Supporting Online Material for

Similar documents
A behaviour analysis of Theory of Mind: from interpretation to application

Eye-tracking brings focus to 'theory of mind'

Implicit and explicit Theory of Mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: The impact of experience

[ 2011] Lu Wang ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

BIROn - Birkbeck Institutional Research Online

Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorder

PREDICTIVE GAZE DURING OBSERVATION OF IRRATIONAL ACTIONS IN ADULTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM CONDITIONS

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

Revisiting the Strange Stories: Revealing Mentalizing Impairments in Autism

Title: Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Praise for Teaching Children with Autism to Mind-Read: The Workbook

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions

The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in Japan: A Cross-Cultural Comparison

Study N Ages Study type Methodology Main findings m;f mean (sd) FSIQ mean (sd) Del;UPD Range Range

5. Theory of Mind. Toyoaki Nishida Kyoto University

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR

Autism Spectrum Disorder. Aims for Session 3 03/04/2018. Neurodevelopmental Disorders Professor Pamela Heaton. Session 3

Teaching children with autism to talk about private events: establishing the verbal behaviour of emotions, inferences and perspective taking.

The use of eye-tracking to explore social difficulties in cognitively able students with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study

Understanding Perspective-taking, False Belief, and Deception from a Behavioural Perspective

",! " ", " " " " "!!! " $.

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Cultural background modulates how we look at other persons gaze

Is Theory of Mind Understanding Impaired in Males with Fragile X Syndrome?

IMAGINATION, LANGUAGE AND AUTISM: INSIGHTS FROM EYE MOVEMENTS. Heather Ferguson, University of Kent

Lecture. The relationship between metacognition and the

Guessing versus choosing and seeing versus believing in false belief tasks

Spontaneous Attention to Faces in Asperger Sndrome using Ecologically Valid Static Stimuli

Investigating false belief levels of typically developed children and children with autism

Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language

Mechanisms of Belief-Desire Reasoning Inhibition and Bias

Referential and Cooperative Bias: in Defence of an Implicit Theory of Mind. Josef Perner. University of Salzburg

University of Warwick institutional repository: This paper is made available online in accordance with publisher

Mechanisms of belief-desire reasoning: Inhibition and bias

Teaching children with autism to talk about private events: establishing the verbal behaviour of emotions, inferences and perspective taking.

the degree of dissociation between theory of mind and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage

The links between Autistic traits, Theory of Mind and Moral Judgement.

A cue-based approach to theory of mind : Re-examining the notion of automaticity

PATIENCE YOU MUST HAVE MY YOUNG PADAWAN"

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Toddlers Transitions on Non-verbal Falsebelief Tasks Involving a Novel Location: A Constructivist Connectionist Model

Attentional Differences in a Driving Hazard Perception Task in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Durham Research Online

Which assessment tool is most useful to diagnose adult autism spectrum disorder?

Brief Report: Impression Formation in High-Functioning Autism: Role of Nonverbal Behavior and Stereotype Activating Information

Supplementary Information. Enhancing studies of the connectome in autism using the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange II

Investigation of autistic traits through strategic decision-making in games with adaptive agents

FAR FROM ACTION-BLIND: REPRESENTATION OF OTHERS ACTIONS IN INDIVIDUALS WITH AUTISM

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure

AUTISM: THEORY OF MIND. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION

Breaking the Rules: Do Infants Have a True Understanding of False Beliefs? Jessica Yott. A Thesis. The Department. Psychology

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years

A Computational Developmental Model of the Implicit False Belief Task

SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1

TargeT PoPulaTion identifying appropriate ParTiciPanTs

Social Cognition: What Is It and What Does It Tell Us About How to Teach? Kari Dunn Buron.

Modeling the Influence of Situational Variation on Theory of Mind Wilka Carvalho Mentors: Ralph Adolphs, Bob Spunt, and Damian Stanley

Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development

Simplifying Second-order Belief Attribution: What Facilitates Children s Performance on Measures of Conceptual Understanding?

