Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Similar documents
Reinforcing Effects of Social Stimuli on Responding After. Implementation of A Pairing Procedure. A Thesis Presented. Amanda J.

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

A Comparison of Methods for Marking Correct Responses. in Matching to Sample. A Thesis Presented. Kara Schmidt

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Increasing Independent Task Engagement in an Individual. with Autism. A thesis presented. Jessica Paredes

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

Establishing and Testing Conditioned Reinforcers Comparison of the Pairing Vs. Discriminative Stimulus Procedures with Individuals with Disabilities

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

The Impact of Formatting Skill Acquisition Procedures and Treatment Integrity. A Thesis Presented. Kara E. Lamb

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

A Comparison of Functional Analysis Results When Conducted in Contrived and. Home Settings. A Thesis Presented. Dana DeIngenis

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn

Acquisition when reinforcement is delayed. A Thesis Presented. Stefanie Fillers. In partial requirements. For the degree of.

Behavioral Development Bulletin. Mary E. McDonald 1, Sharon A. Reeve 2, and Erin J. Sparacio 3. Volume 19 Number 1 March 2014

A Comparison of Two Interventions to Treat Food Selectivity. A Thesis Presented. Shannon C. Garvey

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

A comparison of response-contingent and noncontingent pairing in the conditioning of a reinforcer

The Effects of Varying Response Requirements on. Reinforcer Potency. A Thesis Presented. Deborah L. Edelman

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S.

topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION JUNE 2012 ISSUE 12

A COMPARISON OF TWO PAIRING PROCEDURES TO ESTABLISH PRAISE AS A REINFORCER CLAUDIA L. DOZIER BRIAN A. IWATA APRIL S. WORSDELL DAVID M.

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director

Overview. Environmental Causes of Problem Behaviours Exhibited by Children and Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Assessment

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders. Fall 2017

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY

USING PROGRESSIVE RATIO SCHEDULES TO EVALUATE EDIBLE, LEISURE, AND TOKEN REINFORCEMENT. Danielle M. Russell, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

Comparison of Reinforcement Alone and in Combination with Redirection for. Treating Automatically-Reinforced Motor Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented

Behavioral Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Joseph Michael Lambert. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

AN OPERANT APPROACH TO TEACHING JOINT ATTENTION SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

Using Pivotal Response Training with Peers in Special Education to Facilitate Play in Two Children with Autism

STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESSFUL INSTRUCTION

Using Antecedent Based Intervention & Reinforcement: From an Elementary Perspective

POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AS TREATMENT FOR PROBLEM BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT STEVEN W. PAYNE AND CLAUDIA L.

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

Evaluation of DRO Schedules to Reduce Disruptive Behavior in a Preschool Classroom

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

CHAPTER 15 SKINNER'S OPERANT ANALYSIS 4/18/2008. Operant Conditioning

An Evaluation of the Value and Conditioning of Choice as a Reinforcer for Typically. Developing Children. Julie Ann Ackerlund Brandt

I give special thanks to Ed Sbardellati, who has been a pivotal support, during this study, and throughout my career as a behavior analyst.

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness

Abstract. Authors. Shauna Whiteford, Ksusha Blacklock, Adrienne Perry. Correspondence. Keywords

e-newsletter ABA Literature Summary topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION ...

Self-Management and Functional Analysis

Simple Strategies for Behavior Change

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Practical Strategies to Address Challenging Behavior. Bridget A. Taylor, Psy.D., BCBA-D, Alpine Learning Group

Establishing Conditioned Reinforcers in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

TREATMENT OF COVERT SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PRADER-WILLI SYNDROME

Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

A Thesis Presented. Delna Bharucha. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology. In partial fulfillment of the requirements

Using TAG to Increase Play Skills

Teaching Two Children Diagnosed with Autism to Tolerate Physical Contact

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

RESPONSE PATTERNS IN FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS. Christine M. Gibson, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

Identifying & Enhancing the Effectiveness of Positive Reinforcement

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC TASKS: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF IDIOSYNCRATIC ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS JENNIFER MCCOMAS

4/4/2017. Intervention Strategies for the Reduction of Repetitive Behavior in Persons with ASD

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF SOCIAL AVOIDANCE JILL M. HARPER

VICARIOUS REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURES: AN ANALYSIS OF STIMULUS CONTROL AND POTENTIAL SIDE EFFECTS DANIELLE L. GUREGHIAN

An Overview of Naturalistic ABA Strategies for Young Children with Autism

A Non-Medication Based Treatment Proposal For Individuals Diagnosed With A Tic Disorder and Intellectual Disabilities

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

Helping the Student With ADHD in the Classroom

The Combined Effects of Picture Activity Schedules and Extinction Plus Differential Reinforcement on Problem Behavior During Transitions

MAKING IT WORK AS AN ABA THERAPIST

ABSTRACT ASSESSMENT-BASED INTERVENTIONS

What is Behavior? Course Objectives. What is Behavior? 11/2/13. (And how do we manage it?)

