The Category-Focus Implicit Association Test. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Journal link: This article does not

Similar documents
It s brief but is it better? An evaluation of the Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT) Klaus Rothermund 1 & Dirk Wentura 2

Clarifying the Role of the Other Category in the Self-Esteem IAT

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

Revised Top Ten List of Things Wrong with the IAT

Exercise effects in the Implicit Association Test (IAT)

Patricia Power, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes. Ian Stewart

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects?

Completing a Race IAT increases implicit racial bias

Free Associations as a Measure of Stable Implicit Attitudes

Assessing Anxiety with Extrinsic Simon Tasks

Consequential Validity of the Implicit Association Test: Comment on the Article by Blanton and Jaccard. Anthony G. Greenwald, University of Washington

Application of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to a Study of Deception

Method-Specific Variance in the Implicit Association Test

Mental Ownership: Does Mental Rehearsal Transform Novel Stimuli Into Mental Possessions? Teri A. Kirby. University of Exeter. Anthony G.

22/07/2014. Evaluations of new food technologies looking beyond the rational actor model. Question. Consumer response models

Beyond Reaction Times: Incorporating Mouse Tracking Measures into Implicit Association Test to. Examine Its Underlying Process

The Brief Implicit Association Test. N. Sriram University of Virginia. Anthony G. Greenwald University of Washington

Implicit Measures in Social and Personality Psychology

Using Implicit Measures in Attitude and Personality Research. Wilhelm Hofmann University of Chicago Booth School of Business

Strategies for Reducing Racial Bias and Anxiety in Schools. Johanna Wald and Linda R. Tropp November 9, 2013

Understanding and Using the Brief Implicit Association Test: I. Recommended Scoring Procedures. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia.

Is Kate Winslet more American than Lucy Liu? The impact of construal processes on the implicit ascription of a national identity

Validity of the Salience Asymmetry Interpretation of the Implicit Association Test: Comment on Rothermund and Wentura (2004)

Method-Specific Variance in the Implicit Association Test

A portable version of the go/no-go association task (GNAT)

Validity of a happiness Implicit Association Test as a measure of subjective well-being q

What Drives Priming Effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure?

Supplement to Nosek and Smyth (2005): Evidence for the generalizability of distinct implicit

Drug and Alcohol Dependence

Understanding and Using the Brief Implicit Association Test: Recommended Scoring Procedures

Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes. Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

Abstract Attitude-based interventions are often a key element in attempts to change behavior, but do not always

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI

Unconscious Knowledge Assessment

Colin Tucker Smith Ghent University. Kate A. Ratliff Tilburg University. Brian A. Nosek University of Virginia

A Comparative Investigation of Seven Indirect Attitude Measures

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Black 1 White 5 Black

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Forging links with the self to combat implicit bias

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Measuring and making use of implicit strengths : Toward further development of positive psychology. Hisamitsu Tsuda and Satoshi Shimai

Implicit measures of automatic evaluation

The Components of Mental Health and Test Anxiety in Talented and Normal Schools Students

Implicit Attitude Generalization Occurs Immediately; Explicit Attitude Generalization Takes Time Kate A. Ranganath and Brian A.

Early neural markers of implicit attitudes: N170 modulated by intergroup and evaluative contexts in IAT

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes

Mental Rehearsal Increases Liking for Repeatedly Exposed Stimuli. Teri A. Kirby

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

The Effects of Racial Similarity and Dissimilarity on the Joint Simon Task

Reliability and validity of the Single-Target IAT (ST-IAT): Assessing automatic affect towards multiple attitude objects

Methodological Issues for the IAT 1

Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition?

What Can Be Learned From Simulations?

Reducing Social Judgment Biases May Require Identifying the Potential Source of Bias

Supplementary Materials. Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Evolutionary Psychology. The Inescapable Mental Residue of Homo Categoricus. Book Review

BRIEF REPORT. Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse

The Implicit Self. Rutgers University, Piscataway, New Jersey, USA. University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

A Go/No-go approach to uncovering implicit attitudes towards safe and risky driving

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

Anti-fat, pro-slim, or both?