References 1. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2006). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th edition. Minnesota: American Guidance Service. 2. Wilkinson, K.

Theory of Mind and Au0sm. Read: Baron- Cohen et al Happé 1993

How Does the Gender of Spokesperson Affect the Audience s Perception

Processing demands in belief-desire reasoning: inhibition or general difficulty?

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Enhanced Visual Search for a Conjunctive Target in Autism: A Research Note

Apes submentalise. Cecilia Heyes. All Souls College and Department of Experimental Psychology. University of Oxford. Oxford OX1 4AL.

What is Autism and. How to Make a Diagnosis. Dene Robertson

The Role of Inhibitory Control and Social Cognition in a Naturalistic Theory of Mind Test

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine

The effect of social stories on reduction of challenging behaviours in autistic children

REPORT Why specific developmental disorders are not specific: On-line and developmental effects in autism and dyslexia

Coralie CHEVALLIER. 1. Background to the classic paper

Is theory of mind understanding impaired in males with fragile X Syndrome? Grant, C., Apperly, I. & Oliver, C

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

Writing, Asperger Syndrome and Theory of Mind. Heather M. Brown and Perry D. Klein. Faculty of Education

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Seeing the World through Others Minds: Inferring Social Context from Behaviour. Selangor, Malaysia.

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

Humans and chimpanzees attend differently to goal-directed actions

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING AND THE FALSE BELIEF TASK: A DEVELOPMENTAL STUDY

Although directions of influence are difficult to clarify,

Chapter 6: Non-parametric models

Measuring the value of social engagement in adults with and without autism

Perspective-taking deficits in people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective investigation

Increasing Social Awareness in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS)

How to Pass the False-Belief Task Before Your Fourth Birthday

Goal prediction in 2-year-old children with and without autism spectrum disorder: An eyetracking study: Goal prediction in...

Three Minute Review. critiques of Piaget s theories information processing perspective

Three Minute Review. 1. Sensorimotor Stage (0-2) physics, senses, movement, object permanence

Three Minute Review. Test Yourself. Recommended Homework

Why do we look at people's eyes?

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Research Article Eye Movement Sequences during Simple versus Complex Information Processing of Scenes in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Transcription:

www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/1176170/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome Atsushi Senju,* Victoria Southgate, Sarah White, Uta Frith *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: a.senju@bbk.ac.uk This PDF file includes: Materials and Methods SOM Text References Published 16 July 2009 on Science Express DOI: 10.1126/science.1176170 Other Supporting Online Material for this manuscript includes the following: (available at www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/1176170/dc1) Movies S1 and S2