ASD Strategies in Action

TEACHING SIMPLE AUDITORY DISCRIMINATIONS TO STUDENTS WITH AUTISM. Kristine L. Marino, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

Steps for Implementation: Token Economy Programs

REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA, AND RACHEL H.

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

The Use of a Visual Aid to Facilitate Transitional Periods for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Effects of Differential Reinforcement of Short Latencies on Response Latency, Task Completion, and Accuracy of an Adolescent with Autism

Transcription:

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers A Thesis Presented by Hilary A. Gibson The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in the field of Applied Behavior Analysis Northeastern University Boston, MA May 2009

NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY Bouvé College of Health Sciences Graduate School Thesis Title: Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers Author: Hilary A. Gibson Department: Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology Approved for Thesis Requirements of Master of Science Degree D. Daniel Gould, Ph.D., BCBA Date Karen E. Gould, Ph.D., BCBA Date Pamela M. Olsen, MSEd, BCBA Date

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers by Hilary A. Gibson B.A., Rollins College Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Applied Behavior Analysis in the Bouvé College of Health Sciences Graduate School of Northeastern University, May 2009

Acknowledgements I would like to express my gratitude to Dr. Daniel Gould, who served as advisor for my thesis. He was supportive throughout all stages of the study, including initial planning, obtaining materials, data collection, editing, and presenting the thesis. I would also like to thank the other members of my thesis committee, Dr. Karen Gould and Pamela Olsen, for their assistance with my project. Finally, I would like to thank my colleagues who observed sessions and collected interobserver agreement data.

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers Table of Contents ABSTRACT 4 INTRODUCTION 5 METHOD 9 Participant 9 Setting and Materials 9 Variables and Definitions 10 Measurement and IOA 10 Procedure 11 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 14 REFERENCES 20 FIGURE CAPTIONS 21 FIGURES 22

Social Consequences 4 Abstract A reinforcer assessment of social consequences was conducted to determine the pre-pairing reinforcing value of two social stimuli, praise and back pats. A pairing procedure and an S D procedure were evaluated to determine which, if either, would effectively condition social consequences as effective reinforcers. One child with autism participated in the study. It was determined that prior to conditioning neither social stimulus functioned as a reinforcer. Social consequences were not effectively conditioned as reinforcers using the pairing procedure, however praise and back pats may have been conditioned as weak reinforcers using the S D procedure.

Social Consequences 5 Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers Positive reinforcement is the backbone of Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA). The use of positive reinforcement has been extensively researched and presented in the ABA literature. Results of these studies have supported a conclusion that in order for a response to be increased effectively, a stimulus with potent reinforcing properties must be provided contingent on that response. Positive reinforcement is used in clinical settings to increase a wide variety of desirable behavior and to decrease undesirable behavior. The reinforcing effectiveness of stimuli is often idiosyncratic and must therefore be assessed accurately and frequently in order for effective treatment to be administered (Kelleher & Gollub, 1962). While numerous studies have highlighted the utility of primary, or unconditioned, positive reinforcement in the applied settings, problems can arise with its use. These problems include a delay between the time a target response occurs and the delivery of a primary reinforcer, and satiation with a particular primary reinforcer (Kadzin & Bootzin, 1972). While primary reinforcement can be used to effectively and efficiently teach and maintain performance of a wide range of skills, situations exist in which it is beneficial to utilize conditioned reinforcement. Kazdin and Bootzin (1972) present the benefits associated with the use of conditioned reinforcement in the applied setting. Conditioned reinforcers can help reduce the time that elapses between when a response occurs and when a reinforcer is delivered, that is, they allow a response to be reinforced at any time. Performance on a task can be maintained for long periods of time and components of a sequence or chain do not need to be interrupted to deliver conditioned reinforcers. Further, generalized conditioned reinforcers may be less sensitive to the effects of satiation than primary reinforcers.

