Flexibility of working memory encoding in a sentence picture sound verification task

Similar documents
Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

Encoding of Information in Auditory Displays: Initial Research on Flexibility and Processing Codes in Dual-task Scenarios

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010.

To link to this article:

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014.

To link to this article:

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods

HOW DO PEOPLE THINK THEY REMEMBER MELODIES AND TIMBRES? PHENOMENOLOGICAL REPORTS OF MEMORY FOR NONVERBAL SOUNDS

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013.

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington

Published online: 17 Feb 2011.

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

ENCODING AND REPRESENTATION OF INFORMATION IN AUDITORY GRAPHS: DESCRIPTIVE REPORTS OF LISTENER STRATEGIES FOR UNDERSTANDING DATA

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Memory Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Ineke J. M. van der Ham a & Gregoire Borst b a Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Online publication date: 08 June 2010

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

Satiation in name and face recognition

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Using the method of adjustment to enhance collision warning perception

Selective attention to words and colours John Morton a ; Susan M. Chambers a a

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning

Medical Research Council, Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Sometimes Losing Your Self in Space: Children s and Adults Spontaneous Use of Multiple Spatial Reference Frames

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE.

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the

Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance. Amy B. Adcock. The University of Memphis

Working Memory and Mental Health: A Primer for the Mental Health Professional

Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of S-R episodes

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA. Online publication date: 16 February 2011 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Shifting Covert Attention to Spatially Indexed Locations Increases Retrieval Performance of Verbal Information

Running head: SIZE BRIGHTNESS MAPPING 1. Cross-Sensory Mapping of Feature Values in the Size-Brightness Correspondence can be

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

A model of parallel time estimation

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Classifying pictures and words: Implications for the dual-coding hypothesis

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

A Project to Clone Companion Animals Mark Greene Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

INTRODUCTION METHODS

Auditory Scene Analysis

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Groningen, The Netherlands

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model

Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events

TICK-MARKS, AXES, AND LABELS: THE EFFECTS OF ADDING CONTEXT TO AUDITORY GRAPHS

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

The allocation of visual short-term memory capacity: Evidence for a flexible storage mechanism

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory

Research Review: Multiple Resource Theory. out in multi-task environments. Specifically, multiple display layout and control design

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

Where is the donut? Factors influencing spatial reference frame use

The number line effect reflects top-down control

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

AUDITORY GRAPHS: THE EFFECTS OF REDUNDANT DIMENSIONS AND DIVIDED ATTENTION. S. Camille Peres and David M. Lane

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

The role of modality congruence in the presentation and recognition of taskirrelevant stimuli in dual task paradigms.

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Binding across space and time in visual working memory

