Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF. Interpretive Report. Developed by SAMPLE

Similar documents
by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

Teacher Form Interpretive Report

Tasks of Executive Control TEC. Interpretive Report. Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

Checklist for Executive Function Skills Elementary Level

Tests/subtests that may capture this skill a,b. How it might look in school or in the home c Response inhibition

Tasks of Executive Control TEC. Score Report. Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

Executive Functioning

A Look Inside. Executive Functioning Deficits. Wendy Kelly, M.A., C. Psych. Assoc. Psychological Services, Kawartha Pine Ridge DSB

SAMPLE. Autism Spectrum Rating Scales (6-18 Years) Teacher Ratings. Interpretive Report. By Sam Goldstein, Ph.D. & Jack A. Naglieri, Ph.D.

Score Report N. Florida Ave. Lutz, FL

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Parent Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

SAMPLE REPORT. Conners 3 Parent Short Form Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Tasks of Executive Control TEC. Protocol Summary Report. Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

An Overview of The New Brown Executive Function/Attention Scales (Brown EF/A Scales) Presenter and Author. Change in Understanding ADHD

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Teacher Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Interpretive Report. Developed by Peter R. Vagg, PhD, and Charles D. Spielberger, PhD. Client Information

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Samantha Wright. September 03, 2003

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Samantha Wright. September 03, 2003

Children's Depression Inventory 2nd Edition: Parent Maria Kovacs, Ph.D.

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Beyond Lazy and Unmotivated

State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory Interpretive Report (STAXI-2: IR ) by Peter R. Vagg, PhD, and Charles D. Spielberger, PhD. Client Information

ADD/ADHD AND EF. Attention Deficit Disorder/ Attention Deficit with Hyperactivity Disorder and Executive Function Difficulties

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

HTS Report EPS. Emotional Processing Scale. Technical Report. Anna Patient ID Date 29/09/2016. Hogrefe Ltd, Oxford

Overview. The Evidence for Executive Function Assessment 11/9/2016

The Attentional and Interpersonal Style (TAIS) Inventory: Measuring the Building Blocks of Performance

Managing ADHD. Strategies and Modifications for Counselors and Teachers

SUPPORT INFORMATION ADVOCACY

Connecting Attention, Memory. Attention + Memory= LEARNING

ADHD and social skills M. T. LAX-PERICALL CONSULTANT IN CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHIATRY PRIORY HOSPITAL ROEHAMPTON

An Introduction to the CBS Health Cognitive Assessment

Improving Attention and Learning in Children & Adolescents: The Role of Working Memory

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder How to manage these disorganized and inattentive children.

ADHD Dan Shapiro, M.D. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics

Attention Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders

Identifying Information

By Reuven Bar-On. Development Report

Interpretive Report. Client Information

Piers Harris Children s Self-Concept Scale, Second Edition (Piers-Harris 2)

ADDES-3 QUICK SCORE SCHOOL VERION PROFILE SAMPLE

SELF-ESTEEM AND HUMAN RELATIONSHIPS 9. SELF-ESTEEM AND ADHD

Snohomish Middle School 321 West B Street Snohomish, Wa Initial Evaluation

Social and Emotional Foundations of Transitional Kindergarten

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

ADHD Best Strategies 2018

ADD / ADHD in Children

Changing Behavior. Can t get up. Refuses to get up for school. I like school. Sad Poor sleep Angry Thoughts of self harm.

SAMPLE. Behavior Parent Short Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

SAMPLE. Behavior Parent Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

The Role of Executive Functions in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disabilities

Epilepsy and Neuropsychology

FACT SHEET. Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD or ADHD) General Overview (updated 2018)

Parenting, Placement & Dysregulation

MCG-CNVAMC CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY INTERNSHIP INTERN EVALUATION (Under Revision)

Cognitive Changes Workshop Outcomes

Helping the Student With ADHD in the Classroom

Insight Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI)

Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD)/Attention Deficit w/ Hyperactivity Self Test

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Understanding and improving attention and information processing in MS

Overview. Case #1 4/20/2012. Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why?

EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING: FROM STRUGGLE TO SUCCESS

ADHD & Executive Functioning

Difference between ADHD and Executive Functioning. Dr. Josette Abdalla

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism

ADHD or Just Late, Lost, and Unorganized. Presented by: Cyle A. Feingold Phone:

ADHD in the Classroom

Research on SCT at Leiden University: Cohorts: Personal roadmap: Mix of prenatal follow-up and referred cases. Sophie van Rijn

Being Mindful About Mindfulness: A System Wide Approach

Life History Screen. a. Were you raised by someone other than your biologic/birth parents? Yes No

Personal Talent Skills Inventory

The Executive Function Toolbox Susan McKenney

Chapter 3. Perception and Memory. Factors That Affect Teacher s Guidance

Functional Approaches to Managing Memory and Cognitive Deficits in Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury. Janice Osborne Dowdy, MS, CCC-SLP, CBIS

SAMPLE. Conners Clinical Index Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Thinking and Memory in MS

Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment

6/11/2014. Session 2. INTERPERSONAL COMMUNICATION and EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE. Learning objectives. Learning objectives continued

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

7/31/2017. Outline. What is mindfulness? What is complex ADHD? Mindful Awareness and ADHD Care. Complex ADHD & Mindfulness

IIII. Assessment. by the. Numbers6

Creation and Use of the Pervasive Developmental Disorder Behavior Inventory (PDDBI) Parent Form

Mental Health. Integration. School Baseline Evaluation Packet. The school has my permission to return forms directly to the clinic.

Attention- Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Parent Talk. Presented by: Dr. Barbara Kennedy, R.Psych. Dr. Marei Perrin, R.Psych.

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder The Impact of ADHD on Learning. Miranda Shields, PsyD

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Emotional Intelligence Version

Executive Functions and ADHD

HOW IS PACE TO BE USED

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

BBHI 2 Brief Battery for Health Improvement 2 STANDARD REPORT PATIENT INFORMATION

Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE. Prepared for: By: Session: 22 Jul Madeline Bertrand. Sample Organization

Autism Spectrum Disorder Through the Lifespan

SUBSCALE DEFINITION LOW SCORE HIGH SCORE. Good Attachment Good relationships with others Hostile toward authority Positive attitude toward authority

Aspect Positive Behaviour Support

Transcription:

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF Interpretive Report Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff Client Information Client Name : Sample Client Client ID : SC 987 Gender : Male Grade: 2 nd. Age : 8 Birthdate : 02/13/2000 Test Date : 02/29/2008 Test Form : Parent Form Test Description : ADHD Rater Name : Mrs Client Relationship to Client : Parent PAR Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. / 16204 North Florida Ave. / Lutz, FL 33549 / 1.800.331.8378 / www.parinc.com BRIEF : Interpretive Report Copyright 1996, 1998, 2000, 2001, 2002, 2003, 2004, 2007, 2008 by Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. All rights reserved. May not be reproduced in whole or in part in any form or by any means without written permission of Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Version: 3.01.007

Client ID: SC 987 Page 2 of 10 The BRIEF was developed to provide a window into the everyday behavior associated with specific domains of the executive functions. The BRIEF can serve as a screening tool for possible executive dysfunction. The clinical information gathered from an in-depth profile analysis is best understood, however, within the context of a full assessment that includes (a) a detailed history of the child and the family, (b) performance-based testing, and (c) observations of the child s behavior. A thorough understanding of the BRIEF, including its development and its psychometric properties, is a prerequisite to interpretation. As with any clinical method or procedure, appropriate training and clinical supervision is necessary to ensure competent use of the BRIEF. This report is confidential and intended for use by qualified professionals only. This report should not be released to the parents or teachers of the child being evaluated. If a summary of the results specifically written for parents and teachers is desired, the BRIEF Feedback Report can be generated and given to the interested parents and/or teachers. T scores are used to interpret the child s level of executive functioning as reported by parents and/or teachers on the BRIEF rating forms. These scores are linear transformations of the raw scale scores (M = 50, SD = 10). T scores provide information about an individual s scores relative to the scores of respondents in the standardization sample. Percentiles, which are also presented within the BRIEF-SP, represent the percentage of children in the standardization sample who fall below a given raw score. In the process of interpreting the BRIEF, review of individual items within each scale can yield useful information for understanding the specific nature of the child s elevated score on any given clinical scale. In addition, certain items may be particularly relevant to specific clinical groups. Placing too much interpretive significance on individual items, however, is not recommended due to lower reliability of individual items relative to the scales and indexes. Overview Sample's parent completed the Parent form of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) on 02/29/2008. There are no missing item responses in the protocol. Responses are reasonably consistent. The respondent s ratings of Sample do not appear overly negative. In the context of these validity considerations, ratings of Sample's executive function exhibited in everyday behavior reveal some areas of concern. The overall index, the Global Executive Composite (GEC), was elevated (GEC T = 66, %ile = 93). The Metacognition Index (MI) was within normal limits (MI T = 62, %ile = 85) while the Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) was elevated (BRI T = 69, %ile = 96). Within these summary indicators, all of the individual scales are valid. One or more of the individual BRIEF scales were elevated, suggesting that Sample exhibits difficulty with some aspects of executive function. Concerns are noted with his ability to inhibit impulsive responses (Inhibit T = 80, %ile = 98), sustain working memory (Working Memory T = 67, %ile = 95), and plan and organize problem solving approaches (Plan/Organize T = 65, %ile = 91). Sample's ability to adjust to changes in routine or task demands (Shift T = 57, %ile = 79), modulate emotions (Emotional Control T = 60, %ile = 86), initiate problem solving or activity (Initiate T = 53, %ile = 70), organize his environment and materials (Organization of Materials T = 45, %ile = 35), and monitor his own behavior (Monitor T = 62, %ile = 91) is not described as problematic by the respondent.