Attitudes and the Implicit-Explicit Dualism

!""#$%&'#%$#(#)!$)*&+,&+-*&#!+& & &.& & Adding Significance to the Implicit Association Test. Peter Stüttgen. Joachim Vosgerau.

Validity of the Salience Asymmetry Account of the Implicit Association Test: Reply to Greenwald, Nosek, Banaji, and Klauer (2005)

Brief Report. Is sport still a masculine domain? A psychological glance

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

The Effects of Color, Congruency and Distractors on Short Term Memory. Jenny Braun. Hanover College

Implicit Consumer Preferences and Their Influence on Product Choice

Measures Among Current Nondonors. A dissertation presented to. the faculty of. the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University

Words May Assess Implicit Attitudes About Fluent & Stuttered Speech

Through a Glass, Less Darkly?

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

No Measure Is Perfect, but Some Measures Can be Quite Useful

Consumer Persuasion: Indirect Change and Implicit Balance

Laila M. Martinussen Technical University of Denmark Implicit attitudes towards risky and safe driving in a Danish sample

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Balanced Identity Theory

Learning Within-Category Attribute Correlations in a One-Attribute Visual Search Classification Paradigm

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories

A longitudinal study on how implicit attitudes and explicit cognitions synergistically influence physical activity intention and behavior

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Think Leader, Think White? Capturing and Weakening an Implicit Pro-White Leadership Bias

Through a Glass, Less Darkly? Reassessing Convergent and Discriminant Validity in Measures of Implicit Self-Esteem.

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Implicit gambling attitudes in problem gamblers: Positive but not negative. implicit associations

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

The Positivity Effect in Attributions of Praise and Blame. Jamie Hughes and Daniel Krohn. New Mexico State University

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Explicit Control Of Implicit Responses Simple Directives Can Alter IAT Performance

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010.

Using Groups to Measure Intergroup. Prejudice. Erin Cooley 1 and B. Keith Payne 2. Prejudice. Article

Transcription:

Category-Focus IAT 1 RUNNING HEAD: The Category-Focus IAT The Category-Focus Implicit Association Test Frank Siebler 1, Roberto González 2, Gabriela Ordóñez 2, Gerd Bohner 3, Andrés Haye 2, David Sirlopú 2, Andres Millar 2, Pablo de Tezanos-Pinto 2, and David Torres 2 1 Universitetet i Tromsø 2 Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile 3 Universität Bielefeld This is the final author version of an article to appear in Social Psychology, 41, 2, 2010 by Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Journal link: http://www.hhpub.com/journals/sp/. This article does not exactly replicate the final version published in the journal Social Psychology. It is not a copy of the original published article and is not suitable for citation. Author Note Frank Siebler, Institutt for Psykologi, Universitetet i Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway; Roberto González, Gabriela Ordóñez, Andrés Haye, David Sirlopú, Andres Millar, Pablo de Tezanos-Pinto, and David Torres, Escuela de Psicología, Faculdad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile; Gerd Bohner, Abteilung für Psychologie, Universität Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany. Preparation of this manuscript was facilitated by grants from the Chilean National Board of Scientific and Technological Research (CONICyT) to Roberto González (FONDECYT no. 1020954), and from the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD) to Gerd Bohner (415-alechile/ale-02/21671). A preliminary report was presented at the British Psychological Society Psychology Section Annual Conference, Huddersfield, UK, September 11-13, 2002. Address correspondence to Frank Siebler, Institutt for Psykologi, Universitetet i Tromsø, 9037 Tromsø, Norway. Email address: frank@psyk.uit.no; telephone: +47-776-49232; fax: +47-776-45291.