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [76.97.68.97] On: 26 July 2013, At: 18:38 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Cognitive Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pecp21 Flexibility of working memory encoding in a sentence picture sound verification task Michael A. Nees a & Bruce N. Walker b a Department of Psychology, Lafayette College, Easton, PA, USA b School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, USA Published online: 26 Jul 2013. To cite this article: Journal of Cognitive Psychology (2013): Flexibility of working memory encoding in a sentence picture sound verification task, Journal of Cognitive Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2013.801846 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.801846 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2013 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.801846 Flexibility of working memory encoding in a sentencepicturesound verification task Michael A. Nees 1 and Bruce N. Walker 2 1 Department of Psychology, Lafayette College, Easton, PA, USA 2 School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, USA Downloaded by [76.97.68.97] at 18:38 26 July 2013 Dual-process accounts of working memory have suggested distinct encoding processes for verbal and visual information in working memory, but encoding for nonspeech sounds (e.g., tones) is not well understood. This experiment modified the sentencepicture verification task to include nonspeech sounds with a complete factorial examination of all possible stimulus pairings. Participants studied simple stimulipictures, sentences, or soundsand encoded the stimuli verbally, as visual images, or as auditory images. Participants then compared their encoded representations to verification stimuliagain pictures, sentences, or soundsin a two-choice reaction time task. With some caveats, the encoding strategy appeared to be as important or more important than the external format of the initial stimulus in determining the speed of verification decisions. Findings suggested that: (1) auditory imagery may be distinct from verbal and visuospatial processing in working memory; (2) visual perception but not visual imagery may automatically activate concurrent verbal codes; and (3) the effects of hearing a sound may linger for some time despite recoding in working memory. We discuss the role of auditory imagery in dual-process theories of working memory. Keywords: Auditory imagery; Working memory; Encoding; Visual imagery; Verbal encoding. Influential accounts of cognition (e.g., Baddeley & Logie, 1992; Paivio, Clark, Digdon, & Bons, 1989; Wickens, 2002) have emphasised a dichotomy between verbal (symbolic) and visuospatial (analogic) processing during the stage of active information processing that is commonly referred to as working memory. In these dual-process perspectives, words and visual images are handled by independent conceptual mechanisms that work in parallel. Comparatively little attention has been paid to the processing of auditory analogic representationsauditory images of nonverbal acoustic properties of stimuliin working memory. Current theoretical perspectives leave gaps in their explanations of how nonspeech sounds are remembered and rehearsed, although these activities are clearly pervasive in human cognition (Schellenberg & Trehub, 2003). This experiment used a verification task to examine auditory imagery, verbal encoding, and visuospatial imagery in working memory; similar tasks have been used extensively to examine the strategies and task dependencies associated with encoding. Clark and Chase (1972) presented simple sentences (e.g., star is above plus ) simultaneously with either a consistent or inconsistent picture. Participants performed a two-choice reaction time task in which they either confirmed or denied that the sentence described the picture. The researchers arrived at a model of the task that presupposed a common, immutable, verbal encoding strategy for both sentences and pictures, Correspondence should be addressed to Michael A. Nees, Department of Psychology, Lafayette College, Oechsle Hall, 350 Hamilton Street, Easton, PA 18042, USA. E-mail: neesm@lafayette.edu This experiment partially satisfied requirements for the first author s doctoral dissertation. Amy Gress collected data for this study. # 2013 Taylor & Francis

2 NEES AND WALKER whereby participants converted both stimuli to propositional representations to perform comparisons. Tversky (1975) demonstrated the use of a visual imagery strategy in sentencepicture verification with the successive presentation of the picture after a 5 s delay, and Glushko and Cooper (1978) also found evidence of a visual imagery strategy with successive presentation of the sentence followed by the picture. When participants were allowed to control the amount of time they studied the sentence before the picture stimulus appeared, Macleod, Hunt, and Mathews (1978) found a subgroup of participants who were spontaneously forming a mental image of the sentence to compare to the picture during verification. Mathews, Hunt, and MacLeod (1980) used the sentencepicture verification task to examine flexibility of encoding. Participants were explicitly instructed to use either verbal or visual imagery strategies to accomplish the task. Using verbal encoding, participants reaction times were extremely consistent with the predictions of a verbal encoding strategy. Mean data during imagery conformed nearly perfectly to the expected patterns of a visual strategy, whereas the mean fit from a spontaneous strategy condition somewhat resembled data from the verbal conditionexactly as would be expected with heterogeneous strategy use with a majority of participants spontaneously favouring a verbal approach. Related work has shown that when participants are not given a specific encoding strategy for the task, they seem to actively adapt their strategy based on task demands, such as the expected format of the verification stimulus (Kroll & Corrigan, 1981; Noordzij, van der Lubbe, & Postma, 2005). Despite the consistent pattern of results that have emerged from using sentencepicture tasks to study encoding, surprisingly few modifications to the original stimuli and format of the task have been attempted. The fundamental reasoning of stimulus verification paradigms could be adapted to study the encoding of a great variety of stimuli. Further, almost all research using the paradigm has involved unidirectional (sentencepicture) verification tasks; complete factorial manipulations (e.g., including picturesentence translations) have not been undertaken. We modified the sentencepicture verification procedure to examine encoding for sentences, pictures, and nonspeech sounds in a three (study stimulus format) by three (encoding strategy) by three (verification stimulus format) within-subjects design. Stimuli depicted simple, fictitious stock states (i.e., either price increased or price decreased ), and the encoding strategy was manipulated at three levels (verbal encoding, visual imagery, and auditory imagery) in different blocks of the study via instructions. Trials consisted of: (1) a study period for a stimulus (a sentence, a picture, or a sound), during which participants encoded the study stimulus as either a verbal representation, a visual image, or an auditory image; (2) a brief delay; and (3) the presentation of a verification stimulus (a sentence, a picture, or a sound) for which participants made a speeded comparison of their encoding to confirm or disconfirm a match with the original stimulus s depicted state for the stock. Regardless of the format of the study stimulus, verification times were predicted to be fastest when the external verification stimulus format matched the format encoded in working memory. Participants METHOD Participants (N 48, 17 females, M age 20.0 years, SD1.7) were recruited from undergraduate psychology courses and received course credit for their participation in the study. Apparatus A program written with Macromedia Director 2004 presented stimuli and collected data. Visual presentations were made on a 43.2 cm Dell LCD monitor. Sounds were presented with Sennheiser HD 202 headphones. Stimuli Stimuli were designed to convey information about the stimulus state (increasing or decreasing) as simply as possible. Picture stimuli were unlabelled line graphsone that showed the stock trend increasing and one that showed decreasing (see Figure 1). Sentence stimuli described the state of the stock with two words price increased or price decreased presented in approximately 40 point font at the centre of the screen. Sound stimuli used two discrete tonesc4 (262 Hz) and C5 (523 Hz). Each tone was 100 ms