Client ID: SC 987 Page 3 of 10 BRIEF Score Summary Table Index/Scale Raw Score T Score Percentile 90% C.I. Inhibit 29 80 98 75-85 Shift 14 57 79 49-65 Emotional Control 21 60 86 55-65 Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) 64 69 96 65-73 Initiate 14 53 70 45-61 Working Memory 24 67 95 62-72 Plan/Organize 27 65 91 59-71 Organization of Materials 10 45 35 39-51 Monitor 19 62 91 54-70 Metacognition Index (MI) 94 62 85 58-66 Global Executive Composite (GEC) 158 66 93 63-69 Scale Raw Score Cumulative Percentile Protocol Classification Negativity 0 90 Acceptable Inconsistency 0 98 Acceptable Note: Male,age-specific norms have been used to generate this profile. For additional normative information, refer to Appendix A - D in the BRIEF Professional Manual.

Client ID: SC 987 Page 4 of 10 Profile of BRIEF T Scores T Score 100 T Score 100 95 95 90 90 85 80 75 70 65 60 55 50 45 40 85 80 75 70 65 60 55 50 45 40 35 35 30 Emotional Working Plan/ Org. of Inhibit Shift Control Initiate Memory Organize Materials Monitor BRI MI GEC T Score 80 57 60 53 67 65 45 62 69 62 66 Percentile 98 79 86 70 95 91 35 91 96 85 93 Raw score 29 14 21 14 24 27 10 19 64 94 158 Missing n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a Note: Male,age-specific norms have been used to generate this profile. For additional normative information, refer to Appendixes A - D in the BRIEF Professional Manual. 30

Client ID: SC 987 Page 5 of 10 Validity Before examining the BRIEF profile, it is essential to carefully consider the validity of the data provided. The inherent nature of rating scales (i.e., relying upon a third party for ratings of a child s behavior) brings potential bias to the scores. The first step is to examine the protocol for missing data. With a valid number of responses, the Inconsistency and Negativity scales of the BRIEF provide additional validity indexes. Missing items The respondent completed 86 of a possible 86 BRIEF items. There are no missing responses in the protocol, providing a complete data set for interpretation. Inconsistency Scores on the Inconsistency scale indicate the extent to which the respondent answered similar BRIEF items in an inconsistent manner relative to the clinical samples. For example, a high Inconsistency score might be associated with marking Never in response to the item Gets out of control more than friends while at the same time marking Often in response to the item Acts too wild or out of control. T scores are not generated for the Inconsistency scale. Instead, the raw difference scores for the 10 paired items are summed and the total difference score (i.e., the Inconsistency score) is used to classify the protocol as either Acceptable, Questionable, or as Inconsistent. The Inconsistency score of 0 falls within the Acceptable range, suggesting that the rater was reasonably consistent in responding to BRIEF items. Negativity The Negativity scale measures the extent to which the respondent answered selected BRIEF items in an unusually negative manner relative to the clinical sample. A higher raw score on this scale indicates a greater degree of negativity, with less than 3% of respondents scoring above 7 in the clinical sample. As with the Inconsistency scale, T scores are not generated for this scale. The Negativity score of 0 falls within the acceptable range, suggesting that the respondent s view of Sample is not overly negative and that the BRIEF protocol is likely to be valid. Global Executive Composite Composite and Summary Indexes The Global Executive Composite (GEC) is an overarching summary score that incorporates all of the BRIEF clinical scales. Although review of the Metacognition Index, Behavioral Regulation Index, and individual scale scores is strongly recommended for all BRIEF profiles, the GEC can sometimes be useful as a summary measure. In this case, the two summary indexes are not substantially different, with T scores separated by 7 points. Thus, the GEC adequately captures the nature of the overall profile. With this in mind, Sample's T score of 66 (%ile = 93) on the GEC is moderately elevated as compared to the scores of his peers, suggesting some difficulty in one or more areas of executive function.