Social Consequences 6 Social consequences can function as conditioned reinforcers. Praise statements such as That s right or Good can effectively reinforce behavior in typically developing individuals and in many individuals with disabilities. The praise statements gain their reinforcing properties through their relation to other reinforcers (Skinner, 1953). Parents reinforce and punish their children s behavior from a young age and social stimuli such as praise statements, smiles, and frowns come to shape children s behavior. Individuals with autism, however, often do not respond to social consequences as a typically developing individual would (Lovaas, 1966). It is important to identify preferred social stimuli of individuals with autism and additionally to determine whether these stimuli function as reinforcers (Smaby, MacDonald, Ahearn, & Dube, 2007). Smaby et al. (2007) evaluated a method for assessing preference for social consequences of individuals with developmental disabilities by determining the levels at which various social stimuli supported responding. The assessment yielded differentiated results from which a preferred social consequence could be determined for each participant. The preferred social consequence was the one that increased rates of responding the most over baseline levels. Smaby et al. effectively evaluated preference of individuals with autism for three different social consequences and also evaluated their effectiveness as reinforcers by comparing responding maintained by each social consequence for each participant to responding during an extinction condition. Although Smaby et al. assessed the effectiveness of social consequences as conditioned reinforcers, they did not examine methods for establishing these reinforcers. The establishment of conditioned reinforcers in applied settings is a topic that has not been sufficiently researched. Williams (1994) describes some factors affecting the establishment of stimuli as conditioned reinforcers and the persistence of conditioned reinforcement effects. These

Social Consequences 7 variables include the schedule of pairing of the stimulus with the primary reinforcer and the percentage of times the stimulus was followed by reinforcement. Previous research has determined that the effectiveness of a conditioned reinforcer is dependent on the number of pairings of that stimulus with back-up reinforcers (Kelleher & Gollub, 1962). The number of pairing trials necessary and the most effective pairing methods, however, are not well understood. In one of the few studies of social stimuli as conditioned reinforcers, Dozier, Iwata, Thomason, and Wilson (2008) investigated the establishment of verbal praise as a conditioned reinforcer. Three procedures were evaluated: a new response procedure, an established response procedure, and a procedure that included the established response procedure with schedule thinning. In the new response procedure, a neutral stimulus was paired with a primary reinforcer. The previously neutral stimulus was then delivered contingent on a new free operant response and the subsequent rate of responding was measured to determine whether the stimulus had acquired reinforcing properties. In the established response procedure, the neutral stimulus was delivered, paired with a primary reinforcer, contingent on a response. The neutral stimulus alone was subsequently delivered contingent on a free operant response to determine if the stimulus had been conditioned as a reinforcer. The third procedure investigated was similar to a brief stimulus presentation procedure. In such a procedure a brief stimulus is occasionally paired with a primary reinforcer to determine whether responding can be maintained in components of second-order schedules of reinforcement (Kelleher, 1966). It was determined that the new response procedure was ineffective for all four participants. Praise was effectively conditioned for four of eleven participants using the established response procedure. The established response procedure with food-schedule thinning effectively conditioned praise as a reinforcer for

Social Consequences 8 all seven participants, however it was noted that thin food schedules yoked to the schedule of food delivery in the established response condition also maintained responding during a reinforcer assessment for five of five participants assessed (Dozier et al.). Although the Dozier et al. study contains promising results, the study has not been published and therefore many procedural details are lacking. Additional research has examined the establishment of to-be-conditioned neutral stimuli as discriminative stimuli (S D s). Although discriminative stimuli can function as reinforcers, the literature suggests that it is not necessary to establish a stimulus as an S D in order for it to be conditioned as a reinforcer (Kelleher & Gollub, 1962). Holth (personal communication, September 2008) investigated one procedure, referred to as the S D procedure, for establishing social stimuli as conditioned reinforcers. First, a neutral social stimulus was established as an S D for a specific response. This response then produced the primary reinforcer. The conditioned reinforcer value of the social stimulus was then tested by delivering the stimulus as a consequence contingent on an arbitrary response. The results suggested that the initially neutral social stimulus had been established as a conditioned reinforcer. Lovaas et al. (1966) utilized a similar S D procedure and reported that while a traditional pairing procedure (similar to the Dozier et al., 2007, new response procedure) did not effectively condition social stimuli as reinforcers, social stimuli were effectively conditioned as reinforcers when they were established as S D s for responses that produced food. Applied research has demonstrated the importance of positive reinforcement in increasing target behaviors, managing challenging behaviors of individuals with developmental disabilities, and establishing new behaviors. Research has also investigated conditioned reinforcement and its application for treatment of individuals with developmental disabilities. The effective and