SOUNDSENTENCEPICTURE VERIFICATION TASK 3 in length (with 10 ms onset and offset ramps) and synthesised with the MIDI piano instrument. The sound represented either an increasing stock price (C4 followed by C5) or a decreasing stock price (C5 followed by C4); higher pitch has been associated with higher position in space (Ben- Artzi & Marks, 1995). Downloaded by [76.97.68.97] at 18:38 26 July 2013 Procedure Participants were told to remember the state of the stock depicted in the first (study) stimulus for later comparison with the second (verification) stimulus. Participants were given 36 practice trials without a prescribed encoding strategy as an introduction to the task. Participants then experienced 72 trials of each encoding strategy; the order of the encoding strategies was counterbalanced across participants. For a verbatim example of encoding instructions, see the Appendix. Verbal (sentence) encoding. Participants were instructed to encode the study stimulus as a sentence that stated either price increased or price decreased. Visual imagery. Participants were instructed to encode the study stimulus as one of the two simple visual images shown in Figure 1. Auditory imagery. Participants were instructed to encode the study stimulus in pitch memory as one of the two-note sound stimuli, with pitch increasing if the stock price increased or pitch descending if the stock price decreased. Task. At the beginning of each encoding strategy block, participants confirmed that they understood the prescribed encoding strategy. The trial structure is depicted in Figure 2. Participants kept their left and right index fingers on the Z and? keys, respectively, and pressed either key to begin a trial. The keypress initiated the study stimulus (a sound, picture, or sentence), and Figure 1. The picture stimuli. Figure 2. Structure of a trial. participants encoded the study stimulus according to the assigned strategy. Participants pressed either key when they had encoded the stimulus in the prescribed format, and the verification stimulus appeared after a 3000 ms delay with a blank grey screen. Participants pressed the Z key for matches (when the verification stimulus matched the state of their encoding of the study stimulus) or the? key for mismatches. The mapping of keys to responses was counterbalanced across participants. Participants were instructed to verify as quickly as possible while following encoding instructions and avoiding errors, and the program provided feedback about verification reaction times following each trial. All possible stimulus combinations appeared twice in the 72 trials per block, and presentations within a block were randomly interleaved. The dependent variable was verification time, defined as the time from the onset of the verification stimulus until the participant pressed a response key to confirm or disconfirm its match with their encoding. The percentage of correct responses was a secondary measure of interest. Each trial (excluding practice) concluded with a brief strategy compliance check; participants self-reported the encoding strategy with which they had accomplished the task (for a description of this manipulation check in our previous work, see Nees & Walker, 2011, p. 312). Participants choices were limited to sound [auditory imagery] strategy, word [verbal] strategy, picture [visuospatial imagery] strategy, or not sure. To reduce the potential for demand characteristics to influence responses, the questionnaire instructed: Please be honest even if you used the wrong strategyit is very important that we know which strategy that you used. Participants also were encouraged by the experimenter to accurately report their strategy use, even if they had used the wrong strategy.