Client ID: SC 987 Page 6 of 10 Behavioral Regulation and Metacognition Indexes The Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) captures the rated child s ability to shift cognitive set and modulate emotions and behavior via appropriate inhibitory control. It is comprised of the Inhibit, the Shift, and the Emotional Control scales. Intact behavioral regulation is likely to be a precursor to appropriate metacognitive problem solving. Behavioral regulation enables the metacognitive processes to successfully guide active systematic problem solving; and more generally, behavioral regulation supports appropriate self-regulation. The Metacognition Index (MI) reflects the rated child s ability to initiate, plan, organize, self-monitor, and sustain working memory. It can be interpreted as Sample's ability to cognitively self-manage tasks and to monitor his performance. The MI relates directly to a child s ability to actively problem solve in a variety of contexts. It is composed of the Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials, and Monitor scales. Examination of the indexes reveals that the Behavioral Regulation Index is elevated (T = 69, %ile = 96) while the Metacognition Index is within the average range (T = 62, %ile = 85). This suggests difficulties with inhibitory control, emotional modulation, or ability to shift set but also suggests relatively preserved working memory, initiating, planning, organizing, and self-monitoring. Clinical Scales The BRIEF clinical scales measure the extent to which the respondent reports problems with different types of behavior related to the eight domains of executive functioning. The following sections describe the scores obtained on the clinical scales and the suggested interpretation for each individual clinical scale. Inhibit The Inhibit scale assesses inhibitory control and impulsivity. This can be described as the ability to resist impulses and the ability to stop one s own behavior at the appropriate time. Sample's score on this scale is highly elevated (T = 80, %ile = 98) as compared to his peers. Children with similar scores on the Inhibit scale typically have marked difficulty resisting impulses and difficulty considering consequences before acting. They are often perceived as (a) less in control of themselves than their peers, (b) having difficulty staying in place in line or in the classroom, (c) interrupting others or calling out in class frequently, and (d) requiring higher levels of adult supervision. Often, caregivers and teachers are particularly concerned about the verbal and social intrusiveness and the lack of personal safety observed in children who do not inhibit impulses well. Such children may display high levels of physical activity, inappropriate physical responses to others, a tendency to interrupt and disrupt group activities, and a general failure to look before leaping. In the contexts of the classroom and assessment settings, children with inhibitory control difficulties often require a higher degree of external structure to limit their impulsive responding. They may start an activity or task before listening to instructions, before developing a plan, or before grasping the organization or gist of the situation. Examination of the individual items that comprise the Inhibit scale may be informative and may help guide interpretation and intervention.

Client ID: SC 987 Page 7 of 10 Shift The Shift scale assesses the ability to move freely from one situation, activity, or aspect of a problem to another as the circumstances demand. Key aspects of shifting include the ability to (a) make transitions, (b) tolerate change, (c) problem-solve flexibly, (d) switch or alternate attention, and (e) change focus from one mindset or topic to another. Sample's score on the Shift scale falls within the average range as compared to like-aged peers (T = 57, %ile = 79). This suggests that Sample is generally able to change from task to task or from place to place without difficulty, is able to think of or accept different ways of solving problems, and is able to demonstrate flexibility in the performance of day to day activities. Emotional Control The Emotional Control scale measures the impact of executive function problems on emotional expression and assesses a child s ability to modulate or control his or her emotional responses. Sample's score on the Emotional Control scale falls within the average range as compared to like-aged peers (T = 60, %ile = 86). This suggests that Sample demonstrates an appropriate ability to modulate or regulate emotions overall. Sample is generally described as reacting to events appropriately; without outbursts, sudden and/or frequent mood changes, or excessive periods of emotional upset. Initiate The Initiate scale reflects a child s ability to begin a task or activity and to independently generate ideas, responses, or problem-solving strategies. Sample's score on the Initiate scale is within the average range as compared to like-aged peers (T = 53, %ile = 70). This suggests that Sample is generally able to begin, start or get going on tasks, activities, and problem-solving approaches appropriately. Working Memory The Working Memory scale measures on-line representational memory; that is, the capacity to hold information in mind for the purpose of completing a task, encoding information, or generating goals, plans, and sequential steps to achieving goals. Working memory is essential to carry out multistep activities, complete mental manipulations such as mental arithmetic, and follow complex instructions. Sample's score on the Working Memory scale is moderately elevated as compared to like-aged peers (T = 67, %ile = 95). This suggests that Sample has some difficulty holding an appropriate amount of information in mind or in active memory for further processing, encoding, and/or mental manipulation. Further, Sample's score suggests difficulties sustaining working memory, which has a negative impact on his ability to remain attentive and focused for appropriate lengths of time. Caregivers describe children with fragile or limited working memory as having trouble remembering things (e.g., phone numbers or instructions) even for a few seconds, losing track of what they are doing as they work, or forgetting what they are supposed to retrieve when sent on an errand. They often miss information that exceeds their working memory capacity such as instructions for an assignment. Clinical evaluators may observe that a child cannot remember the rules governing a specific task (even as he or she works on that task), rehearses information repeatedly, loses track of what responses he or she has already given on a task that requires multiple answers, and struggles with mental manipulation tasks (e.g., repeating digits in reverse order) or solving arithmetic problems that are orally presented without writing down figures.