Category-Focus IAT 2 Abstract We propose the Category-Focus IAT (CF-IAT) as an instrument to measure the implicit associations of single concepts. The CF-IAT aims to direct respondents attention to a subset of the experimental materials. In a first study using the CF-IAT, Chilean adolescents (N = 49), members of either the indigenous minority (Mapuche) or the non-indigenous majority, completed CF-IATs assessing implicit attitudes toward these groups. Results revealed, in both groups, neutral implicit evaluation of the ingroup, but negative implicit evaluation of the outgroup. Process evidence suggests that the CF-IAT s manipulation of attentional focus was successful. Keywords: implicit attitudes; intergroup relations; focus of attention Word counts: abstract 93; manuscript 2719

Category-Focus IAT 3 The Category-Focus Implicit Association Test Research on social cognition distinguishes between explicit cognition, which is accessible to introspection, and implicit cognition, which reflects introspectively unidentified or inappropriately identified past experience (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). A popular measure of implicit cognition is the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). It assesses the difference in association strength between each of two target concepts (e.g., Self and Other ) and an attribute dimension (e.g., positive or negative evaluation). Because of their relative (or comparative) nature, IAT scores are susceptible to apparently minor variations in the target concepts. For instance, implicit self-esteem scores, as assessed by a Self versus Other IAT, differed significantly if the Other concept referred to a close friend, rather than to an unspecified other (Karpinski, 2004; see also Brendl, Markman, & Messner, 2001). Several IAT-like singleconcept measures are available that may mitigate the relativity issue. Below we review these instruments. Then we propose the Category-Focus IAT (CF-IAT), a new single-concept measure that combines the strengths of currently available instruments. 1 Finally, we report CF-IAT results from a study on implicit intergroup attitudes. Measures of implicit association Two aspects are most relevant for the present purpose, namely the numbers of stimulus categories that are (a) used in the stimulus set, and (b) addressed in the task instructions. The Standard IAT. The IAT (Greenwald et al., 1998) measures the differential association of two target concepts (e.g. "Blacks" versus "Whites") with positive and negative evaluation (e.g. "pleasantness" versus "unpleasantness"). Stimuli representing these four categories are shown on the computer screen, one at a time. Participants respond by pressing one of two keys, according to task instructions that vary between trial blocks. In two critical blocks, task instructions assign two categories (both a target concept and an evaluation) to each response key. For instance, in the first block, participants may be asked to press the left-hand key if a White face or a pleasant word appears, but to press the right-hand key if a Black face or an unpleasant word appears. In the second

Category-Focus IAT 4 block, the assignment of target concepts (but not of evaluations) to response keys is reversed. If people associate one kind of faces more easily with pleasantness than with unpleasantness, then they are likely to complete one of the blocks more quickly than the other. The response speed difference between blocks defines a respondent s IAT score. A strength of the IAT stems from the use of stimuli from an even number of categories (four: two target concepts, and two evaluations). This design allows for a well-balanced composition of the set of stimuli presented to participants. Both within and across blocks, three dimensions of the stimulus set can simultaneously be held constant: (a) the number of stimuli per category, (b) the presentation frequency of each stimulus, and (c) the proportions of left-hand and right-hand responses required. Further, because each block uses the same stimulus set, taking a difference score between blocks automatically removes undesired sources of variance (e.g., effects of variability in the word length of stimuli). Thereby, IAT scores are protected against a range of threats to validity related to the composition of the stimulus set. A limitation of the IAT arises from the use of all four categories in the task instructions. By referring to all categories, the implicit evaluation of two target concepts is measured concurrently, yielding a single score that cannot be further decomposed (for discussion see Karpinski, 2004; Nosek & Banaji, 2001). Concurrent assessment is unique to the Standard IAT (see Table 1). Single Concept IATs. Single-concept IATs use three categories in both the stimulus set and the task instructions. The Single-Target IAT (ST-IAT; Wigboldus, Holland, & Van Knippenberg, 2006) has a target concept share a response key with positive evaluation in one of the blocks (e.g., Islamic & positive versus negative), but with negative evaluation in the other (e.g., positive versus Islamic & negative). The Single-Attribute IAT (Penke, Eichstaedt, & Asendorpf, 2006) and the Single-Category IAT (Karpinski & Steinman, 2006; see also Bohner, Siebler, González, Haye, & Schmidt, 2008) also use stimuli from three categories in each block, address all categories in the task instructions, and change the reponse key assignment for one of the categories between blocks.

