Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg 1

Similar documents
Adaptive Resonance Theory

Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System

Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus

Coordinated learning of grid cell and place cell spatial and temporal properties: Multiple scales, attention, and oscillations

Exploring the Functional Significance of Dendritic Inhibition In Cortical Pyramidal Cells

Grandmother cohorts: Multiple-scale brain compression dynamics during learning of object and sequence categories

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making

NEED RECURRENT GATED DIPOLE TO EXPLAIN SECONDARY INHIBITORY CONDITIONING 1. Pair Onset of CS 1 with shock READ (I) CIRCUIT

Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS

Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective *

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

HOW HALLUCINATIONS MAY ARISE FROM BRAIN MECHANISMS OF LEARNING, ATTENTION, AND VOLITION

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

Investigation of Physiological Mechanism For Linking Field Synapses

BBS-D _ Mather_ Houghton. The Dentate Gyrus and the Hilar Revised

Sparse Coding in Sparse Winner Networks

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization

Cholinergic suppression of transmission may allow combined associative memory function and self-organization in the neocortex.

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators

Transitions between dierent synchronous ring modes using synaptic depression

Cell Responses in V4 Sparse Distributed Representation

A half century of progress towards a unified neural theory of mind and brain with applications to autonomous adaptive agents and mental disorders

Anxiolytic Drugs and Altered Hippocampal Theta Rhythms: The Quantitative Systems Pharmacological Approach

Spiking Inputs to a Winner-take-all Network

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

Artificial Neural Networks (Ref: Negnevitsky, M. Artificial Intelligence, Chapter 6)

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press.

NEOCORTICAL CIRCUITS. specifications

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001

Basics of Computational Neuroscience: Neurons and Synapses to Networks

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Dendritic compartmentalization could underlie competition and attentional biasing of simultaneous visual stimuli

Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function

Reversed and forward buffering of behavioral spike sequences enables retrospective and prospective retrieval in hippocampal regions CA3 and CA1

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Sensory Physiology and the Thalamus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

Oscillatory Neural Network for Image Segmentation with Biased Competition for Attention

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

What do you notice?

How do spatial learning and memory occur in the brain? Coordinated learning of entorhinal grid cells and hippocampal place cells

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

TOWARDS A UNIFIED THEORY OF NEOCORTEX: Laminar Cortical Circuits for Vision and Cognition

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence

Toward a spiking model of the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex

Neocortex. Cortical Structures in the Brain. Neocortex Facts. Laminar Organization. Bark-like (cortical) structures: Shepherd (2004) Chapter 12

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

Neural Dynamics of Gestalt Principles of Perceptual Organization: From Grouping to Shape and Meaning

Attention, Binding, and Consciousness

Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning

How has Computational Neuroscience been useful? Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

Introduction to EEG del Campo. Introduction to EEG. J.C. Martin del Campo, MD, FRCP University Health Network Toronto, Canada

Different inhibitory effects by dopaminergic modulation and global suppression of activity

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki

Observational Learning Based on Models of Overlapping Pathways

Synaptic plasticity and hippocampal memory

Cognitive Neuroscience Section 4

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

Competition Elicits Arousal and Affect

COGS 107B Week 1. Hyun Ji Friday 4:00-4:50pm

Neurobiological Models of Visual Attention

The storage and recall of memories in the hippocampo-cortical system. Supplementary material. Edmund T Rolls

How does the ventral pathway contribute to spatial attention and the planning of eye movements?

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

A Computational Model of Context Processing

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

arxiv: v1 [q-bio.nc] 13 Jan 2008

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski

Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250

Limber Neurons for a Nimble Mind

Dendritic Mechanisms of Phase Precession in Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons

Modeling the Impact of Recurrent Collaterals In CA3 on Downstream CA1 Firing Frequency

Modeling goal-directed spatial navigation in the rat based on physiological data from the hippocampal formation

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction. Paul A. Dudchenko

Identify these objects

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma

A Biophysical Model of Cortical Up and Down States: Excitatory-Inhibitory Balance and H-Current

Electrophysiological and firing properties of neurons: categorizing soloists and choristers in primary visual cortex

Active Control of Spike-Timing Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in an Electrosensory System

A toy model of the brain

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

Why is our capacity of working memory so large?

