YOUNG PEOPLE, DRINKING HABITS, TRANSPORTATION AND PEER RELATIONS. A QUESTIONNAIRE STUDY

Similar documents
The role of attitudes in transportation studies

SOCIAL NORMS, ATTITUDES AND INTENTIONS CONCERNING DRINKING AND DRIVING. A CROSS-NATIONAL COMPARISON

Items pertaining to drunk driving

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

Evaluation of the. Learn2Live Road Safety Intervention for Young People. Plymouth Pavilions event. 25 th November 2011.

DRUG USE OF FRIENDS: A COMPARISON OF RESERVATION AND NON-RESERVATION INDIAN YOUTH

The Determination and Implication of Minimum Legal Drinking Age. MLDA, short for Minimum Legal Drinking Age, was set to twenty-one years old by

Outcome Report - Alcohol Wise

An Evaluation of the High Risk Offender Scheme

THE POTENTIAL IMPACTS OF LUAS ON TRAVEL BEHAVIOUR

Does habitual car use not lead to more resistance to change of travel mode?

Chapter 10 Section 1 Notes

Chapter 10. Alcohol Is a Drug

Composite Prevention Profile: City of Chicago, Illinois

Drink Driving in Finland Petri Jääskeläinen

A process model of voluntary travel behavior modification and effects of Travel Feedback Programs (TFPs)

Small glasses Big consequences!

How Long Does It Take a Person to Sober Up? Some Mathematics and Science of DUI

South Australian Alcohol and Other Drug Strategy

Other Substance Use Among Students in Grades 7 to 12 in the NBPSDHU Region

RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS ANALYSIS

Exploring the Utility of an Extended Theory of Planned Behavior Framework for School-Based Gambling Prevention Programs

Supply of Alcohol to Young People Aged Under 18 Years. June 2017

REVISED. Humboldt County 2007

The Association Between Risk-taking Behavior and Helmet Use Among Motorcyclist

Attitude = Belief + Evaluation. TRA/TPB and HBM. Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior. TRA: Constructs TRA/TPB

Alcoholism markers in their relation to prevention and rehabilitation

Driving Drunk, Driving High: A Comparison of Student Attitudes Towards Driving while Drunk Versus Driving while High on Cannabis

How to change young drivers attitudes and behaviour with respect to drivingwhile-intoxicated

ROAD SAFETY (OR LACK THEREOF) IN THE PUBLIC MIND

Alcohol-free on the road (Alcohol-vrij op weg)

Results from GPS in Serbia SMART questionnaire. Biljana Kilibarda Institute of Public Health of Serbia

Alcohol Impaired Driving Recruiting and Preparing Peer Leaders

Traditional Prevention Strategies and the Social Norms Approach

The Effectiveness of Pre-Driver Training

REVISED. Tulare County 2007

SELF-REPORTED ALCOHOL USE AND SEXUAL BEHAVIORS OF ADOLESCENTS'

REVISED. Stanislaus County 2007

Analyzing Determinants of Educational Methods in Environmental Education by using the Theory of Planned Behavior.

Drink Driving Fast Facts Draft

2012 FLORIDA YOUTH SUBSTANCE ABUSE SURVEY. Miami-Dade County

Marijuana Use and Perception Compared to Other Substances among High School Students in Denver, CO in 2013 & 2015 Healthy Kids Colorado Survey (HKCS)

The End of the Decline in Drink Driving in Britain?

Student Alcohol Use at The University of Montana NCHA Key Findings and Comparisons to National Reference Data

Alcohol Use and Related Behaviors

Get Me Home Socialising, drinking and safer car travel for young adults Executive Summary

Core Alcohol and Drug Survey - Long Form. Consortium Number = Institution Number = Number of Surveys =

National Data

Facts About Drinking. HHHH HH Partial/Cloze Dictations 33. Introduction

Illinois State University (Online)

Core Alcohol and Drug Survey - Long Form. Consortium Number = Institution Number = Number of Surveys = 6905

Core Alcohol and Drug Survey - Long Form. Consortium Number = Institution Number = Number of Surveys = 56937

Talk About Alcohol quiz How much do you know?