Category-Focus IAT 5 While solving the issue of concurrent assessment, the omission of a fourth category in the single-concept IATs comes at a cost: With an uneven number of categories, the stimulus set cannot be balanced on the same number of dimensions as was possible with the Standard IAT. First, if the proportions of left-hand versus right-hand responses shall be held constant in a block, the category not sharing a key must be represented by a greater number of stimuli. Second, the category requiring greater presentation frequency of its stimuli is a different one in each block. Consequently, the composition of the stimulus set differs between blocks. Taking a difference score between blocks therefore removes undesired variance to a lesser degree than in the Standard IAT. 2 The Go/No-go Association Task. In the Go/No-go Association Task (GNAT; Nosek & Banaji, 2001), participants respond by either pressing ( go ) or not pressing ( no-go ) a single response key. To determine no-go decisions, response deadlines are applied, after which the experiment proceeds automatically. Importantly, the GNAT decouples the number of categories used in the stimulus set from the number of categories addressed in the task instructions: Task instructions name only those categories that require the go response. For example, in one block, participants were asked to press the key if a stimulus belonged to one of two categories (fruit & good), but not otherwise. By the actual composition of the stimulus set, the no-go response was thus tacitly assigned to two stimulus categories (bugs & bad) that remained unaddressed. In the next block, the assignment of evaluations to response options was reversed (fruit & bad versus, again tacitly, bugs & good). The performance difference between blocks was used as an index of the implicit attitude toward the directly addressed target concept (fruit). Thus, while measuring implicit attitudes towards single objects, the GNAT maintains the Standard IAT s balanced stimulus set. Key to the GNAT procedure is a division of categories into foreground versus background categories, by either addressing or not addressing a given category in the task instructions. Rather than removing stimulus categories from the experimental materials, the GNAT removes categories from respondents focus of attention. The Category-Focus IAT

Category-Focus IAT 6 Our review identified the GNAT as the single-concept measure of greatest methodological rigor. However, different from the IAT paradigm, the GNAT paradigm uses response deadlines. These are not trivial to determine (see Nosek & Banaji, 2001). Moreover, in the GNAT paradigm, response latency data are systematically unavailable for no-go trials. A complete set of response latencies, as provided by the IAT paradigm, appears desirable because it enables additional insight into cognitive processes. We shall demonstrate this in our Results section. From these considerations, we designed the CF-IAT to combine the strengths of the GNAT paradigm and the IAT paradigm. The CF-IAT uses balanced stimulus sets (comprising stimuli from four categories) and requires an active response to each stimulus. Its task instructions address a subset of the categories in the stimulus set. Specifically, whereas the categories sharing one of the response keys are addressed by their names (e.g., Black & pleasant), the categories sharing the other key are addressed by a summary label (e.g., Other). An implicit attitude score is computed as the performance difference between two blocks (e.g., Block 1: Black & pleasant vs. Other; Block 2: Black & unpleasant vs. Other). The present study We applied the CF-IAT in a larger study, conducted in 2002 and 2003, on the intergroup attitudes of non-indigenous Chileans and an indigenous Chilean minority, the Mapuche (Haye et al., in press). The relation between these groups is characterized by mutual prejudice and a history of conflict (Saiz, 1986, 2002). Therefore, members of either ethnic group should implicitly prefer their respective ingroup over the outgroup (see Greenwald et al., 1998, Study 2). Our participants completed two CF-IATs each. The same stimulus sets were used in both, but task instructions focused participants attention on either the ingroup or the outgroup. We expected the mere variation of category focus to reveal a pattern of implicit attitudes such that ingroup CF-IAT scores should be more favorable than outgroup CF-IAT scores. This prediction was tested against a null-hypothesis whereby the two CF-IAT scores might not differ from each other, or might show a different pattern of means than predicted.