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature,

A Neural Theory of Visual Search: Recursive Attention to Segmentations and Surfaces

Transcription:

Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg 1 1 Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems Center for Adaptive Systems, and Center of Excellence for Learning in Education, Science and Technology Boston University 677 Beacon St Boston, Massachusetts 02215 steve@bu.edu Telephone: 617-353-7858/7 Fax: 617-353-7755 http://www.cns.bu.edu/~steve July 29, 2008 Revised: November 24, 2008 CAS/CNS-TR-08-003 Hippocampus, in press Running head: Beta Oscillations during Hippocampal Place Cell Learning Corresponding author: Stephen Grossberg, steve@bu.edu Keywords: grid cells, category learning, spatial navigation, adaptive resonance theory, entorhinal cortex 1

Abstract The functional role of synchronous oscillations in various brain processes has attracted a lot of experimental interest. Berke et al. (2008) reported beta oscillations during the learning of hippocampal place cell receptive fields in novel environments. Such place cell selectivity can develop within seconds to minutes, and can remain stable for months. Paradoxically, beta power was very low during the first lap of exploration, grew to full strength as a mouse traversed a lap for the second and third times, and became low again after the first two minutes of exploration. Beta oscillation power also correlated with the rate at which place cells became spatially selective, and not with theta oscillations. These beta oscillation properties are explained by a neural model of how place cell receptive fields may be learned and stably remembered as spatially selective categories due to feedback interactions between entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. This explanation allows the learning of place cell receptive fields to be understood as a variation of category learning processes that take place in many brain systems, and challenges hippocampal models in which beta oscillations and place cell stability cannot be explained. 2

The role of the hippocampal system in spatial navigation has been of great interest since O Keefe and Dostrowsky (1971) showed the spatial correlates of pyramidal cell firing in the hippocampus. Many of these cells tend to fire in a specific portion of the environment independently of the head direction and movement speed, hence the term place cells. Such place cell selectivity can develop within seconds to minutes, and can remain stable for months (Thompson and Best, 1990; Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Muller, 1996; Frank et al., 2004). Many models of hippocampal place cell formation have been proposed but, until recently, none has explained this combination of fast learning and stable memory, which is often called the stability-plasticity dilemma (Grossberg, 1980, 1999). How place cells are learned and remembered has attracted even more interest since the recent discovery of grid cells (Hafting et al., 2005) within entorhinal cortical circuits that project to the hippocampus. Berke et al. (2008) have reported that beta oscillations occur during the learning of hippocampal place cell receptive fields in novel environments.. Paradoxically, beta power was very low during the first lap of exploration, grew to full strength as a mouse traversed a lap for the second and third times, became low again after the first two minutes of exploration, and remained low on subsequent days of exploration. Beta oscillation power also correlated with the rate at which place cells became spatially selective, and did not correlate with theta oscillations. Given the rapidity with which place cell learning occurred, and the sharp increase in beta activity during the second exposure to the environment, it would seem that a highly selective learning mechanism is at work. The present article explains these properties of beta oscillations as natural consequences of brain processes that enable place cell receptive fields to solve the stabilityplasticity dilemma. This explanation unifies three parallel streams of modeling activity, and leads to testable predictions aimed at clarifying the underlying neural mechanisms. Fast Learning and Stable Memory. The first stream of modeling activity concerns how the brain can quickly learn to categorize information in the world, and to remember it without experiencing catastrophic forgetting. As noted above, how the brain combines rapid plasticity with long-term memory stability is called the stability-plasticity dilemma (Grossberg, 1980, 1999). Adaptive Resonance Theory, or ART, predicts how the brain accomplishes this feat. The development of ART included the discovery of how the brain learns recognition categories using self-organizing maps (Grossberg, 1976, 1978; Kohonen, 1984). In a selforganizing map, distributed patterns of input features are represented at a first processing level F 1. They activate adaptive connections to a second processing level F 2, whose cells represent recognition categories, or compressed representations, of the feature patterns (Figure 1a). These category cells compete with one another to choose one, or a small number, of winning cells that receive the largest total inputs. The winning cells then send teaching signals to abutting synaptic knobs, which learn the signals that reach them from the feature level via their adaptive connections. Grossberg (1976, 1978) mathematically proved that these associative and competitive mechanisms can together learn stable categories in response to a sparse series of input patterns whose statistics do not change through time. However, in response to a dense series of inputs whose statistics do change through time, catastrophic forgetting of previously learned categories can occur, during which the same input pattern can activate ever-changing categories when it is repeated in a series of intervening input patterns, because the intervening patterns can recode previously learned categories. Dense non-stationary inputs are, however, frequently encountered in the world in which we live. Thus, a self-organizing map does not solve the stability-plasticity dilemma. 3