Materials and Methods Participants. Data from 19 participants with Asperger Syndrome (5 female, 14 male), as well as 17 neurotypical participants (5 female, 12 male), matched by age and IQ, were included in the analyses (Table 1). An additional neurotypical participant was excluded from the analyses because of a technical problem. All the participants were recruited through the autism research database at the UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, and written informed consent was obtained from each participant before the study began. The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee. All participants in the Asperger group had previously been diagnosed with Asperger Syndrome by a qualified clinical psychologist or psychiatrist. To assess their current clinical manifestation, the Autism Diagnostic Observational Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G: (1)) was administered. Eight of the Asperger participants met the criteria for Autism, 5 for Autism Spectrum, and the remaining 6 scored below the cut-off point. These latter participants were significantly older (mean age 48 vs. mean age 32, t(17) = 2.67, p =.02, Cohen s d = 1.18, t-test) and presumably better compensated. We report the results obtained from all the participants with AS, having confirmed that they did not change when only those participants scoring above the ADOS score of 7 were included in the analyses. Verbal, non-verbal and full-scale IQ was measured with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III UK: (2)). All participants in both the AS and neurotypical group completed the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ: (3)) as well. The participants also completed a battery of Theory of mind (ToM) tasks, which consisted of five first-order false belief tests and two second-order false belief tests, requiring the participant to either interpret another person s behaviour or to understand another s emotion based on the false belief of a protagonist (Sally-Ann: (4); Smarties: (5); Interpretational false belief: (6); Belief-emotion and Real-apparent emotion: (7); Ice Cream Van: (8); Coat Story: (9)). References for these tests are presented in the text. Answers were marked as a pass or fail (1 or 0) for each task. In addition the participants were asked to justify their choice; this was marked as a correct mental state justification (e.g. because she doesn t know it s in the box ), a correct non-mental state justification (e.g. because she left it in the basket ), or an incorrect justification (e.g. because no-one stole it apart from Anne ). This additional scoring (1, 0.5 or 0 respectively) aimed to check for false positive responses when the participant was achieving the correct answer by guessing, and also was expected to increase the variation in responses with the aim of avoiding ceiling effects. Control questions (memory and reality questions: e.g. where did Sally put the ball in the beginning? ) were also administered. Prompt questions (e.g. where did the icecream man tell John he was going? ) were asked during the more complex tasks, which all participants were required to pass in order to check for comprehension of the scenario. A composite ToM score was calculated by summing the number of correct responses and justifications (max = 14) across these 7 tests. ToM scores ranged from 4 to 13.5 in the Asperger group, and from 9 to 12.5 in neurotypical group. There was no significant difference between the two groups on the composite ToM score (Table 1). All participants with Asperger syndrome passed the two classic False Belief tasks (i.e. Sally-Ann and Smarties). In addition, an advanced theory of mind test (Strange Stories, (10) using eight ToM stories (11)) was also administered. After each story had been read, the participant was presented with a question about its content (e.g. Why did Brian say this?). Correct answers have to provide context-appropriate mental state explanations for the story characters' non-literal utterances. The score represents the sum total of correct answers about the mental

states of protagonists (fail = 0; pass = 1) and correct justifications (fail = 0; physical justification = 0.5; mental justification = 1; max = 16). Full test materials and detailed scoring criteria is presented in White et al. (12). The scores ranged from 10 to 16 in the Asperger group, and from 12 to 16 in the neurotypical group. No group differences were observed (Table 1). Apparatus and Stimuli. A Tobii (Stockholm, Sweden) T120 Eye Tracker, integrated with a 17-inch TFT monitor was used to present the stimuli and record eye movements. Stimulus presentation and recording were controlled via a computer with Tobii s Studio software. Participants were seated approximately 50 cm from the monitor. A nine-point calibration was completed before the stimulus presentation. The stimuli consisted of four familiarization trials, followed by one of two test trials (Figure 1, Movies S1 and S2). All participants viewed the same four familiarization trials, and were randomly assigned to receive the FB1 or FB2 test trial. The stimuli were identical to those used in our previous study (13), with two exceptions. Firstly, in our previous study with young children, a large number of participants were excluded for failing to pass the second (final) familiarization trial. To ensure participants learnt the contingency between the cues (illumination and chime) and subsequent window opening, we added an additional two familiarization trials in the beginning in which no object was hidden, but a small toy was visible sitting on top of one of the boxes. This scene was then followed by the onset of the cues, and the actor subsequently opened the window above the box on which the toy sat. Secondly, in the single test trial presented to each participant, no action outcome was revealed. Instead, the movie was frozen for 5 seconds following the 1 second illumination and chime. The purpose of the familiarization trials was (a) to show that the actor s goal was to obtain a specific object, and (b) to teach the participant the link between the windows being illuminated together with a corresponding chime sound, and the subsequent opening of one of the windows by the actor. To be included in the analysis, participants were required to understand the contingency between the cues (illumination of the windows with the simultaneous activation of a chime sound) (Figure 1B) and the actor s impending reaching, and had to display anticipatory looking toward the correct window by the fourth familiarization trial. These trials did not involve the actor having a false belief. All the participants met this criterion. Two versions of the test trials, FB1 (Movie S1) and FB2 (Movie S2), were used in order to counterbalance the first and last location of the target object, as well as the last location of the actor s gaze, which could affect participants eye movements. The same familiarization trials were presented to all the participants, in the same order, and then each participant observed a single test trial (FB1 or FB2), counterbalanced between participants. The reason why we only administered one trial per participant was to avoid the possibility that they would inadvertently learn the outcome (i.e. nothing happens for the period of 5s after the illumination and a chime), which could affect their motivation to look towards the windows on any subsequent trial. See Movie S1 and S2 for videos used in the current study. Data reduction. Eye-tracking data was analyzed as follows: After recording, a gaze replay file showing the exact location of each participant s gaze was exported at 25 frames per second with the Tobii Studio program. From the exported data, fixation points were coded frame-by-frame starting from the point when the windows were illuminated to the end of stimulus presentation (6 seconds). Having obtained the total looking time to each of the two windows, a differential looking score (DLS) was calculated by subtracting the total looking time to the incorrect window from looking to the correct window, and by dividing it by the sum

