A developmental learning rule for coincidence. tuning in the barn owl auditory system. Wulfram Gerstner, Richard Kempter J.

Similar documents
Temporal coding in the sub-millisecond range: Model of barn owl auditory pathway

Temporally asymmetric Hebbian learning and neuronal response variability

A dendritic model of coincidence detection in the avian brainstem

Temporal coding in the auditory brainstem of the barn owl

Computational maps (1, 2) transform information about the

Formation of temporal-feature maps by axonal propagation of synaptic learning 1

Active Control of Spike-Timing Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in an Electrosensory System

Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression

Spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity can form zero lag links for cortical oscillations.

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report

Auditory System & Hearing

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Neural Development of Binaural Tuning through Hebbian Learning Predicts Frequency-Dependent Best Delays

Long-term depression and recognition of parallel "bre patterns in a multi-compartmental model of a cerebellar Purkinje cell

Theme 2: Cellular mechanisms in the Cochlear Nucleus

21/01/2013. Binaural Phenomena. Aim. To understand binaural hearing Objectives. Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source

Adaptive leaky integrator models of cerebellar Purkinje cells can learn the clustering of temporal patterns

The Auditory Nervous System

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning

Processing in The Superior Olivary Complex

Beyond Vanilla LTP. Spike-timing-dependent-plasticity or STDP

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 2. Neuron. Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava

Electrophysiology. General Neurophysiology. Action Potentials

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Nucleus Laminaris. Yuan WangJason Tait Sanchez, and Edwin W Rubel

Lecture 7 Hearing 2. Raghav Rajan Bio 354 Neurobiology 2 February 04th All lecture material from the following links unless otherwise mentioned:

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell:

Lab 4: Compartmental Model of Binaural Coincidence Detector Neurons

A Model of Visually Guided Plasticity of the Auditory Spatial Map in the Barn Owl

Binaural Hearing. Steve Colburn Boston University

Transitions between dierent synchronous ring modes using synaptic depression

Inhibition: Effects of Timing, Time Scales and Gap Junctions

1 Introduction Synchronous ring of action potentials amongst multiple neurons is a phenomenon that has been observed in a wide range of neural systems

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization

Neuroethology in Neuroscience or Why study an exotic animal

A general error-based spike-timing dependent learning rule for the Neural Engineering Framework

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Model-Based Reinforcement Learning by Pyramidal Neurons: Robustness of the Learning Rule

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at

The Role of Coincidence-Detector Neurons in the Reliability and Precision of Subthreshold Signal Detection in Noise

Different inhibitory effects by dopaminergic modulation and global suppression of activity

Dynamic Stochastic Synapses as Computational Units

Structure of a Neuron:

The storage and recall of memories in the hippocampo-cortical system. Supplementary material. Edmund T Rolls

STDP enhances synchrony in feedforward network

Basics of Computational Neuroscience: Neurons and Synapses to Networks

On learning time delays between the spikes from different input neurons in a biophysical model of a pyramidal neuron

Temporally Asymmetric Hebbian Learning, Spike Timing and Neuronal Response Variability

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Hearing in the Environment

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press.

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 5. The peripheral auditory system: The ear as a signal processor

Spectrograms (revisited)

Distributed Synchrony of Spiking Neurons in a Hebbian Cell Assembly

Cellular Bioelectricity

Realization of Visual Representation Task on a Humanoid Robot

Retinal Waves and Ocular Dominance Columns

Bioscience in the 21st century

Signal detection in networks of spiking neurons with dynamical synapses

What is Anatomy and Physiology?

QUIZ YOURSELF COLOSSAL NEURON ACTIVITY

Stable Hebbian Learning from Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models

NEW ROLES FOR SYNAPTIC INHIBITION IN SOUND LOCALIZATION

The Central Auditory System

The mammalian cochlea possesses two classes of afferent neurons and two classes of efferent neurons.