Social Consequences 9 efficient establishment of conditioned reinforcers, including social reinforcers, is an ongoing area of interest in applied research because of the utility of these stimuli in applied settings. The purpose of the current study was to determine the pre-pairing reinforcing value of social consequences and to evaluate a pairing procedure and an S D procedure to determine which, if either, would effectively condition social consequences as reinforcers. Method Participant Mary was a 4-year-old girl diagnosed with autism. She was enrolled in an intensive early intervention preschool program for children with autism. She had previous experience with conditioned reinforcement, using tokens during academic programming. The tokens had previously been paired with edibles and the edibles were gradually faded out until the participant earned 10 tokens and then traded these tokens in for a toy or edible. Mary had deficits in social skills and often needed prompting to participate in group activities. Her teachers reported that she would likely be integrated into a typical classroom in the next year. She was selected to participate in the present study because effective naturalistic reinforcers such as praise would be much easier to deliver and less stigmatizing when delivered in a public school classroom rather than other stimuli (e.g., edibles, tokens, etc). Setting and Materials All sessions were conducted in either the student s individual classroom area or in an assessment room. Each area contained a desk and two chairs. A timer, a stopwatch, a video camera, and a counter were all used in the study. Other materials included a variety of edible

Social Consequences 10 stimuli presented in the preference assessment and the high-preference edible item selected in this assessment (chips). The same tokens and token board used in the participant s academic programming were used in the token reinforcer assessment. Dependent Variable and Response Definition The dependent variable during the preference assessment was a selection response. Selection was defined as picking up an item and putting it in the mouth. The dependent variable during all reinforcer assessments was hand raising. Hand raising was defined as lifting her arm so that her hand and her elbow were above shoulder height. In order for the next response to be scored she must first lower her hand and elbow below shoulder height. If she raised both hands at the same time, one response was scored. The social consequences investigated were praise and back pats. Praise was delivered by the experimenter in a normal conversational tone. Three phrases were alternated: Good work, Nice job, and That s great. Back pats were delivered by the experimenter by lightly tapping the participant behind the shoulder with an open hand two times. Measurement Method and IOA The experimenter or another trained therapist served as the primary observer for each session. During preference assessment sessions, the experimenters collected selection data for each trial. The data sheet included which stimuli were presented, the positioning of the stimuli, and which stimulus was selected. Percent selection was determined for each stimulus by dividing the number of times the item was selected by the total number of times it was presented and

Social Consequences 11 multiplying this number by 100. A second observer collected reliability data during 33.3% of sessions. Interobserver agreement (IOA) for selection was 100%. During reinforcement sessions, experimenters tallied responses or used a counter hidden from the view of the participant to determine the frequency of responses. Response rate was calculated by dividing the number of responses in the session by the duration of the session. Interobserver agreement data were collected during at least 33% of reinforcer assessment sessions in all phases of the study. A trained second observer either collected data during the sessions or watched the video tape after the session and scored responses using the same criteria as the primary observer. Agreement was calculated for each session with a second observer by dividing the smaller count by the larger count and multiplying by 100. The mean IOA score for all reinforcer assessment sessions was 98.6% agreement. Procedure Phase 1. A preference assessment was conducted to establish a highly-preferred edible item. A 16-item Paired Stimulus preference assessment included only edible items and closely followed the procedures outlined by Fisher et al. (1992). The 16 stimuli were presented within arm s reach of the participant in pairs. Each stimulus was paired with every other stimulus and delivered in a quasi-random order. The stimuli were available for 10 s. Selection was recorded when the participant touched one of the items with her hand. Attempts to select both items were blocked. The participant was allowed to immediately consume the selected edible and the other stimulus was removed from the table. If no selection was made in 10 s, both items were removed and no response was scored.