Category-Focus IAT 7 Method Participants Fifty eight students from four schools in Santiago, Chile, participated in the study. They attended courses for students of age 12-14 years. The data of nine participants were discarded due to an excessive number of overly quick responses (see below). The final sample comprised 49 participants (27 indigenous, 22 non-indigenous). Design and variables Participant origin (ethnicity: indigenous vs. non-indigenous) was treated as a betweensubjects factor. A separate CF-IAT was conducted toward each target group, constituting the two levels of a within-subjects factor (category focus: on the Indigenous vs. on the Non-Indigenous). Note that the latter factor reflects the CF-IAT s core mechanism, the variation of task instructions. Within each CF-IAT, critical blocks were run twice. That served to familiarize participants with the task, but also allowed to compute two CF-IAT scores towards the same target; subsequent analyses treated the dual scores as the two levels of a within-subjects factor (block type: practice vs. test). The dependent variables were participants four (2 target groups X 2 block types) CF-IAT scores each. Materials and procedure Stimulus materials. The stimuli were drawn from a pretested pool of pleasant words, unpleasant words, indigenous stimuli, and non-indigenous stimuli (six to seven items each). Example words are, pleasant: peace, love; unpleasant: war, sadness. The stimuli referring to social groups were typical surnames (e.g., indigenous: Huilcaleo; non-indigenous: Morales) and digital photographs of indigenous and non-indigenous Chileans. Trial blocks. Participants completed ten trial blocks (see Table 2). A block s assignment of categories to response keys was indicated by initial instructions, and by labels in the top-left and top-right corners of the screen. Importantly, whereas we actually presented stimuli from all four categories in each critical block, instructions and on-screen labels directly addressed only a subset

Category-Focus IAT 8 (two of four) of these categories. Stimuli were presented one by one, centered on the computer screen. Incorrect responses were indicated by the letter X in red color and needed to be corrected. Correct responses triggered a 250ms blank-screen interval, followed by the next stimulus. Practice blocks comprised 28 stimuli; test blocks comprised 40 stimuli. Scoring. CF-IAT scores depict the implicit evaluation of the target group that was addressed in the task instructions. We computed separate scores from practice block data and test block data, respectively. Four scores were computed: Non-Indigenous target / practice (Block 3 minus Block 7), Non-Indigenous target / test (Block 4 minus Block 8), Mapuche target / practice (Block 9 minus Block 5), and Mapuche target / test (Block 10 minus Block 6). The computation of scores followed the improved IAT scoring algorithm (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003), yielding a measure called D. In line with recommendations, we removed cases with more than ten percent of overly quick responses (i.e., faster than 300 ms). On the resulting CF-IAT D scores, a positive sign indicates positive evaluation of the target group, whereas a negative sign indicates negative evaluation of the target group. Results Category-Focus IAT Reliability of measures. Practice block and test block CF-IAT scores correlated positively and significantly, both when the target group was the ingroup, r(47) =.30, p =.037, and when the target group was the outgroup, r(47) =.28, p =.049. For comparison, the correlation of scores towards different target groups was small and nonsignificant (practice blocks: r =.10; test blocks: r =.05), suggesting that the two CF-IAT scores did assess distinct attitudes as intended. Difference between conditions. We entered participants four CF-IAT scores into a mixedmodel ANOVA with participant ethnicity (indigenous vs. non-indigenous) as between-subjects factor, and repeated measures on block type (practice vs. test) as well as category focus (on the Mapuche vs. on the Non-Indigenous). See Table 3 for means and standard deviations. The analysis