Emanuel Todorov, Athanassios Siapas and David Somers. Dept. of Brain and Cognitive Sciences. E25-526, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139

Efficient Emulation of Large-Scale Neuronal Networks

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Hearing II Perceptual Aspects

VS : Systemische Physiologie - Animalische Physiologie für Bioinformatiker. Neuronenmodelle III. Modelle synaptischer Kurz- und Langzeitplastizität

STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

Binaural processing of complex stimuli

Chapter 6 subtitles postsynaptic integration

Neuromorphic computing

CODING OF AUDITORY SPACE

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or to the head TA (jcolas).

The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve

GABAergic Inhibition Sharpens the Frequency Tuning and Enhances Phase Locking in Chicken Nucleus Magnocellularis Neurons

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

Computing with Spikes in Recurrent Neural Networks

Modeling the cochlear nucleus: A site for monaural echo suppression?

Questions Addressed Through Study of Behavioral Mechanisms (Proximate Causes)

Temporal processing in sensory systems Benedikt Grothe* and Georg M Klump

Model-free detection of synchrony in neuronal spike trains, with an application to primate somatosensory cortex

An Auditory System Modeling in Sound Source Localization

A SUPERVISED LEARNING

Auditory System & Hearing

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Spatial hearing and sound localization mechanisms in the brain. Henri Pöntynen February 9, 2016

Neurons: Structure and communication

Learning to localise sounds with spiking neural networks

L14. Sound Localization 2

Neuron Phase Response

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Transcription:

A developmental learning rule for coincidence tuning in the barn owl auditory system Wulfram Gerstner, Richard Kempter J.Leo van Hemmen Institut fur Theoretische Physik, Physik-Department der TU Munchen D-85748 Garching bei Munchen, Germany and Hermann Wagner Lehrstuhl fur Zoologie/Tierphysiologie Institut fur Biologie II, RWTH Aachen Kopernikusstr. 6, D-5274 Aachen, Germany Binaural coincidence detection is essential for the localization of external sounds and requires auditory signal processing with high temporal precision. We present an integrate-and-re model of spike processing in the third order nucleus laminaris of the auditory pathway of the barn owl. Each input spike generates an excitatory postsynaptic potential with a width of 25 s. Output spikes occur with a tenfold enhanced temporal precision. This is possible since neuronal connections are ne tuned during a critical period of development as has been suggested by recent experiments. This rule does not only explain the temporal presicion in the output, but causes also a tuning to interaural time dierence. The learning rule is of the Hebbian type: A synaptic weight is increased if a presynaptic spike arrives at about the same time as or slightly before postsynaptic ring. A presynaptic spike arriving after postsynaptic ring leads to a decrease of the synaptic ecacy. Introduction Owls are able to locate acoustic signals based on the extraction of interaural time dierence (ITD) by coincidence detection [, 2, 3]. The spatial resolution of sound localization found in behavioral experiments corresponds to a temporal resolution of auditory signal processing in the range of a few s. It follows that present address: Swiss Federal Insititute of Technology, Center for Neuromimetic Systems, EPFL-DI, CH-5 Lausanne, email: wgerst@di.ep.ch

both the ring of spikes and their transmission along the so-called time pathway of the auditory system must occur with high temporal precision. Each neuron in the nucleus laminaris, the third processing stage in the ascending auditory pathway, responds to signals in a narrow frequency range. Its spikes are phase locked to a stimulation tone for frequencies up to 8 khz [4, 5]. The nuclues laminaris is of particular interest, because it is the rst processing stage where signals from the right and left ear converge. Owls use the interaural phase dierence (that is, the interaural time dierence modulo the period T of the stimulus) for azimuthal sound localization. For a successful localization, the temporal precision of spike encoding and transmission must be at least in the range of some s. This poses at least two severe problems. First, the neural architecture has to be adapted to operating with high temporal precision. Considering the fact that the total delay from the ear to the nucleus magnocellularis is approximately 2-3 ms [4], a temporal precision of some s requires some ne tuning, possibly based on learning. Recent experiments have shown that the tuning to ITDs evolves during a critical period of the development [6]. Here we suggest that Hebbian learning is an appropriate mechanism. Second, neurons must operate with the necessary temporal precision. A ring precision of some s seems truly remarkable considering the fact that neuronal time constants are probably in the range of -3 s [7, 8, 9]. It is shown below that neuronal spikes can be transmitted with a temporal precision of 25 s despite the width of a single excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) of 25 s. This is possible because spikes are always triggered during the rise time of an EPSP. 2 Neuron model We concentrate on a single frequency channel of the auditory pathway and model a neuron of the nucleus laminaris. Since synapses are directly located on the soma or very short dendrites, the spatial structure of the neuron can be reduced to a single compartment. In order to simplify the dynamics, we take an integrate-and-re unit. Its membrane potential changes according to d dt u =? u + I(t) () where I(t) is some input and is the membrane time constant. The neuron res, if u(t) crosses a threshold #. This denes a ring time t out. After ring u is reset to an initial value u =. Since auditory neurons are known to be fast, we assume a membrane time constant of s. The laminaris neuron receives input from several presynaptic neurons k K. Each input spike at time t f k generates a current pulse which decays exponentially with a time constant r = s. The magnitude of the current 2