Figure 1. ART dynamics and learning: (a) Input pattern I is instated across feature detectors at level F 1 as activity pattern X (hatched pattern). I also nonspecifically excites the orienting system ρ, while X inhibits ρ. I excitation and X inhibition are balanced. I also generates output pattern S, which is multiplied, or gated, by learned bottom-up adaptive weights. These gated signals add to form input pattern T, which causes a recurrent on-center off-surround network to competitively select an activity pattern Y across winning recognition categories at level F 2. (b) Y generates top-down output pattern U. U signals are multiplied by adaptive weights and added at F 1 cells to form a prototype V that encodes the learned expectation of Y. Such a prototype represents shared features in all input patterns that activate Y. If V mismatches any I features at F 1, then a new activity pattern X* (hatched pattern) is selected at F 1, which is active at I features that match V. Matched features support gamma oscillations. Mismatched features (white area) are inhibited. Mismatched features support beta oscillations. When X changes to X *, total inhibition decreases from F 1 to ρ. (c) If inhibition decreases sufficiently, ρ releases a nonspecific arousal burst to F 2 ; that is, novel events are arousing. Arousal resets F 2 by inhibiting Y. (d) After Y is inhibited, X is reinstated and Y stays inhibited as X activates a different category pattern Y* at F 2. Search continues until a better-matching or novel F 2 category is selected. When search ends, an attentive resonance triggers learning of the attended data. [Adapted with permission from Carpenter and Grossberg (1993).] 4

Attentive Matching, Resonant Learning, and Mismatch-Mediated Reset. ART showed how the stability-plasticity dilemma could be solved by using learned top-down expectations. These expectations are read out from the category level F 2 to the feature level F 1 via adaptive top-down connections (Figure 1b), and matched at F 1 against the bottom-up input feature patterns. A topdown, modulatory on-center, off-surround network realizes this matching process (Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987, 1991; Grossberg, 1995, 1999), and explains data about biased competition (Desimone, 1998; Kastner and Ungerleider, 2001). The modulatory on-center reads out learned weights corresponding to features in the prototype of the learned category. Top-down matching occurs with these primed features, while the off-surround suppresses mismatched features that are not in the on-center. If the match is good enough, then selected features form an attentional focus. A feedback loop of mutual excitation then develops between the attended feature pattern and the active category, carried via their adaptive bottom-up and top-down connections. This mutual excitation causes a context-sensitive resonant state that synchronizes, amplifies, and prolongs cell responses, as it simultaneously inhibits mismatched features and categories. Such a resonance drives fast learning within both the bottom-up adaptive weights that define recognition categories, and the top-down adaptive weights that define learned expectations; hence the name adaptive resonance. This is unlike learning in a self-organizing map, which is triggered by bottom-up events alone. If the match is not good enough, then a mismatch state develops which causes reset of the current category and search for another category that can better learn to represent and predict the data (Figures 1c and 1d). These predicted ART mechanisms have been supported by many behavioral and neurobiological experiments; see Grossberg (2003), Grossberg and Versace (2008), and Raizada and Grossberg (2003) for reviews. The main point for present purposes is that top-down attentive matching and mismatch-mediated reset can solve the stability-plasticity dilemma. Every Input Pattern Can Initially be Matched by its Top-down Expectation. On the first learning trial, when a category is first selected by its bottom-up connections to represent a novel set of features, its top-down expectation must be able to match any input feature pattern. The category cannot know in advance what feature pattern it will end up representing. If it mismatched this input pattern, reset would be triggered, and learning could not get started. How a self-organizing neural system gets started is a critical issue for any type of learning. In ART, all top-down expectations can initially match any input pattern because they have excitatory adaptive weights in their modulatory on-centers that start out large and are broadly distributed across the network (e.g., Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987, 1991). Subsequent learning trials prune these adaptive weights as they gradually select a more localized pattern of critical features in a category s modulatory on-center, while its other top-down weights approach zero. Partial mismatches occur as learning prunes a category s critical feature pattern. Larger mismatches reset active categories to search for better-matching ones (Figures 1c and 1d). Mismatch and Beta Oscillations. Is there a relationship between mismatches and beta oscillations? Were this the case, then all the Berke et al. (2008) data could be explained: Beta oscillations are not seen during the first lap because, on the first learning trial, there are no topdown mismatches. Beta oscillations begin during the second lap and are correlated with the rate at which place cells became selective because mismatches occur when learning is refined. Indeed, Berke et al. (2008) reported that the extent of beta-entrainment predicted the 5