Social Consequences 12 Phase 2a. A reinforcer assessment was conducted to determine whether the highpreference item (chip) chosen from the preference assessment would function as a reinforcer. Hand raising was the free-operant response used in the assessment. The two conditions were FR1 and Extinction. During the FR1 condition, the edible selected in the preference assessment (chips) was delivered contingent upon every response. The experimenter delivered the chip into the participant s mouth. During the extinction condition, there were no programmed consequences. Prior to the sessions, the experimenter conducted two demonstration trials. The experimenter said Raise hand. Contingent on hand raising, the experimenter delivered either a chip during the FR1 condition or nothing during the extinction condition. After these two demonstration trials the experimenter delivered the following instructions to the participant, You can raise your hand as many times as you want and you will earn chips. During the extinction condition, the experimenter instructed the participant, You can raise your hand as many times as you want but you will not earn anything. Sessions lasted for 3 min. FR1 and extinction sessions were alternated and sessions were run until there was stable responding or differential responding in the FR1 and Extinction conditions. Phase 2b. A reinforcer assessment was conducted to determine whether social consequences functioned as reinforcers prior to conditioning. The conditions in this assessment were verbal praise, back pats, extinction, and high-preference edible. These conditions were alternated in a multielement design. Edible and extinction sessions were identical to Phase 2. During the verbal praise condition, the experimenter delivered the following instructions to the participant, You can raise your hand as many times as you want and you will earn nice job, good work, or that s great. One of the utterances was delivered following each response during the two demonstration trials. During the back pats condition, the experimenter delivered

Social Consequences 13 the following instructions to the participant, You can raise your hand as many times as you want and you will earn (experimenter patted participant s back two times). Two back pats were delivered following each response during the two demonstration trials. The praise, back pats, and extinction conditions were alternated in quasi-random order. Sessions were run until stable responding was achieved. The high-preference edible condition was re-introduced at the end of the phase to show differential responding between conditions and to demonstrate that the social consequences did not function as reinforcers, as compared to an edible reinforcer, prior to conditioning. Phase 3. Pairing was conducted to try to establish the neutral social stimuli (NS) as conditioned reinforcers. Procedures were similar to the new response procedure (Dozier et al., 2007). A praise statement was delivered and immediately followed by a piece of chip. There were 10 pairings per session. The three praise statements, Good job, Nice work, and That s great were alternated. A similar procedure was used with back pats. Two pats were delivered by the experimenter and followed immediately by a piece of chip. There were 10 pairings per session. Four pairing sessions were carried out with each NS (40 pairings of each) before moving on to Phase 4. Phase 4. Probe sessions were conducted to determine the reinforcing value of the previously NS. The sessions were identical to Phase 2b except the conditions were presented in the ratio of 3 praise as consequence, 3 back pats as consequence, 1 extinction, and 1 FR1 edible. Phase 3 and Phase 4 were alternated until stable responding was observed in Phase 4 sessions or until the response rates in Phase 4 sessions matched those of Phase 2a.

Social Consequences 14 Phase 5. Another baseline reinforcer assessment of the social consequences was conducted prior to the second conditioning procedure. The procedure was identical to Phase 2b. The conditions praise, back pats, extinction, and high-preference edible were alternated in quasirandom order until stable responding was observed. Phase 6. An S D procedure was used to establish the NS as conditioned reinforcers. Ten pieces of chip were placed on a plate. The plate was located on the table directly in front of the participant and within arm s reach. On each of 10 trials, the experimenter delivered the S D (one of the praise statements) and the participant took one chip from the plate and consumed it. This procedure was repeated until all chips were consumed. The procedure was carried out similarly with back pats, except the S D was two back pats. Sessions of praise and back pat pairings were alternated. There were four praise pairing sessions and four back pat sessions, or 40 total pairings of each, before moving on to Phase 7. Phase 7. Probe sessions were conducted to determine the reinforcing value of the previously NS. The sessions were identical to Phase 4. Phase 8. A reinforcer assessment was conducted to determine whether the tokens that the participant used during daily programming functioned as conditioned reinforcers. Sessions were the same as other reinforcer assessment sessions except that contingent on hand raising the experimenter put a token on the token board. When 10 tokens had been accumulated on the board, the participant traded the tokens in by handing the board to the experimenter. The experimenter delivered a chip to the participant in exchange for the board. The timer was stopped from the time the participant handed the board over until the time the participant consumed the chip.