pulse depends on the coupling strength J k. The total input is I(t) = KX XFk k= f= J k exp( t? tf k r ) (t? t f k ) (2) where (x) is the unit step function and the sum runs over all synapses k K and all input spikes f F k. Combining Eqs. () and (2) we nd that each input evokes an EPSP with a width of 25 s at half maximum. This decay is twice as fast as in chicken [7, 8, 9], but seems reasonable for an expert auditory-cue based hunter like the barn owl. We focus on a laminaris neuron which is tuned to a 5 khz signal (period T = 2s) and stimulate both ears with a tone of the optimal frequency and vanishing interaural time dierence (ITD=). Input spikes from presynaptic terminals are phase locked to the stimulus tone with a jitter of 4 s. The mean phase of spikes arriving at a synapse k is dierent from the mean phase at another synapse k because the transmission lines from the ear to the nucleus laminaris have dierent delays. It has been estimated from experimental data that the signal transmission delay k from the ear to the nucleus laminaris varies between 2 and 3 ms [4]. Note that a delay as small as.2 ms shifts the signal by a full period. Thus, if the delays k for the synapses k with k K are chosen at random between 2 and 3 ms, then the periodicity of the signal is lost after integrating over dierent presynpatic inputs. We therefore need a tuning process which selects transmission lines with matching delays. 3 Temporal tuning through learning We assume a developmental period of unsupervised learning during which a ne tuning of the temporal characteristics of signal transmission takes place. Before learning the laminaris neuron receives many inputs (K = 6, 3 from each the left and the right ear) with weak coupling (J k = ). Due to the broad distribution of delays the total input (2) has, apart from uctuations, no temporal structure. The output is not sensitive to the interaural time dierence (ITD); cf. Fig., top. During learning some connections increase their strength to a maximum of J max = 3, others are removed. A slight sensitivity to the ITD develops as seen in experiments [6]; cf. Fig., middle. After learning, the laminaris neuron receives 75 inputs from each ear [5]. The connections to those neurons have become very eective; cf. Fig., bottom. The surviving inputs all have nearly the same time delay modulo the period T of the stimulus. Thus, after learning the number of synapses is reduced and only inputs with matching delays survive. On the other hand, with 75 inputs remaining from each ear there is a high degree of convergence or 'pooling' which leads to a signicant increase in temporal precision. To see this, we compare the phase jitter of the output spikes with that of the presynaptic input lines. The phase jitter of 3