REVISED. Inyo County 2007

National Data

Level of Effectiveness

Directed Reading. Lesson: Understanding Teens and Alcohol WHY TEENS DRINK. Lesson: Alcohol and Your Body ALCOHOL IN YOUR BODY

ALCOHOL AND YOU Alcohol

Measuring Road Safety Culture in Relation to Speed Susan Cambridge and Tony Francis Francis and Cambridge Ltd, Christchurch

Key Points For Physicians To Review With Adolescents and Adults Who Have ADHD

University of North Carolina Chapel Hill (online)

Illinois State University (Online)

SAFE COMMUNITIES SURVEY RESULTS 2000

Culture, Perceptions and Social Norms. Dr. Jeff Linkenbach, National MOST Of Us Institute Montana State University

CROATIA. Recorded adult per capita consumption (age 15+) Lifetime abstainers

The Drinking Age and TrafficSafety

Drug and Alcohol Awareness

Teacher. Drinking Games. Information and in-class activity pack. SECTION 1: Introducing Key Concepts. How to use this resource: Page 1 of 4

Indicators of Alcohol and Other Drug Risk and Consequences for California Counties

Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version:

Do I Have a Drinking Problem?

Mendocino County 2010

Underage Drinking in Coconino County. Executive Summary

Youth Involvement In Traffic Accidents In Japan-New Trends

Grand Rapids Effects Revisited: Accidents, Alcohol and Risk

Cultural Differences and Traffic Safety.

OECD Paris, September 2008

HOW DO YOU USE ALCOHOL? DISCUSSION LEADER S OUTLINE. Good morning my name is. Today we will talk about recognizing alcohol use.

Drink driving in Denmark and available measures

Reactions of teenagers and parents to a zero alcohol tolerance law

Validity of scales measuring the psychosocial determinants of HIV/STD-related risk behavior in adolescents

Addendum: Multiple Regression Analysis (DRAFT 8/2/07)

Conflicting Response

Take The Pledge! Underage Alcohol Use. By James L. Holly, MD. Your Life Your Health. The Examiner. May 11, 2006

Intention to consent to living organ donation: an exploratory study. Christina Browne B.A. and Deirdre M. Desmond PhD

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Part I: Alcohol Metabolization Explore and Explain

Alcohol & Drug Abuse Prevention Team. Student Survey Report 2018

Reducing Alcohol-impaired Driving: School-based programs Summary Evidence Table

We admitted that we were powerless over alcohol that our lives had become unmanageable.

XYZ County Schools LifeSkills Training (LST) Program Student Survey Results for the School Year

STARTING A COMMUNITY CONVERSATION ON UNDERAGE DRINKING

We admitted that we were powerless over alcohol that our lives had become unmanageable. Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) (2001, p. 59)

Dear Advice Line, we were bored. My friend got 50me liquor and offered me a drink. I 5aid I couldn't becau5e I had a game that

English summary of Brå report 2013:3. Crime and problem behaviours among year-nine youth in Sweden

CHOICES PRE/POST-TEST

Facts About Alcohol. 2.1 million students between the ages of drove under the influence of alcohol last year

Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls

BEHAVIORS ASSOCIATED WITH ALCOHOL

Transcription:

YOUNG PEOPLE, DRINKING HABITS, TRANSPORTATION AND PEER RELATIONS. A QUESTIONNAIRE STUDY Lars Åberg and Mats Haglund Campus Borlänge, Dalarna University, Borlänge, Sweden and Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden INTRODUCTION Drunk driving is a serious problem in traffic. This is true even in Scandinavia where the number of drunk drivers in traffic is lower than in most other countries. In Sweden about a drunk driver is involved in about 25 % of all fatal traffic accidents. The risk of traffic accidents is higher for young people that for older drivers. There is ample evidence that a combination of young age and alcohol use is a strong risk factor in traffic. Also male drivers predominate the accident statistics with drunken drivers. This is also the situation in Sweden although the legal BAC is lower than in most other countries (0.02%) and the number of drunk drivers in traffic is comparatively low. Most of the discussion concerning this problem has dealt with drunk drivers. However, passengers riding with a drink drivers are also at risk (Everett et al, 2001) and as passengers they are in a position where they might have some influence on driver decisions. In Sweden attempts are being made to persuade young persons to intervene when they observe their friends that are driving after alcohol consumption. They can show their concern by avoiding riding with a drunk driver or by trying to stop him or her from driving. In the present study the experiences and intentions of young males and females concerning their interventions against drunken driving were investigated. Such situations can be expected to involve social interplay between individuals and especially between friends or family members. Social factors have also been suggested to provide a possible explanation for drunk driving behaviour (Åberg, 1993). The Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB (Ajzen, 1991)) is a theoretical approach to explain human decision making based on attitudes, social norms and perceived behavioural control. Especially this last factor is of interest as most people are aware that alcohol intake makes it difficult control decision-making and behaviour. Therefore in the present survey an attempt was made to apply the TPB on young peoples self reported behaviour and intentions concerning drunken driving problems in transportation. A questionnaire was designed aiming at explaining behaviours like to ride with a drunken driver or to try and stop an intoxicated driver from driving. In the construction of items for the questionnaire the TPB was used as a frame of reference.