Social Consequences 15 Results and Discussion Results for the preference assessment are shown in Figure 1. Chips were selected on 86.7% of presentations. Chips were used as the high-preference edible stimulus in subsequent phases because of their availability in the classroom. The results of Phase 2a and Phase 2b are depicted in Figure 2. Response rates are significantly and differentially higher than baseline when hand raising resulted in access to the high-preference edible stimulus (chips). Mean responding across FR1 sessions was 8.7 responses per minute (range, 4.3 to 12), and no responding occurred during Extinction sessions. Therefore, chips were validated as a reinforcer for the participant. In Phase 2b, variable low rates of responding were observed in all conditions (praise, pats, and extinction). Rates of responding decreased and remained stable at zero after session 46 for extinction and praise, and after session 56 for back pats. When chips were delivered contingent on responding at the end of Phase 2b responding increased to a mean of 16.3 (range, 15.7 to 17.3) and differentiation in response rate was evident among conditions. Low- or zero rates of responding continued when praise or back pats were delivered contingent on responding. Thus, in the absence of explicit establishment of praise and back pats as conditioned reinforcers, neither social consequence functioned as a reinforcer for the participant. Phase 3 consisted of 240 total pairings of the social consequences with primary reinforcement. The results of the Phase 4 reinforcer assessment of social consequences are presented in Figure 3. There was an increase in responding over Phase 2b (baseline) for the first praise session (session 47) after the first pairing session, however rates of responding decreased rapidly to baseline levels after this point. Low rates of responding occurred when praise (mean =

Social Consequences 16 1.2; range, 0.0 to 8.7) or back pats (mean = 0.7; range, 0 to 4.3) were delivered contingent on responding and differentially higher rates of responding were observed when chips were delivered contingent on responding (mean = 17; range, 0 to 19.3). Phase 3 pairing and Phase 4 reinforcer assessment of social consequences were alternated until stable responding was observed. The final three sessions for both praise and back pats resulted in zero responding. Thus, neither social consequence was effectively conditioned as a reinforcer for this participant using the pairing procedure. Results from Phase 5 (which served as a new baseline for Phase 7) are displayed in Figure 4. Near-zero rates of responding were observed in the praise, back pats, and extinction conditions. Rates of responding when the high-preference edible was delivered contingent on responding were differentially higher than rates of responding when either social consequence was delivered contingent on responding. Prior to implementation of the S D procedure neither social consequence functioned as a reinforcer for the participant. Phase 6 the S D procedure consisted of a total of 320 conditioning trials. Results from Phase 7 are depicted in Figure 4. Variable low rates of responding were observed in the praise, back pats, and extinction conditions until session 47. After this point the data paths for the praise, back pats, and extinction conditions all co-vary and increase substantially until session 61 when responding in all conditions decreased to previous low rates. An unknown variable appeared to be affecting responding across all conditions in these sessions. Slight differentiation was observed between the social consequences conditions and extinction after session 71. Stable, low rates of responding were maintained when both praise (mean = 1.9; range, 1.3 to 2.3) and back pats (mean = 2.4; range, 1.3 to 3.0) were delivered

Social Consequences 17 contingent on responding. This rate of responding was differentially higher than the rate of responding in the extinction condition (mean = 0.0). These results suggest that the social consequences may have been established as weak conditioned reinforcers using the S D pairing procedure. The results from Phase 8 are presented in Figure 4. Tokens maintained a high rate of responding when delivered contingent on hand raising. Tokens were observed to be effective conditioned reinforcers for the participant. Behavior was maintained at a differentially higher rate when tokens were delivered contingent on responding (mean = 28.1; range, 20.3 to 33.3) compared to rates of responding when social consequences were delivered contingent on responding. All reinforcer assessment data are summarized in Figure 5. It was determined that neither of the social consequences functioned as reinforcers prior to implementation of the conditioning procedures. Social consequences were not effectively conditioned as reinforcers using the pairing procedure (Phase 3), however praise and back pats may have been conditioned as weak reinforcers using the S D procedure (Phase 7). The effect of the social consequences as conditioned reinforcers was weak, however, compared to the effectiveness of tokens as conditioned reinforcers. Data were examined from a random sample of sessions during the S D conditioning procedure to determine whether each social consequence was established as an S D for the response which produced reinforcement. This analysis was conducted to rule out a conclusion that a weak reinforcing effect was observed (compared to tokens) due to an insufficient establishment of the social consequences as S D s. It was determined, however, that the primary