4 NEES AND WALKER RESULTS One participant failed to follow instructions and was excluded from all analyses. Data for 3.9% of trials were excluded because the participant indicated (s)he did not use the instructed strategy. Individual trials were also excluded from intracondition means if the verification response was incorrect or if the time was greater than 3 SDs from individual condition means; this led to the removal of an additional 7.3% of data across all trials. Data clean-up left empty data cells in the design for 9 additional participants; thus, the final sample size analysed was N 38. Other research using the sentencepicture verification task (e.g., Mathews et al., 1980) reported similar percentages of data elimination. Analysis of verification times A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on verification times with Greenhouse-Geisser corrections for sphericity violations. The assumption of sphericity was violated for the main effects of strategy and verification stimulus format, the interaction of strategy with study stimulus format, the interaction of strategy with verification stimulus format, the interaction of study stimulus format with verification stimulus format, and the 3-way interaction, Mauchly s Ws.16.78, ps B.05, s.73.82. The sphericity assumption held for the main effect of study stimulus, Mauchly s W.92, p.05. Pairwise comparisons for the significant main effect of strategy, F(1.65, 60.96) 3.76, pb.05, g 2 p ¼ :09, showed that verification times were fastest when participants used the verbal strategy (M942.15, SE49.17) as compared to the visual imagery (M1030.88, SE 48.12), p B.05, or the auditory imagery strategy (M1037.83, SE 54.77), p B.01. The visual and auditory imagery strategies were not significantly different, p.05. Pairwise comparisons for the main effect of study stimulus format, F(2, 74) 6.13, pb.01, g 2 p ¼ :14, showed that verification times were fastest when participants had studied sounds (M972.30, SE 40.39) as compared to sentences (M 1015.02, SE 48.39), p B.05, or pictures (M 1023.55, SE 49.94), p B.01. The difference between sentences and pictures was not significant, p.05. Pairwise comparisons for the significant main effect of verification stimulus format, F(1.55, 57.30) 30.39, pb.001, g 2 p ¼ :45, showed that verification times were faster when participants verified sentences (M950.27, SE 37.87), pb.001, or pictures (M906.38, SE 47.67), p B.001, as compared to sounds (M1154.22, SE 60.54). The difference between sentences and pictures was not significant, p.05. Main effects should be interpreted with caution due to the significant interactions. For the significant interaction of strategy with verification stimulus format, F(3.14, 116.11) 27.73, pb.001, g 2 p ¼ :43, simple effects analyses at each level of the verification stimulus format showed that when the verification stimulus was a sentence (three left bars of Figure 3), participants responded significantly faster using the verbal strategy (M835.74, SE 44.05) than the visual imagery (M 993.26, SE 45.53), pb.01, or auditory imagery (M 1021.80, SE 45.79), pb.001, strategies, which were not different from each other, p.05. When the verification stimulus was a picture (three middle bars of Figure 3), participants responded faster using the visual imagery strategy (M814.66, SE 39.43) than the auditory imagery strategy (M1039.17, SE 65.14), pb.001, but not the verbal strategy (M 865.31, SE 55.17), p.05. The verbal strategy also was significantly faster than the auditory imagery strategy, pb.001. When the verification stimulus was a sound (three right bars of Figure 3), participants responded faster using the auditory imagery strategy (M1052.52, SE 66.83) than the verbal (M1125.41, SE 62.84), p B.05, or visual imagery (M1284.74, SE 73.56), pb.001 strategies. The verbal strategy was significantly faster than visual imagery, p B.05. For the significant interaction of study stimulus format with verification stimulus format, F(3.03, 112.17) 8.27, pb.001, g 2 p ¼ :18, simple effects analyses at each level of the verification stimulus format showed that when the verification stimulus was either sentences or pictures (left and middle three bars of Figure 4, respectively), there were no significant differences as a function of study stimulus format, ps.088.821. When the verification stimulus was a sound (right three bars of Figure 4), however, participants were significantly faster to respond if they had studied a sound as compared to sentences, pb.01, or a picture, p B.001, and there was no difference between sentences and pictures, p.05. The interaction of strategy with study stimulus, FB1.00, and the three-way interaction, F(5.85, 216.60) 1.12, p.05, were nonsignificant.