improvement in spatial specificity between the first 2 min. Beta oscillations are attenuated after the first few trials because, when place cell learning is stabilized by top-down expectations, no more mismatches occur. The Synchronous Matching ART (SMART) model predicts how beta oscillations occur during mismatch and reset events (Grossberg and Versace, 2008; Versace and Grossberg, 2006). More generally, SMART shows how ART mechanisms of attentive match-based learning and mismatch-mediated category pruning, reset, and search can be realized within the laminar circuits of multiple cortical areas as they interact with primary and higher-order specific thalamic nuclei and nonspecific thalamic nuclei. SMART hereby simulates multiple levels of brain organization, ranging from spiking dynamics to cognitive processing. In SMART, matches between bottom-up input patterns and learned top-down expectations can cause gamma oscillations that support attention, resonance, and spike-timing dependent plasticity (STDP) learning, whereas mismatches can cause slower beta oscillations that inhibit STDP at mismatched cells. The SMART model simulates learning of perceptual and cognitive categories within thalamocortical and corticocortical circuits. However, the neural mechanisms that cause beta oscillations during mismatch states are not specific to the features that are categorized and matched. Thus, if place cells in the hippocampus are learned as spatial categories that obey ART laws, then an explanation of the Berke et al. (2008) data immediately follows. Figure 2. Place cell category learning: Grid cells in entorhinal cortex play the role of level F 1 in Figure 1, whereas place cells in dentate gyrus play the role of category cells in level F 2. Three populations of entorhinal grid cells of five cells each are aligned along the dorso-ventral gradient in entorhinal cortex and have different spatial scales. Their firing profiles are represented as peaks of activity that are aligned with the track along which an 6

animal moves. The animal s current location causes the corresponding grid cells to fire (filled circles). The dentate gyrus granule cell (population) that receives strong projections from all three active grid cells fires (filled circle) and activates a recurrent inhibitory interneuron (population) that suppresses other granule cells. A backpropagating action potential in the winning granule cell s dendrites (dotted arrow) carries a teaching signal that triggers associative learning at synapses receiving active grid cell inputs. Such back-propagating action potentials dissociate read-out of learned associations from their subsequent read-in after a competitive decision is made (Grossberg, 1975; Hasselmo et al., 2002). Only bottom-up projections from grid cells to hippocampal place cells are shown. Resonant learning dynamics also require top-down connections, as in Figure 1. [Reprinted with permission from Gorchetchnikov and Grossberg (2007).] Place Cells as Spatial Categories of Multiple-scale Grid Cells. Gorchetchnikov and Grossberg (2007) have modeled how place cell receptive fields may be learned as spatial catgories in an ART system. In particular, Gorchetchnikov and Grossberg (2007) simulated how adaptive connections from entorhinal grid cells with multiple, but small, spatial scales (Hafting et al., 2005) may activate hippocampal place cells that can represent spaces of many meters during navigational behaviors (Figure 2). In this proposal, multiple-scale entorhinal grid cell activations form the level F 1 feature patterns that are categorized by hippocampal place cells acting like spatial categories in level F 2. The spatial scale of these place cells is the least common multiple of the incoming grid periods (e.g., see p.143 in Hartmann, 1997). By itself, however, these bottom-up grid-to-place cell learned connections cannot solve the stability-plasticity dilemma for the same reasons that no self-organizing map can do so. Hippocampal-to-Entrorhinal Feedback, Novelty Sensitivity, and Visual Landmarks. The stability-plasticity dilemma may be solved, and catastrophic forgetting of place cell maps prevented, only if there is top-down attentive matching of grid cell activity patterns by top-down learned expectations that are read out by active place cells. Mismatches are predicted to cause beta oscillations. Large enough mismatches also activate novelty-sensitive cells that mediate reset of hippocampal place cells (Figures 1c and 1d). Many experiments have discussed noveltysensitive cells in the hippocampus since Vinogradova (1975). Recent neurobiological data from other experimental paradigms support the prediction that beta oscillations occur during mismatch states. For example, Buschman and Miller (submitted for publication) have reported beta oscillations in the frontal eye fields during spatial attention shifts. Buffalo et al (2004) have reported more beta oscillations in deep layers of visual cortex, and more gamma oscillations in superficial levels of visual cortex. These data are consistent with the SMART model prediction that reset is mediated through the deeper layers of visual cortex. This prediction can be tested by varying the number of reset-inducing events per unit time. Given that visual landmarks can influence the firing of place cells, the place-to-grid feedback also clarifies how visual landmarks can modulate the activity of both grid and place cells, whose primary activation is derived from path integration signals (O Keefe and Nadel, 1978; Hafting et al. 2005; Leutgeb et al., 2005; McNaughton et al., 2006). The model in this article may thus be tested by checking if beta oscillations are caused by inducing mismatch between path integration and visual estimates of an animal s position. In addition, the article summarizes many predictions about how grid cells give rise to place cells, and about the control circuitry that regulates refinement and stabilization of this learning process. 7