Attitude Social norm Intention Behaviour Perceived Behaviour control Theory of Planned Behaviour (after Ajzen, 1991) 2. METHOD 2.1 Questionnaire In the questionnaire questions were asked about four different kinds of behaviour of which two, to prevent a drunk driver to drive and to refuse a ride with a drunk driver, were focused in the present paper. Behaviours, evaluations of behaviour, and social factors of importance for young peoples decisions were obtained from focus group investigations of altogether 100 subjects (Englund, Nyberg, and Thiseus, 1996). For each behaviour (self reported) the choice and design of questions were based on the TPB as a frame of reference. Questions were also asked about drinking habits, knowledge about the drunken driving problem, driving experience, etc. 2.2 Subjects A questionnaire about drinking and driving was mailed to a random sample of 2300 young people. The sample was stratified on age and gender and all subjects came from Uppsala County, Sweden. After two reminders 1249 questionnaires were returned, 54.3%. The subjects aged between 15 and 24 years with 47.4% males and 52.6% females. Number of subjects with a driving licence was 582 (286 males and 296 females). The age and gender distribution of the subjects are described in figure 2. There are ten different age groups with a little more than 100 subjects in each group. Between 75 % and 80 % of the subjects had a drivers licence for cars.

Subjects % 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 Age Males Females Figure 2. Distribution of sex and ages among subjects 2.3 Structural equation modelling In the analysis of data LISREL-analysis (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993) was performed separately for each behaviour. As most items of the questionnaires produce ordinal scale data before the analysis the variables were treated with data program (PRELIS), a procedure that corrected data for discontinuity and skewness in distributions (using polyserial correlations). In the LISREL analysis the compatibility between a theoretical model and empirical data is investigated. The basic principles of the analysis are presented in Appendix. 3. RESULTS 3.1 Drinking habits The habits of drinking alcohol increased from a mean of three to four times a year at 15 to between one to two times per month after 18 years of age. There was no difference due to gender. A similar pattern, although on not so frequent, was obtained when the item concerned how often they drunk so much that they felt very drunk. There was a difference after 17 years between sexes as males were drunk more often. The subjects though they could drink between two and three cans of strong beer before they becomes slightly intoxicated and between 6 and 7 cans before they becomes really drunk. It was believed that a male person weighing 70 Kg could drink one to two cans of strong beer, if he is going to drive an hour later, without having a BAC above 0.02 % (the lower BAC limit in Sweden). No differences due to age or gender. The same person was believed to be able to drink 4-5 cans of strong beer before he passes the upper limit (BAC = 0.10 %). Again there were no differences due to age or gender. 3.2 General attitude towards drunk driving The subjects were asked a question about their general attitudes towards drinking one hour before driving. After 2 or 5 cans of strong beer (beer with 5-7% alcohol). The result is presented in Figure 3.

100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 2 cans of beer 5 cans of beer age Figure 3. Percentage of subjects that think that drinking beer before driving is quite wrong. For different age groups and for 2 or 5 cans, respectively. Almost all subjects consider it wrong to drive after 5 cans of beer and 60-80 % that it is wrong after 2 cans. The younger subjects were somewhat more permissive. 3.3 To prevent a drunk driver from driving Several questions were asked concerning the subjects intentions and experiences of situations where they prevented someone to drive after drinking. Except for the youngest subjects - that had very little experience - between 20-40 % reported experience from persuading a driver to withhold from driving and10-20% that they had taken the keys from the driver. When asked if the would prevent a friend that had drunk 2 or 5 cans of beer from driving the subjects mean answers are presented in Figure 4.

100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 after 2 cans of beer after 5 cans o beer Figure 4. Percentage of subjects with clear intentions to prevent a drinking friend from driving. Most subject report that they think they would stop a friend from driving after having consumed 5 cans of beer. About 75 % would do so if the friend had drunk 2 cans of beer. Structural modelling was used to investigate if the TPB satisfactory could explain the correlations obtained between different variables. The result is depicted in figure 5 Attitude -.11 Age -.15 -.25 Social norm.10.27 Intetion to Prevent R²=.52.30 Gender.50.19.31 Perc. beh. control RMSEA=.053 (.044 -.063), Fair fit

Figure 5 Schematic picture of factors influencing intentions to prevent a friend from drunk driving The result presented in Figure 5 shows that the TPB provides a satisfactory explanation for intention to prevent a friend from driving after alcohol consumption. If both direct and indirect effects are considered the social norm is most important (.42), then there is gender (.36), PBC (.31), attitude (-.25), and age (.07) is least important. Drinking habits did not improve the model. 3.4 To ride with a drunk driver Asked if they during the previous three years had had a ride with a drunk driver about 50 % of the subjects admitted that. The distribution of subjects over age and gender is presented in Figure 6. 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 males females Figure 6. Experience of a ride with a drunk driver (percentages)results from application of the TPB-modell on the riding data are shown in Figure 7. Drinking habits Attitude.65 -.20.35.23.16.18 Social norm Intention.18 R²=.58.40 Behaviour R²=.43