SOUNDSENTENCEPICTURE VERIFICATION TASK 5 Figure 3. Verification time as a function of encoding strategy and verification stimulus format. Error bars represent SEM. Figure 4. Verification time as a function of study stimulus format and verification stimulus format. Error bars represent SEM. Analysis of percentage correct A secondary analysis of the percentage of trials correct within conditions was conducted with a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANO- VA) with Greenhouse-Geisser corrections for sphericity violations. The assumption of sphericity was violated for the main effect of verification stimulus format, and the interaction of study stimulus format with verification stimulus format, Mauchly s Ws.41.83, psb.05, s.67.86. The sphericity assumption held for the main effects of strategy and study stimulus format, the interaction of strategy with study stimulus format, the interaction of strategy with verification stimulus format, and the 3-way interaction, Mauchly s Ws.25.96, ps.05. Nonsignificant effects included the main effect of strategy, FB1.00, and the main effect of study stimulus format, F(2, 74) 1.23, p.05. Results showed a significant main effect of verification stimulus format, F(1.71, 63.30) 10.27, pb.001, g 2 p ¼ :22. Pairwise comparisons showed that participants made fewer mistakes when the verification stimulus was a sentence (M94.0%, SE.8), p B.05, or a picture (M 95.6%, SE.7), p B.001, as compared to sounds (M 92.0%, SE.12). Results also showed a significant

6 NEES AND WALKER Downloaded by [76.97.68.97] at 18:38 26 July 2013 Figure 5. Percentage of correct responses as a function of the study stimulus format and the verification stimulus format. Error bars represent SEM. interaction of study stimulus format with verification stimulus format, F(2.62, 97.03) 8.90, pb.001, g 2 p ¼ :19. Simple effects analyses at each level of the verification stimulus format showed that when the verification stimulus was a picture (middle three bars of Figure 5), participants made significantly fewer errors if they had studied a picture (M96.7%, SE.7) as compared to sentences (M94.7%, SE.9), pb.05. When the verification stimulus was a sound (right three bars of Figure 5), participants made significantly fewer mistakes if they had studied a sound (M95.7%, SE1.3) as compared to sentences (M90.2%, SE1.5), p B.001, orpictures(m90.0%, SE 1.3), pb.001. All other differences were nonsignificant, ps.09.83. The interaction of strategy with study stimulus format, the interaction of strategy with verification stimulus format, and the threeway interaction were all nonsignificant, Fs B1.00. DISCUSSION Verification times were predicted to be fastest for a given verification stimulus when it matched the participant s encoding strategy. This pattern was confirmed fully for sentence and sound verification stimuli and partially for picture verification stimuli, for which the visual and verbal encoding strategies were equally fast. Further, no congruency effects were expected between the format of the study stimulus and the format of the verification stimulus, because the study stimulus was to be recoded according to the encoding strategy. This pattern was confirmed for both sentence and picture verification stimuli, but facilitation was observed for sound verification stimuli if a sound had been studied. Results generally offered evidence in support of the hypothesis that, regardless of the external format of a to-be-remembered stimulus, participants could invoke a prescribed strategy to remember a stimulus in working memory. Sentences were verified fastest by participants using a verbal strategy. One of the hallmarks of verbal/ propositional encoding is the supplantation of sensory information (e.g., contained in the pictures and sounds) with the verbal code (see Tracy, Roesner, & Kovac, 1988), and the notion that verbal encoding drops sensory qualia of stimuli was generally supported by this result (though with a possible exception when a sound was studied, see below). The successful implementation of verbal encoding is perhaps not surprising, as verbally labelling stimuli is common in cognition (e.g., Colegate & Eriksen, 1970). When the verification stimulus was a picture, participants responded equally quickly using both visual imagery and verbal encoding. Noordzij et al. (2005) suggested that both the verbal and visual representations of visual percepts are automatically encoded in visual imagery strategies. The appearance of a picture verification stimulus possibly automatically activated an accompanying

