AVATARS AND VASES: THE AUTOMATIC PROCESSING OF WHAT OTHER PEOPLE SEE 1

Similar documents
Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

The influence of conflict monitoring on meta-reasoning and response times in a base rate task

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Sometimes Losing Your Self in Space: Children s and Adults Spontaneous Use of Multiple Spatial Reference Frames

Egocentrism and Automatic Perspective Taking in Children and Adults

Which Way Is Which? Examining Global/Local Processing With Symbolic Cues

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Apes submentalise. Cecilia Heyes. All Souls College and Department of Experimental Psychology. University of Oxford. Oxford OX1 4AL.

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Alternation blindness in the perception of binary sequences

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Head Up, Foot Down. Object Words Orient Attention to the Objects Typical Location. Research Report

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture

The path of visual attention

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

2/27/2014. What is Attention? What is Attention? Space- and Object-Based Attention

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go?

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The edge complex: Implicit memory for figure assignment in shape perception

Transfer of Dimensional Associability in Human Contingency Learning

Erica J. Yoon Introduction

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

Ensemble Coding Of Semantic Information: Are Numerical Averages Generated In Parallel?

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

Attentional set for axis of symmetry in symmetry-defined visual search

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Which way is which? Examining symbolic control of attention with compound arrow cues

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Piloting the use of the Modified Attention Network Task in Children

Effect of irrelevant information on the processing of relevant information: Facilitation and/or interference? The influence of experimental design

Black 1 White 5 Black

The relative attractiveness of distractors and targets affects the coming and going of item-specific control: Evidence from flanker tasks

Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing

Chen, Z. (2009). Not all features are created equal: Processing asymmetries between

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching

Bottom-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Conjunctions

Taming the White Bear: Initial Costs and Eventual Benefits of Distractor Inhibition

Using Inverse Planning and Theory of Mind for Social Goal Inference

Running head: EFFECTS OF EMOTION ON TIME AND NUMBER 1. Fewer Things, Lasting Longer: The Effects of Emotion on Quantity Judgments

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty

Title: Gaze bias during visual preference judgments: effects of stimulus category and

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Processing Emergent Features in Metaphor Comprehension

Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture

Cognition xx (0000) xxx xxx. Brief article. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study

News from the field. in the attention or weight given to particular multisensory cues. L.C.N. Emotion perception. The face and voice of emotion

Title: Gaze bias during visual preference judgments: effects of stimulus category and

Examining the Role of Object Size in Judgments of Lateral Separation

Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm. Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren. Chinese Academy of Sciences. Andrew W.

The Effect of Training Context on Fixations Made During Visual Discriminations

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Splitting Visual Focal Attention? It Probably Depends on Who You Are

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Categorization vs. Inference: Shift in Attention or in Representation?

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

Effects of Task Relevance and Stimulus-Driven Salience in Feature-Search Mode

The Color of Similarity

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Please note that this draft may not be identical with the published version.

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm

The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan

Overriding Age Differences in Attentional Capture With Top-Down Processing

Taking control of reflexive social attention

The "Aha! moment: How prior knowledge helps disambiguate ambiguous information. Alaina Baker. Submitted to the Department of Psychology

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

The influence of regularities on temporal order judgments

Social meaning of ambiguous sounds influences retrospective duration judgments

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching

Durham Research Online

First Sight. Jim Carpenter, PhD, ABPP.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Original Papers. Task conditions and short-term memory search: two-phase model of STM search

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Transcription:

AVATARS AND VASES: THE AUTOMATIC PROCESSING OF WHAT OTHER PEOPLE SEE 1 Pavle Valerjev 2 & Marin Dujmović Department of Psychology, University of Zadar We modified the dot perspective task to conduct a simple visual perception experiment. Our participants had to rapidly judge their own or a perspective of a 3D avatar when those perspectives could show the same (consistent) or different number of stimuli (inconsistent). Participants had to judge, as fast as possible, how many stimuli (vases) were seen in the scene by them, or by the avatar. Other studies have shown information of the other person's perspective was processed automatically prolonging response times for inconsistent trials even when participants made judgments from their own perspective. Recent research has been focused on what is the contribution of social versus perceptual information for the size of this interference. Our goal was to examine whether perspective taking was under the influence of a salient characteristic, skin tone. Participants had to make judgments form self/other perspective in consistent and inconsistent trials for two identical female 3D models which differed only in RGB values of skin color. Results show a significant effect of consistency: participants were faster in consistent trials. A significant consistency-perspective interaction revealed a stronger interference effect when taking the other perspective than the self perspective. The skin color effect was also significant with slightly faster responses for similar-color avatars. Keywords: dot perspective task, visual perspective taking, mentalizing, visual attention Introduction Mentalizing is defined as the ability to represent other people s mental states and is based on the concept of theory of mind (Premack & Woodrugg, 1978). People are capable of attributing mental states to others and take their point of view. Recent research indicated that processing other people s perspective happens on an implicit level even when participants are instructed to ignore the other person (Samson, Apperly, Braithwaite, Andrews, & Bodley Scott, 2010). These authors introduced the dot perspective task procedure. In this procedure participants are asked to take one of two perspectives: their own (self) or the perspective of an observer presented in an image (other). In one set of trials the same number of targets could 1 This research was supported by Grant 4139 from the Croatian Science Foundation. 2 valerjev@unizd.hr 70