of looking time to correct and incorrect windows. The DLS is a measure of preference for looking at one target over another, and can range between 1 and -1, being closer to 1 if the participant spent most time looking at the correct location, and closer to zero if the participant looked randomly to both windows. We also coded the direction of first saccade after the illumination of the windows. Supporting Text Additional analyses on eye tracking data. To further examine the group differences in the pattern of visual scanning, the total number of fixations and the mean duration of fixation during the text trial were calculated and compared between groups. Fixation was defined as the continuous gaze within the radius of 2.5º of visual angle for at least 100 ms, and extracted with the Tobii Studio program. No differences were observed between groups in the number of fixations (Asperger group: Mean±SD: 81.8±36.8; Neurotypical group: 69.4±22.9 in the neurotypical group, t(34) = 1.20, p =.24, Cohen s d =.40, t-test) or duration of fixation (Asperger group: Mean±SD: 639.5±253.7 ms; Neurotypical group: 934.6±625.2 ms, t(34) = 1.89, p =.07, Cohen s d =.62, t-test). In addition, the number of saccades toward the actor s face during the test trial was analyzed to examine the potential group difference in their attention to the actor s gaze. However, results revealed that both groups made a similar number of saccades towards the actor s face (Asperger group: Mean±SD: 8.0±4.9; Neurotypical group: 10.7±6.8, t(34) = 1.39, p =.17, Cohen s d =.46, t-test). Thus, differences in action anticipation based on false belief between the two groups cannot be attributed to an atypical pattern of fixation or reduced monitoring of the actor s gaze in the Asperger group.

Supporting References 1. C. Lord et al., J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 205 (2000). 2. D. Wechsler, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale for Children (WAIS). (Psychological Corporation, London, ed. 3rd UK, 1999). 3. S. Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, R. Skinner, J. Martin, E. Clubley, J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5 (2001). 4. S. Baron-Cohen, A. M. Leslie, U. Frith, Cognition 21, 37 (1985). 5. J. Perner, S. R. Leekam, H. Wimmer, Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 5, 125 (1987). 6. T. Luckett, S. D. Powell, D. J. Messer, M. E. Thornton, J. Schulz, J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 127 (2002). 7. H. M. Wellman, D. Liu, Child Dev. 75, 523 (2004). 8. J. Perner, H. Wimmer, J. Exp. Child Psychol. 39, 437 (1985). 9. D. M. Bowler, J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 33, 877 (1992). 10. F. G. Happe, J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129 (1994). 11. P. C. Fletcher et al., Cognition 57, 109 (1995). 12. S. White, E. Hill, F. Happé, U. Frith, Child Dev. in press. 13. V. Southgate, A. Senju, G. Csibra, Psychol. Sci. 18, 587 (2007).

Movie S1. Stimulus sequence (FB1). The resolution of the movie was reduced for on-line presentation. Movie S2. Stimulus sequence (FB2). The resolution of the movie was reduced for on-line presentation.