verbal code. Visual imagery did not appear to carry a verbal code in the same way that a visual percept may have activated a verbal code, as participants using the visual imagery strategy were slower to verify sentences as compared to those using the verbal encoding strategy. When the verification stimulus was a sound, as predicted, participants responded fastest using the auditory imagery strategy. The overall pattern of results for both verification times and accuracy suggested that people were slower and less accurate at deciphering the basic stimulus state of nonspeech auditory stimuli as compared to sentences and pictures, perhaps because nonspeech sounds were an unfamiliar mode for this type of information. Participants response times and accuracy may have been affected by the specific pitch changes used here, or they may have incurred modality-switching costs (Spence, Nicholls, & Driver, 2001). Another possibility is that auditory imagery was less vivid and useful than visual imagery (Tracy et al., 1988). Interestingly, a congruency effect was observed when the verification stimulus was a sound; participants were at an advantage for both verification times and accuracy (collapsed across encoding strategies) when they had studied a sound as compared to a sentence or picture (Figures 4 and 5, right 3 bars). This finding may be attributable to a persevering form of auditory sensory memory. Researchers have varied widely in their estimates of the time course of auditory sensory memory (ranging from a few hundred milliseconds up to a few seconds, see Cowan, 1984), but this result suggested that the perceptual qualities of a sound may linger for several seconds following perception, even when the stimulus has been recoded deliberately into a verbal or visual image format. The current study offered some qualified evidence to suggest that sentences, pictures, and sounds can be flexibly encoded in working memory. Some evidence was provided for the suggestion (Noordzij et al., 2005) that verbal codes are automatically activated during visual perception, but such activation was not evident with visual imagery. Previous research (Paivio et al., 1989) has suggested that verbal and visuospatial connections may be bidirectional, but our results suggested an asymmetry whereby visual percepts activated accompanying verbal codes but visual imagery did not. Results suggested that the effects of a nonspeech auditory percept linger for some time even despite nonauditory encoding strategies. SOUNDSENTENCEPICTURE VERIFICATION TASK 7 In general, this study suggested that the mode of the perceptual stimulus is no more important, and may be less important, than the encoding strategy for remembering and rehearsal in working memory. Though Baddeley and Logie (1992) argued for the storage component of verbal working memory as the explanatory mechanism of auditory imagery, the implications of this hypothesis remain unresolved in the literature. For example, from this perspective the concurrent processing of speech and nonspeech sounds in working memory should interfere, yet data to this effect are equivocal (for a review, see Smith, Reisberg, & Wilson, 1992). Further, the flexibility of encoding and the consequences of recoding percepts in working memory (e.g., translating words to images and sounds and vice versa) are not well understood (but see Paivio et al., 1989). Research on the role of auditory imagery in cognition and the recoding of percepts in working memory could clarify gaps in existing theory and inform the best practices for presenting information in practical applications. Auditory imagery may have important differences with verbal and visuospatial processes in working memory. The extent to which auditory imagery does or does not require articulation (a verbal, domain-specific processing structure) remains unclear. Researchers have overwhelmingly embraced the premise of separate representational processing systems for verbal and visuospatial information (e.g., Baddeley & Logie, 1992; Paivio et al., 1989; Wickens, 2002). This experiment contributes to an emerging body of evidence that the dual-process dichotomy may omit a limited set of other plausible independent working memory processes such as auditory imagery, and also that existing theories may underestimate the flexibility of encoding processes by unnecessarily linking external stimulus modalities with determinate processing formats in working memory. Original manuscript received July 2012 Revised manuscript received April 2013 First published online July 2013 REFERENCES Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1992). Auditory Imagery and working memory. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), Auditory Imagery (pp. 179197). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ben-Artzi, E., & Marks, L. E. (1995). Visual-auditory interaction in speeded classification: Role of stimulus

