleston, C. (2002). Multimethod assessment of treatment process in chronic low back pain: Comparison of reported pain-related anxiety with directly measured physical capacity. Behav. Res. Ther. 40: 585 594. McCracken, L. M., Gross, R. T., Sorg, P. J., and Edmunds, T. A. (1993a). Prediction of pain in patients with chronic low back pain: Effects of inaccurate prediction and pain-related anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 31: 647 652. McCracken, L. M., Zayfert, C., and Gross, R. T. (1992). The Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale: Development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain 50: 67 73. McCracken, L. M., Zayfert, C., and Gross, R. T. (1993b). The Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale: A multimodal measure of pain specific anxiety symptoms. Behav. Therap. 16: 183 184.

Pain-Related Anxiety 89 McNally, R. J., and Eke, M. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity, suffocation fear, and breath-holding duration as predictors of response to carbon dioxide challenge. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105: 146 149. McNeil, D. W., and Rainwater, A. J. (1998). Development of the Fear of Pain Questionnaire III. J. Behav. Med. 21: 389 410. Melzack, R. (1987). The short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Pain 30: 191 197. Melzack, R., and Katz, J. (2001). The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Appraisal and current status. In Turk, D. C., and Melzack, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Pain Assessment, 2nd ed., Guilford, New York, pp. 35 52. Miaskowski, C. (1999). The role of sex and gender in pain perception and responses to treatment. In Gatchel, R. J., and Turk, D. C. (Eds.), Psychosocial Factors in Pain: Critical Perspectives, Guilford, New York, pp. 401 411. Mogg, K., and Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 36: 809 848. Nachemson, A. L. (1992). Newest knowledge of low back pain. Clin. Orthop. 279: 8 20. Rachman, S. J. (1991). Disorders of emotion: Causes and consequences. Psychol. Inq. 2: 86 87. Schmidt, N. B. (1999). Examination of differential anxiety sensitivities in panic disorder: A test of anxiety sensitivity subdomains in predicting fearful responding to a 35% CO 2 challenge. Cogn. Ther. Res. 23: 3 19. Tollison, C. D., Kriegel, M. L., and Downie, G. R. (1985). Chronic low back pain: Results of treatment at the pain therapy center. South. Med. J. 78: 1291 1300. Turk, D. C. (1990). Customizing treatment for chronic pain patients: Who, what, and why? Clin. J. Pain 6: 255 270. Turk, D. C., and Okifuji, A. (2002). Psychological factors in chronic pain: Evolution and revolution. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 70: 678 690. Vlaeyen, J. W. S., and Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art. Pain 85: 317 332. Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Seelen, H. A. M., Peters, M., de Jong, P., Aretz, E., Beisiegel, E., and Weber, W. E. J. (1999). Fear of movement/(re)injury and muscular reactivity in chronic low back pain patients: An experimental investigation. Pain 82: 297 304. Vowles, K. E., and Gross, R. T. (2003). Work-related beliefs about injury and physical capability for work in individuals with chronic pain. Pain 101: 291 298. Waddell, G. (1987). A new clinical model for the treatment of low back pain. Spine 12: 632 644. Waddell, G., and Main, C. J. (1984). Assessment of severity in low back pain disorders. Spine 9: 204 208. Waddell, G., Somerville, D., Henderson, I., and Newton, M. (1992). Objective clinical evaluation of physical impairment in chronic low back pain. Spine 17: 617 628. Zinbarg, R. E. (1998). Concordance and synchrony in measures of anxiety and panic reconsidered: A hierarchical model of anxiety and panic. Behav. Ther. 29: 301 323. Zvolensky, M. J., Goodie, J. L., McNeil, D. W., Sperry, J. A., and Sorrell, J. T. (2001). Anxiety sensitivity in the prediction of pain-related fear and anxiety in a heterogeneous chronic pain population. Behav. Res. Ther. 39: 683 696.

Queries to Author: A1: Note that the year 1994 in this reference citation has been changed to 1993 according to the reference list. Is it OK? 90