Client ID: SC 987 Page 8 of 10 Appropriate working memory is necessary to sustain performance and attention. Parents of children with difficulties in this domain report that the children cannot stick to an activity for an age-appropriate amount of time and frequently switch tasks or fail to complete tasks. Although working memory and the ability to sustain it have been conceptualized as distinct entities, behavioral outcomes of these two domains are often difficult to distinguish. Plan/Organize The Plan/Organize scale measures the child s ability to manage current and future-oriented task demands. The scale is comprised of two components: plan and organize. The plan component captures the ability to anticipate future events, to set goals, and to develop appropriate sequential steps ahead of time in order to carry out a task or activity. The organize component refers to the ability to bring order to information and to appreciate main ideas or key concepts when learning or communicating information. Sample's score on the Plan/Organize scale is moderately elevated as compared to like-aged peers (T = 65, %ile = 91). This suggests that Sample has some difficulty with the planning and the organization of information which has a negative impact on his approach to problem solving. Planning involves developing a goal or end state and then strategically determining the most effective method or steps to attain that goal. Evaluators can observe planning when a child is given a problem requiring multiple steps (e.g., assembling a puzzle or completing a maze). Sample may underestimate the time required to complete tasks or the level of difficulty inherent in a task. He may often wait until the last minute to begin a long-term project or assignment for school, and he may have trouble carrying out the actions needed to reach his goals. Organization involves the ability to organize oral and written expression as well as to understand the main points expressed in presentations or written material. Organization also has a clerical component that is demonstrated, for example, in the ability to efficiently scan a visual array or to keep track of a homework assignment. Sample may approach tasks in a haphazard fashion, getting caught up in the details and missing the big picture. He may have good ideas that he fails to express on tests and written assignments. He may often feel overwhelmed by large amounts of information and may have difficulty retrieving material spontaneously or in response to open-ended questions. He may, however, exhibit better performance with recognition (multiple choice) questions. Organization of Materials The Organization of Materials scale measures orderliness of work, play, and storage spaces (e.g., desks, lockers, backpacks, and bedrooms). Caregivers and teachers typically can provide an abundance of examples describing a child s ability to organize, keep track of, and/or clean up their belongings. Sample's score on the Organization of Materials scale falls within the average range for like-aged children (T = 45, %ile = 35). Sample is described as being able to keep materials and his belongings reasonably well organized, as having his materials readily available for projects or assignments, and as being able to find his belongings when needed. Monitor The Monitor scale assesses two types of monitoring behaviors: Task-oriented monitoring or work-checking habits and Self-monitoring or interpersonal awareness. The task monitoring portion of the scale captures whether a child assesses his or her own performance during or shortly after finishing a task to ensure accuracy or appropriate attainment of a goal. The self monitoring portion of the scale evaluates whether a child keeps track of the effect that his or her

Client ID: SC 987 Page 9 of 10 behavior has on others. Sample's score on the Monitor scale is within normal limits, suggesting an appropriate overall level of monitoring (T = 62, %ile = 91).

Client ID: SC 987 Page 10 of 10 Executive System Intervention (This section removed for sample report) End of Report