References Berke JD, Hetrick V, Breck J, Green RW. (2008). Transient 23- to 30-Hz oscillations in mouse hippocampus during exploration of novel environments. Hippocampus 18: 519-529. Buffalo EA, Fries P, Desimone R. 2004. Layer-specific attentional modulation in early visual areas. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 30: 717 716. Buschman TJ, Miller EK. (2008). Covert shifts in attention by frontal eye fields are synchronized to population oscillations. Submitted for publication. Carpenter GA, Grossberg S. 1987. A massively parallel architecture for a self-organizing neural pattern recognition machine. Comput Vis Graphics Image Proc 37:54-115. Carpenter GA, Grossberg S. 1991. Pattern Recognition by Self-Organizing Neural Networks. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Frank LM, Stanley GB, Brown EN. 2004. Hippocampal plasticity across multiple days of exposure to novel environments. J Neurosci 24:7681-7689. Gorchetchnikov A, Grossberg S. 2007. Space, time, and learning in the hippocampus: How fine spatial and temporal scales are expanded into population codes for behavioral control. Neural Networks 20:182-93. Grossberg S. 1975. A neural model of attention, reinforcement, and discrimination learning. Internatl. Rev. Neurobiol. 18: 263 327. Grossberg S. 1976. Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding, I: Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors. Biol Cybern 23: 121 134. Grossberg S. 1978. A theory of human memory: Self-organization and performance of sensorymotor codes, maps, and plans. In Rosen R and Snell F, editors. Progr Theoret Biol. Vol 5. New York: Academic Press, p. 233 374. Reprinted in Grossberg S. 1982. Studies of Mind and Brain. Amsterdam: Kluwer/Reidel. Grossberg S. 1980. How does a brain build a cognitive code. Psychol Rev 87:1-51. Grossberg, S. 1995. The attentive brain. American Scientist, 83, 438-449. Grossberg S. 1999. The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Conscious Cogn 8:1-44. Grossberg S. 2003. How does the cerebral cortex work? Development, learning, attention, and 3D vision by laminar circuits of visual cortex. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Revs. 2: 47-76. Grossberg S, Versace M. 2008. Spikes, synchrony, and attentive learning by laminar thalamocorical circuits. Brain Res, in press. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser MB, Moser E. 2005. Microstructure of the spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436(11): 801 806. Hartmann WM. 1997. Signals, sound, and sensation. New York: American Institute of Physics. Hasselmo, ME, Bodelon C, Wyble BP. 2002. A proposed function for hippocampal theta rhythm: Separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of prior learning. Neural Computation 14: 1-25. Kastner S, Underledier LG. 2001. The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex. Neuropsychologia 12:1263-1276. Kohonen T. 1984. Self-organization and associative memory. Springer-Verlag. Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Moser MB, and Moser EI. 2005. Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory. Current Opinion Neurobiol, 15:738-746. McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, and Moster MB. 2006. Path integration and the neural basis of the cognitive map. Nature Revs Neurosci 7: 663-678. Muller RA. 1996. A quarter of a century of place cells. Neuron 17:813-822. 8

O Keefe JM, Dostrovsky J. 1971. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research 34(1): 171 175. O Keefe JM and Nadel L. 1978. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon. Raizada R, Grossberg S. 2003. Towards a theory of the laminar architecture of cerebral cortex: Computational clues from the visual system. Cerebral Cortex 13: 100-113. Thompson LT, Best PJ. 1990. Long-term stability of the place-field activity of single units recorded from the dorsal hippocampus of freely behaving rats. Brain Res 509:299-308. Versace, M, Grossberg S. 2006. From spikes to inter-areal synchrony: How attentive matching and resonance control learning and information processing by laminar thalamocortical circuits. Society for Neuroscience annual meeting, 65.11/Z12. Vinogradova OS. 1975. Functional organization of the limbic system in the process of registration of information: Facts and hypotheses. In: Isaacson R.L., Pribram, K.H., eds., The hippocampus. Vol. 2, 3-69. New York: Plenum. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. 1993. Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science 261:1055-1058. Acknowledgements Stephen Grossberg was supported in part by the National Science Foundation (SBE-0354378) 9