8 NEES AND WALKER difference. Perception and Psychophysics, 57(8), 11511162. Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 472517. Colegate, R. L., & Eriksen, C. W. (1970). Implicit speech as an encoding mechanism in visual perception. American Journal of Psychology, 83(2), 208215. Cowan, N. (1984). On short and long auditory stores. Psychological Bulletin, 96(2), 341370. Glushko, R. J., & Cooper, L. A. (1978). Spatial comprehension and comparison processes in verification tasks. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 391421. Kroll, J. F., & Corrigan, A. (1981). Strategies in sentence-picture verification: The effect of an unexpected picture. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(5), 515531. MacLeod, C. M., Hunt, E. B., & Mathews, N. N. (1978). Individual differences in the verification of sentence-picture relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(5), 493507. Mathews, N. N., Hunt, E. B., & MacLeod, C. M. (1980). Strategy choice and strategy training in sentencepicture verification. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(5), 531548. Nees, M. A., & Walker, B. N. (2011). Mental scanning of sonifications reveals flexible encoding of nonspeech sounds and a universal per-item scanning cost. Acta Psychologica, 137, 309317. Noordzij, M. L., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Postma, A. (2005). Strategic and automatic components in the processing of linguistic spatial relations. Acta Psychologica, 119(1), 120. Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., Digdon, N., & Bons, T. (1989). Referential processing: Reciprocity and correlates of naming and imaging. Memory and Cognition, 17(2), 163174. Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (2003). Good pitch memory is widespread. Psychological Science, 14(3), 262266. Smith, J. D., Reisberg, D., & Wilson, M. (1992). Subvocalization and auditory imagery: Interactions between the inner ear and inner voice. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), Auditory imagery (pp. 95119). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Driver, J. (2001). The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(2), 330336. Tracy, R. J., Roesner, L. S., & Kovac, R. N. (1988). The effect of visual versus auditory Imagery on vividness and memory. Journal of Mental Imagery, 12(3 & 4), 145162. Tversky, B. (1975). Pictorial encoding of sentences in sentence-picture comparison. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(3), 405410. Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(2), 159177. APPENDIX: EXAMPLE ENCODING INSTRUCTIONS The instructions below are the verbatim encoding instructions for the auditory imagery strategy. The visual imagery and verbal strategy instructions followed parallel forms but substituted details relevant to those strategies, respectively. For this block, you must use the sound strategy. Regardless of whether Stimulus A is a sentence, picture, or sound, you must turn it into a sound in your mind. Use this sound for increasing price: [increasing price sound stimulus played], and this sound for decreasing price: [decreasing sound stimulus played]. Don t press Z or? to leave Stimulus A until you have a vivid sound to represent the stock price in your pitch memory. Keep rehearsing this sound in your mind (during the grey blank screen) until Stimulus B appears. You should imagine the sound playing through your headphones during this time. Summary of instructions for the sound strategy: (1) Turn Stimulus A into either the increasing or decreasing sound in your mind. Don t continue until you can hear the sound in your mind, even if the actual stimulus is a sentence or picture. (2) Press Z or? to continue. During the brief wait with the grey screen, you should be hearing the sound in your pitch memory. Imagine the sound playing in the headphones. (3) Decide if the sound in your mind matches the state of the stock (increasing or decreasing) for Stimulus B and make a response. Please get the experimenter now. The experimenter will help you start this block! [The experimenter reiterated the instructions to the participant and checked with the participant to confirm that the participant understood the instructions.] It is very important that you follow these exact instructions on every trial until further notice!