the presynaptic spike trains corresponds to a temporal precision of 4 s. In contrast, after learning the output spikes have a temporal precision of 25 s. Thus our learning rule leads to the ne tuning of the neuronal connections necessary for precise temporal coding. We use a variant of Hebbian learning. In standard Hebbian learning, synaptic weights are changed if pre- and postsynaptic activity occurs simultaneously. In the context of temporal coding by spikes, the concept of `simultaneous activity' has to be rened. We assume that a synapse k is changed, if a presynaptic spike t f k and a postsynaptic spike t out occur within a learning window W (t f k? t out). More precisely, each pair of presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes changes a synapse J k by an amount J k = W (t f k? t out): (3) Depending on the sign of W (x), a contact to a presynaptic neuron is either increased or decreased, in agreement with recent experimental results []. A decrease below J k = and an increase above J max is not allowed. In our model, we assume a function W (x) with two phases. For x, the function W (x) is positive. This leads to a strengthening (potentiation) of the contact with a presynaptic neuron k which is active shortly before or after a postsynaptic spike. Synaptic contacts which become active more than 3 ms later than the postsynaptic spike are decreased. Finally, an active synapse is always increased by a small amount, even if no output spike occurs. The eect of learning on a synapse k after F k input spikes and N out output spikes is J k = F k + XX Nout Fk n= f= W (t f k? tn out ) : (4) We require R W (s)ds =?W <. The combination of decrease and increase then balances the average eects of potentiation and depression and leads to a normalization of the number and weight of synapses. Learning occurs in three phases. At the beginning, input and output spikes are not correlated. In particular, output spikes are not phase locked to the stimulation tone. During this phase, all weights J k approach a transitory equilibrium (dened by hj k i = ) which leads to a mean output ring rate r = =W. We recall that each synapse k corresponds to a delay line with a delay k. In Fig. we have plotted the value J for each synapse as a function of the delay. The initial distribution is at. Shot noise due to the spike nature of the signals leads to spontaneous symmetry breaking and starts the second phase of learning (Fig., middle). As a result, J() develops a T -periodic modulation. In the third and nal stage of learning, all values J() approach a limiting value, either or J max = 3. The output spikes now show a high degree of phase locking measured by the vector strength. Moreover, the output ring rate r depends on the interaural time dierence (ITD), as found in experiments with adult owls. 4

3.2 J( ) 3 T VS.2 r [khz] 3.2 2 3 [ms] T ITD Figure : Development of tuning to a 5kHz tone. In the left column, we show the strength of synaptic contacts as a function of the delay. On the righthand side, we show the vector strength (vs, solid line) and the output ring rate (r, dashed) as a function of the interaural time delay (ITD). Top. Before learning, there are 6 synapses (3 from each ear) with dierent delays, chosen randomly from a Gaussian distribution with mean 2.5 ms and variance.3 ms. The output is not phase-locked (vs :) and shows no dependence upon the ITD. Middle. During learning, some synapses are strengthened others decreased. Those synapses which increase have delays which are similar to each other or dier by multiples of the period T = :2 ms of the stimulating tone. The vector strength of the output increases and starts to depend on the ITD [6]. Bottom. After learning, only 5 synapses (75 from each ear) survive. Both the ring rate r and the vector strength vs show the characteristic dependence upon the ITD as seen in experiments with adult owls [5]. The neuron has the maximal response (r = 2 Hz) for ITD= which is the stimulus used during the learning session of the model neuron. The vector strength at ITD= is vs :8 which corresponds to a temporal precision of 25 s. By denition, the vector strength is proportional to the amplitude of the rst Fourier component of the distribution of output phases, normalized so that vs = indicates perfect phase locking (innite temporal precision). 5

Acknowledgments: R.K. holds scholarship of the state of Bavaria. W.G. has been supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) under grant number He 729/2-2, H.W. by a Heisenberg fellowship of the DFG. References [] L. A. Jeress, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 4, 35 (948). [2] M. Konishi, Trends in Neurosciences 9, 63 (986). [3] C. E. Carr, Annual Rev. Neurosci. 6, 223 (993). [4] W. E. Sullivan and M. Konishi, J. Neurosci. 4, 787 (984). [5] C. E. Carr and M. Konishi, J. Neurosci., 3227 (99). [6] C. E. Carr, in Proceedings of the th International Symposium on Hearing (Irsee, Bavaria, 994), pp. 2{26. [7] E. M. Overholt, E. W. Rubel, and R. L. Hyson, J. Neurosci. 2, 698 (992). [8] A. D. Reyes, E. W. Rubel, and W. J. Spain, J. Neurosci. 4, 5352 (994). [9] A. D. Reyes, E. W. Rubel, and W. J. Spain, J. Neurosci. 6, 993 (996). [] H. Markram, J. Lubke, M. Frotscher, and B. Sakmann, preprint (996) 6