Social Consequences 18 reinforcer was taken within 3 s of the occurrence of the social consequence on 100% of trials, suggesting that the social stimuli did function as S D s for responses which then produced the primary reinforcer. One limitation to the current investigation was that only two conditioning procedures were implemented. Future research might include a condition similar to the established response procedure, in which pairing is contingent on a response (Dozier et al., 2008). Another limitation was the difference in numbers of pairing trials in the two conditioning procedures. Sessions were carried out in each condition until stable responding was observed, leading to a greater number of trials in the S D procedure condition. One question raised by the difference in response rates maintained by the (previouslyconditioned) tokens and the social consequences is the role of the tangible tokens. That is, for this participant the tokens were presented and accumulated and thus constantly present. This is contrasted with the social consequences, which were of a more ephemeral nature. Further, during a reinforcer assessment with tangible stimuli, such as tokens, a trade-in usually occurs at some time after each token is earned. This exchange of tokens for backup reinforcers allows for additional pairing of the stimulus with the back-up reinforcer (functionally becoming a secondorder schedule of reinforcement). In the reinforcer assessments with social stimuli, however, no trade-in occurred after each session and the stimuli, once presented, were no longer available. Future research might examine the schedule of pairing of social consequences with primary reinforcers. If the stimuli had in fact acquired reinforcing properties during the pairing procedures, it is possible that this value was extinguished between the time that the pairing sessions were conducted and all the subsequent reinforcer assessments had been carried out. Previous research has shown that a conditioned reinforcer can lose its reinforcing value if the

Social Consequences 19 frequency with which it is paired with primary reinforcer is too low (Williams, 1994). A future design might include fewer reinforcer assessment probe sessions between pairings. Future research might also examine the nature of the stimuli used. As mentioned above, the social stimuli are presented and then terminated. It is not clear whether it was the ephemeral (brief presentation) nature of the stimuli or the social nature of the stimuli that prevented them from functioning as strong conditioned reinforcers. Additional studies might isolate these characteristics and compare the conditioning of tangible, non-social stimuli (e.g., tokens), brief non-social stimuli (e.g., tone) and brief social (praise) stimuli. It is important that research continues to be conducted in this area so that an effective method can be identified to condition social stimuli as reinforcers.

Social Consequences 20 References Dozier, C. L., Iwata, B. A., Thomason, J. L., & Wilson, D. M. (2007). Establishing Praise as a Conditioned Reinforcer? Presented at the 2007 ABA Conference in Chicago, Il. Kazdin, A. E., & Bootzin, R. B. (1972). The token economy: An evaluative review. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 343-372. Kelleher, R. T. (1966). Conditioned reinforcement in second-order schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 475-485. Kelleher, R. T. & Gollub, L. R. (1962). A review of positive conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 543-597. Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Kinder, M. I., Rubenstein, B. D., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. (1966). Establishment of social reinforcers in two schizophrenic children on the basis of food. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 4, 109-125. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: Free Press. Smaby, K., MacDonald, R. P., Ahearn, W. H., & Dube, W. V. (2007). Assessment protocol for identifying preferred social consequences. Behavioral Interventions, 22, 311-318. Williams, B. A. (1994). Conditioned reinforcement: Experimental and theoretical issues. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 261-285.

Social Consequences 21 Figure Captions Figure 1. Percent selection of edible stimuli during a paired stimulus preference assessment. Figure 2. Responding (hand raising) during reinforcer assessments conducted in Phase 2a and Phase 2b. Stimuli delivered contingent on responding were no programmed consequence (EXT), chips (FR1), praise, and back pats. Figure 3. Responding (hand raising) during reinforcer assessments conducted in Phase 2b and Phase 4. Stimuli delivered contingent on responding were no programmed consequence (EXT), chips (FR1), praise, and back pats. Arrows denote pairing sessions (Phase 3). Figure 4. Responding (hand raising) during reinforcer assessments conducted in Phase 5, Phase 7 and Phase 8. Stimuli delivered contingent on responding were no programmed consequence (EXT), chips (FR1), praise, and back pats, and tokens. Arrows denote pairing sessions (Phase 6). Figure 5. Responding (hand raising) during all reinforcer assessments. Stimuli delivered contingent on responding were no programmed consequence (EXT), chips (FR1), praise, and back pats, and tokens.

Figure 1

Figure 2

Figure 3

Figure 4

Figure 5