-.54 -.31 PBC RMSEA=.052 (.045 -.059) Fair fit. Figure 7. Structural picture of factors affecting riding with a drunk driver If only the direct effects of the TPB variables are considered a model with attitude and PBC can predict intention and behaviour in accordance with the TPB. Also drinking habits have direct effects on both intention and behaviour. Social norm does not fit into the model unless it is assumed to affect both attitude and PBC. However, in that case social norm has the strongest total effect on behaviour (.42), stronger intention (.40), attitude (.37), Drinking habits (.37) and PBC (-.20). Even to explain intention social norm has a strong influence (.61), before PBC (-.38), attitude (.35) and drinking habits (.16). The model including the Social norm had the best fit of the models tested with a RMSEA 0.052, indicating a fair fit and explaining 43 % of self reported behaviour and 58 % of the variation of intentions for the future. 4. DISCUSSION The results of the present study are based on self-reported data from a sample with a quite low return rate. Thus it is possible that the subjects have a more negative view on drunk driving than young people in general. Such a bias will lead to reduced variation in TPB variables and make the model fit less likely. Still the TPB model fits the present data quite well. Also, a model that allows for indirect effects shows that the normative factor might be the most important influence on intentions and behaviours. A model that only allows for direct effects (like ordinary analysis of regression) will neglect influences from the social environment. One interpretation of the results supports the idea in Sweden that efforts should be directed at young people, especially young females, that not themselves are likely to be driving intoxicated but that have friends that are more Agat risk. By influencing and supporting the good people it might be possible to prevent other persons from committing dangerous violations in traffic. 5. REFERENCES Berg, L (1993) Drinking and driving: intentions, attitudes and social norms of Swedish male drivers. Accident analysis and prevention. 25, 289-296. Ajzen, I. (1991) The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 50, 179-211. Englund, A., Nyberg, A., and Thiseus, J. (1996). Ungdomars förhållningssätt till trafik och alkohol en gruppintervjustudie. Department of Education, Uppsala University. Evans, L. (1991). Traffic safety and the driver. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Everett, S.A, Shults, R.A., Barrios, L.C., Sacks, J.S., Lowry, R., Oeltmann, J. (2001). Trends and subgroup differences in transportation-related injury risks and safety

behaviors among high school students, 1991-1997. Journal of adolescent health, 28, 228-234. Jöreskog, K.G., and Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8 : structural equation modelling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago, USA: IL Scientific Software International APPENDIX Using LISREL technique to test a theoretical model. The intention of most researchers is to establish causal explanations between variables of interest. An experiment is necessary if the purpose is to be able to say that the state one variable is an effect of the state in another variable. If the analysis is based on questionnaire data solely the possibility to obtain causal explanations is not so easy or rather non-existing. It is possible though to state that data are in accordance with a theoretical model even if such a statement is not a proof of causality. Structural equation modelling (SEM) of which LISREL is one example provides a mean to discuss causal relations and suitability of theoretical models based on, for example, questionnaire data. Though SEM initially might be rather complicated to perform the basic ideas are not so difficult. Below an attempt is made to explain the use of LISREL to modify theoretical models. Assume the following chain of causal relations (theoretical model): A B C Variable A is assumed to affect B that in turn is assumed to affect C. In this way there is a direct effect C from B and an indirect effect on C from A. To test if this theoretical model is in according with data the three variables are measured and inter-correlated, resulting, for example, in the following matrix: A B C A -.5.4 B -.6 If the theoretical model (A-B-C) is true the relationships could be described as:.5.6 A B C Then the corresponding correlation matrix would be A B C A -.5.3 (the indirect effect of A o B -.6 on C is.5*.6=.3)

Thus the theoretical matrix deviates from the empirical. Correlation AC is.3 instead of.4. However if a direct effect is allowed from A to C, e.g. the following model is suggested:.5 A.13 B.53 C This model results in a correlation matrix that is identical with the original matrix. Thus according to the statistical analysis a new theoretical model including a direct path from A to C is suggested. This is the basic principle behind the analysis presented in the present paper. The questionnaire was based on the TPB and after the subjects responses, correlation calculations and LISREL analysis a revised TPB model is suggested. However, no model can be modified based on data only. In addition there is need for theoretically sound arguments that supports the revised theory. Thus in the simple example used here, a direct path from A to C would improve the model fit but there should also some arguments why this path should be acceptable.