Category-Focus IAT 9 revealed a significant two-way interaction of participant ethnicity and category focus, F(1,47) = 17.31, p <.001, MSE = 0.85; all other F < 1.5. To follow up on the interaction we repeated the analysis within participant groups. For nonindigenous participants, scores were comparatively more positive when category focus was on the Non-Indigenous rather than the Mapuche, F(1,21) = 13.07, p =.002, MSE = 0.68; all other F < 1. For indigenous participants, scores were comparatively more positive when the target group were the Mapuche rather than the Non-Indigenous, F(1,26) = 5.98, p =.022, MSE = 1.00; all other F < 1. Thus, the predicted pattern of intergroup attitudes was observed in both subsamples. Difference from zero. CF-IAT scores were submitted to t-tests for difference from zero (neutral). For either participant group, scores towards the respective ingroup were neutral, whereas scores towards the respective outgroup were unequivocally negative (see Table 3). Process evidence A final set of analyses addressed whether participants focused on the intended subset of categories. Directly naming some categories in the task instructions, while referring to others only indirectly (with the summary label Other ), should render the former categories more accessible in memory than the latter. Heightened accessibility of a category, in turn, should facilitate responding to its associated stimuli. To test for the expected response facilitation, we computed two response latency means. The first mean averaged across those trials where the stimulus category was named directly. The second mean averaged across the remaining trials (where the stimulus needed to be classified as Other ). A difference score was then derived by subtracting the former mean score from the latter, and was transformed into a D score. On the resulting score, a positive (negative) sign indicates that participants responded more (less) quickly if stimuli belonged to a category that was addressed directly, rather than by the summary label. Separate scores were computed for each of the four (block type x target group) within-subjects conditions. Difference between conditions. We submitted participants four scores each to a mixedmodel ANOVA with participant ethnicity (indigenous vs. non-indigenous) as between-subjects

Category-Focus IAT 10 factor, and repeated measures on block type (practice vs. test) as well as category focus (on the Mapuche vs. on the Non-Indigenous). As the only effect approaching significance in this analysis, test blocks tended to have more positive scores than practice blocks, F(1,47) = 3.54, p =.066, MSE = 0.06; all other F < 1.9. Difference from zero. After collapsing across factors that had not shown an effect in the ANOVA, scores were submitted to t-tests for difference from zero. Whereas practice block scores (M = 0.02, SD = 0.19) did not differ significantly from zero, t < 1, ns., test block scores (M = 0.08, SD = 0.16) did, t(48) = 3.74, p <.001. Thus, for test blocks, the category focus manipulation demonstrably resulted in an expected response facilitation. Discussion The CF-IAT is a new measure of single concepts implicit associations; it was designed to integrate the strengths of existing instruments. A first study with the CF-IAT revealed the predicted pattern of implicit intergroup attitudes in a sample of Chilean students. The CF-IAT aims to direct respondents attention to a subset of the categories that comprise the stimulus set. The process evidence suggests that respondents did in fact focus their attention as prescribed by task instructions, and did so more strongly with greater practice. CF-IAT scores computed from practice blocks and test blocks showed a significant but low correlation. This is likely to underestimate the instrument s internal consistency, which may increase with more sophisticated analyses, or (as suggested by the process evidence) when giving respondents more practice. Finally, we should point out that the CF- IAT does not assess implicit associations in an absolute or context-free way. For the time being, however, the CF-IAT appears a promising tool for the assessment of single-concept associations.

Category-Focus IAT 11 Endnotes 1 Sriram and Greenwald (2009) independently developed a similar instrument, the Brief Implicit Association Test. 2 It should be noted that the authors of single-concept IATs devised measures to reduce the impact of confounds, for example in the calculation of the score.