be viewed from both perspectives while for the other set a different number of targets can be viewed from the two perspectives (see Figure 1). Figure 1. An inconsistent type of trial from Samson et al. (2010, pp. 1261) First, both the perspective and number of targets were cued. Then, participants had to decide whether the cued number of dots could be seen from the cued perspective. The main effect was a slower response in inconsistent conditions. This effect proved to be stronger when they had to take the perspective of the other person, but was also present even when they were instructed to take the self perspective which implied that the other perspective is automatically processed. This effect has been replicated in other studies. However, it is not clear whether it is a bottom-up process based on perceptive cues or if there is a top-down social component contributing to the effect. According to Santiesteban, Catmur, Coughlan Hopkins, Bird, and Heyes (2014) the effect is perceptive in nature. They used human-like avatars and arrows which were perceptively similar to the avatar. Results showed no difference in the interference effect for the two types of figures. Using a different paradigm Cole and his colleagues (Cole, Smith, & Atkinson, 2015; Cole, Atkinson, Le, & Smith, 2016) reached the same conclusion. They tested the social component by placing barriers in the field of view of the avatars. Since they found no effect of the barriers on the interference they concluded participants did not automatically process the avatar s mental state. Rather, participants just processed the cueing information. On the other hand Kragh Nielsen, Slade, Levy, and Holmes (2015) found that the interference effect was lower for abstract compared to semisocial and social central figures. They also found a positive correlation between components of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index and interference size for social but not for the other conditions. The aim of our study was to replicate the typical interference effect with a slightly different procedure by directly asking participant how many stimuli can be seen depending 71

on the perspective, and to determine the impact of manipulating the similarity of a humanlike avatar and our participants on that effect. Method Participants and design A total of 35 participants (all female undergraduate psychology students) completed a 2x2x2 repeated measures experiment. The independent variables were consistency (consistent/inconsistent), perspective (self/other) and avatar skin color (light/dark). Stimuli All of the presented stimuli were designed using Daz3D software. They consisted of a female avatar and one or two objects (vases). Depending on the gaze orientation of the avatar and the position of the objects (always one or two present objects) the avatar could see the same or a different number of objects as the participant (consistency variable). The same 3D avatar was used for both levels of the skin color variable. The only difference was the RGB value of skin tone color (Figure 2). Figure 2. Examples of presented stimuli 72

Procedure Participants completed two types of training tasks. The first was a choice reaction task in order for the participants to become familiar with the mode of responding. After that they completed four practice trials using the avatar stimuli. The main measurement consisted of 96 trials (12 per experimental situation). The single trial procedure is depicted in Figure 3. Figure 3. Single trial procedure Participants were instructed to answer, as fast and as accurately as possible, how many objects could be viewed depending on the perspective taking cue, by pressing the appropriate numerical key ( 1 or 2 ). This was different from the original procedure in which participants reacted to a cued number rather than the number of targets. Stimuli from all eight experimental situations were randomized for each participant and presented in a single block. Results For each participant results were formed as median scores for each condition. Data for response times were normally distributed for all eight experimental conditions. A 2(consistency) x 2(perspective) x 2(skin tone) repeated measures ANOVA resulted in a significant consistency effect (F(1,34) = 32.49, p <.01, ηp 2 =.49). Participants responded faster in consistent compared to inconsistent trials. Skin tone color effect was also significant (F(1,34) = 4.88, p <.05, ηp 2 =.13). Participants were slightly faster when reacting to stimuli in which the avatar had lighter skin color. Finally, the consistency-perspective interaction effect was also significant (F(1,34) = 5.45, p <.05, ηp 2 =.14). The results are depicted in Figure 4. 73

Figure 4. Response times as a function of skin tone, consistency and perspective The interference effect was stronger when taking the other perspective than when taking the self perspective which was confirmed by Tukey HSD post-hoc tests. On average participants made 2.62% errors in consistent and 6.31% in inconsistent trials. Error rates were not normally distributed so the difference was compared using the Wilcoxon matched pairs test, which showed the difference was significant (T = 69, Z = 3.36, p <.01). To further test the robustness of the skin color effect the same analysis was conducted only on the last third of trials. We speculated that if the effect was robust it would remain regardless of training effects. The 2x2x2 repeated measure ANOVA showed only a significant effect of consistency (F(1,34) = 19.51, p <.01, ηp 2 =.36). The skin color effect was no longer significant. The consistency-perspective interaction effect was also not significant but the same trend remained, even post-hoc comparisons show a stronger interference effect in the other perspective. Discussion and conclusion Our study replicated the robust interference effect found in previous research. Participants were generally faster to respond in consistent trials compared to inconsistent trials. Additionally, this effect was stronger when taking the other perspective than the self 74

perspective. Even though the avatar s perspective had an interference effect in inconsistent trials we can conclude the self perspective is more dominant, which is also a replication of the results from Samson et al. (2010). There was no interaction between interference size and skin tone color, and the general effect of skin color disappeared with practice which indicates it is not an effect of social but rather perceptive information. We can conclude our participants experienced the same magnitude of interference regardless of skin tone color. Even though our results do not imply an impact of social information, further research should manipulate one or more social components of the avatars. Gender, age, socio-economic status (based on clothes of the avatar), and more complex signs like subculture affiliation are all candidates for experimental manipulation. It is our hypothesis that the effect of social information is subtle and sensitive requiring strict and thorough investigation. References Cole, G.G., Smith, D.T., & Atkinson, M.A. (2015). Mental state attribution and the gaze cueing effect. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77, 1105-1115. Cole, G.G., Atkinson, M., Le, A.T.D., & Smith, D.T. (2016). Do humans spontaneously take the perspective of others? Acta Psychologica, 164, 165-168. Kragh Nielsen, M., Slade, L., Levy, J.P., & Holmes, A. (2015). Inclined to see it your way: Do altercentric intrusion effects in visual perspective taking reflect an intrinsically social process? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(10), 1931-1951. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515-526. Samson, D., Apperiy, 1. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255-1266. Santiesteban, I., Catmur, C., Coughlan Hopkins, S., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2014). Avatars and arrows: implicit mentalizing or domain-general processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 929 937. 75