Category-Focus IAT 12 References Bohner, G., Siebler, F., González, R., Haye, A., & Schmidt, E. A. (2008). Situational flexibility of in-group-related attitudes: A single category IAT study of people with dual national identity. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 11, 301-317. Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 760 773. Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4-27. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197-216. Haye, A., González, R., Ordóñez, G., Bohner, G., Siebler, F., Sirlopú, D., Millar, D., De Tezanos- Pinto, P., & Torres, D. (in press). System-perpetuating asymmetries between explicit and implicit intergroup attitudes among indigenous and non-indigenous Chileans. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. Karpinski, A. (2004). Measuring self-esteem using the Implicit Association Test: The role of the other. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 22-34. Karpinski, A. & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16-32. Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625-664.

Category-Focus IAT 13 Penke, L., Eichstaedt, J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2006). Single-attribute implicit association tests (SA- IAT) for the assessment of unipolar constructs: The case of sociosexuality. Experimental Psychology, 53, 283-291. Saiz, J. L. (1986). Estereotipos adscritos al indígena mapuche por adultos no mapuches de Chile meridional. Revista Interamericana de Psicología, 20, 55-68. Saiz, J. L. (2002). Atribución de estereotipos: Los indígenas mapuches que perciben los chilenos. In J. F. Morales, D. Páez, A. L. Kornblit, & D. Asún (Eds.), Psicología social (pp. 145-151). Buenos Aires: Pearson Education. Sriram, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The Brief Implicit Association Test. Experimental Psychology, 56, 283-294. Wigboldus, D. H. J., Holland, R. W., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2006). Single target implicit associations. Manuscript under review.

Category-Focus IAT 14 Table 1 Properties of Measures of Implicit Association Measure No. of distinct No. of Assessment of categories response keys 2nd target Stimulus Task set instructions Standard IAT 4 4 2 concurrently Single Concept IATs 3 3 2 sequentially Go/No-go AT 4 2 1 sequentially Category-Focus IAT 4 2 2 sequentially Notes. The table depicts properties of measures of implicit association. Column Stimulus set shows the number of distinct categories that comprise the stimulus set. Column Task instructions shows the count of categories in the stimulus set that are directly addressed in task instructions. Column No. of response keys shows the count of response keys available to participants. Column Assessment of 2nd target reflects whether attitudes toward first and second target object (if any) are assessed simultaneously or one after the other. AT = association task; IAT = implicit association test.

Category-Focus IAT 15 Table 2 Trial-Block Order Block No. Left-hand label Right-hand label 1 MAPUCHE NON-INDIGENOUS 2 PLEASANT UNPLEASANT 3 NON-INDIGENOUS or UNPLEASANT OTHER 4 NON-INDIGENOUS or UNPLEASANT OTHER 5 MAPUCHE or PLEASANT OTHER 6 MAPUCHE or PLEASANT OTHER 7 NON-INDIGENOUS or PLEASANT OTHER 8 NON-INDIGENOUS or PLEASANT OTHER 9 MAPUCHE or UNPLEASANT OTHER 10 MAPUCHE or UNPLEASANT OTHER Notes. Ten trial blocks were completed. Eight of these were critical: four practice blocks (3, 5, 7, and 9) as well as four test blocks (4, 6, 8, and 10).

Category-Focus IAT 16 Table 3 Category-Focus IAT Scores as a Function of Participant Ethnicity and Target Group Participant Ethnicity Mapuche Non-Indigenous Target Group Practice Mapuche -.02 (.87) -.68 * (.90) Non-Indigenous -.36 + (.96).01 (.97) Test Mapuche -.03 (.77) -.50 * (.68) Non-Indigenous -.62 ** (.68).08 (.68) Practice & Test Mapuche -.02 (.63) -.59 ** (.61) Non-Indigenous -.49 * (.69).05 (.70) Notes. Cell entries depict D-scores on the Category-Focus-IAT measures, separately for practice blocks (top panel), test blocks (middle panel), and practice blocks plus test blocks combined (bottom panel). Positive (negative) scores indicate positive (negative) implicit evaluation of the target group. Figures in parentheses are standard deviations. Difference from zero: + p <.10, * p <.01, ** p <.001. Cell N, from left to right: 27, 22.