METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Similar documents
WESTERN MICHIGAN UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

Evaluating Progress in Behavioral Programs for Children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders: Continuous Versus Intermittent Data Collection

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

Amanda M. Karsten (née Firth), Ph.D., BCBA-D 1215 Wilbraham Road, Mailbox S-5026 Springfield, MA (413)

National University of Ireland, Maynooth

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

3/20/2018. Agenda. This presentation is based on the collaborative work by staff at The New England Center for Children

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

The Role of Joint Control in Teaching Complex Behavior to Children with Autism

Using a Behavioral Skills Approach to Train a Mother to Teach Basic Learning and Self-Help Skills to Her Daughter Diagnosed with Rett Syndrome

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

The Effect of Varying Teacher Presentation Rates on Responding During Discrete Trial Training for Two Children With Autism

Sarah C. Wymer, Jonathan Tarbox, Gracie A. Beavers & Christopher A. Tullis

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

Generalization across Modalities of Expressive and Receptive Language. Acquisition through the Use of Multiple Exemplar Training and Errorless

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

A Suggested Methodology for the Application of Parent Training in Teaching Verbal Behavior to Children with Autism

MAKING IT WORK AS AN ABA THERAPIST

TEACHING CHILDREN WITH AUTISM TO TELL SOCIALLY APPROPRIATE LIES RYAN BERGSTROM AND ADEL C. NAJDOWSKI MARISELA ALVARADO JONATHAN TARBOX

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

The Effects of Multiple-Tact and Receptive-Discrimination Training on the Acquisition of Intraverbal Behavior

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Acquisition of Picture Exchange-Based vs. Signed Mands and Implications to Teach Functional Communication Skills to Children with Autism

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

---EBP Brief Packet---

GCAP Final Project Letter of Intent. Intervening with Autism: Linking Theory to Practice: Margaret R Davidson

Cultivating Motivation During Natural Environment Teaching and Group Instruction for Diverse Learners. Pam Salerno Aja Weston Michael Houck

The Effect of Manual Sign Mand Training and Prompt Delay on the Acquisition. of Vocal-Verbal Behavior by a Two-Year-Old with PDD-NOS

Story Time Language Program Summary Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

Abstract. Authors. Shauna Whiteford, Ksusha Blacklock, Adrienne Perry. Correspondence. Keywords

Impact of an Instructional Manual on the Implementation of ABA Teaching Procedures by Parents of Children With Angelman Syndrome

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY

Improving Education and Treatment in Autism with Organizational Behavior Management 2009 National Autism Conference, August 3, 2009

CURRICULUM VITAE January 2008

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND KRIS M. KEENEY GWINNETT COUNTY PUBLIC SCHOOLS, GEORGIA

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Implementing Discrete Trial Teaching

Teaching Staff Who Work With Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders to Evaluate the Treatment Integrity of Discrete-Trials Teaching Sessions

Training New Instructors to Implement Discrete Trial Teaching Strategies With Children With Autism in a Community-Based Intervention Program

Teaching Language to Young Children. Functional Analysis and Intervention

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010

The Effects of Precision Teaching With Textual or Tact Relations on Intraverbal Relations

Acquisition and Generalization of Tacts across Stimulus Modes in Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder

A Comparison of a Matrix Programming and Standard Discrete Trial Training Format to Teach Two-Component Tacts


International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

e-newsletter ABA Literature Summary topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION ...

An Examination of A Reading Comprehension Intervention in Secondary Students with ASD: CSR HS Pilot Year 2

Behavioral Language Interventions for Children with Autism: Comparing Applied Verbal Behavior and Naturalistic Teaching Approaches

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

Staff Development Day 2013

The Effects of Varying Levels of Procedural Integrity during Prompting. on Conditional Discrimination Performance. A Thesis Presented.

Comparing the Teaching Interaction Procedure to Social Stories: A Replication Study

Autism Spectrum Disorders

Autism Spectrum Disorders

The effect of stimulus choice on a discrimination task: pictures vs. objects

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

Evaluating Picture Exchange and the ipad as a Speech Generating Device to Teach Communication to Young Children with Autism

Effective Interventions for Students with ASD: Practical Applications for Classroom Success. Seminar Two

Effective Interventions for Students with ASD: Practical Applications for Classroom Success. Seminar Two. Objectives

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Apprenticeship in Applied Behaviour Analysis: Behaviour Innovations Revised February 2015

Playing to Learn: START. Creative Teaching Ideas to Enhance Social Validity of ABA Therapy for Young Learners. Please go to Menti.

A SORTING-TO-MATCHING METHOD TO TEACH COMPOUND MATCHING TO SAMPLE RACHEL S. FARBER

Using Pivotal Response Training with Peers in Special Education to Facilitate Play in Two Children with Autism

Examining the Effects of Lower Intensity ABA Therapy on Meaningful Progress with Clients Diagnosed with Autism. Wendy Nebbia M.Ed.

Evaluation of Real-time Feedback to Train Caregivers to Conduct Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions

Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference

An Investigation of Stimulus Pairing and Listener Training to Establish Emergent Intraverbals in Children with Autism

TRANSFER OF MAND TOPOGRAPHIES TO TACT RELATIONS AND VICE VERSA IN TWO VOCAL-VERBAL CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Jill E. Castellani, B.A.

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

Remediation of Prompt Dependence to Promote Independent Skill Acquisition for Children Clinically Diagnosed With Autism Spectrum Disorder

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1

Association for Behavior Analysis-International

Title: Stimulus Pairing Training in. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Kosuke Takahashi a. Jun ichi Yamamoto b

An Evaluation of Mand and Tact Assessment Procedures

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Behavioral Development Bulletin. Mary E. McDonald 1, Sharon A. Reeve 2, and Erin J. Sparacio 3. Volume 19 Number 1 March 2014

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 42, 827 832 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2009) COMPARISON OF DATA-COLLECTION METHODS IN A BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTION PROGRAM FOR CHILDREN WITH PERVASIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS: A REPLICATION ADEL C. NAJDOWSKI, VARDUI CHILINGARYAN, RYAN BERGSTROM, DOREEN GRANPEESHEH, SUSIE BALASANYAN, BARBARA AGUILAR, AND JONATHAN TARBOX CENTER FOR AUTISM AND RELATED DISORDERS Cummings and Carr (2009) compared two methods of data collection in a behavioral intervention program for children with pervasive developmental disorders: collecting data on all trials versus only the first trial in a session. Results showed that basing a child s progress on firsttrial data resulted in identifying mastery-level responding slightly sooner, whereas determining mastery based on all trials resulted in slightly better skill maintenance. In the current replication, no such differences in indication of mastery or maintenance were observed when data were collected on all trials or the first trial. DESCRIPTORS: autism, data collection, discrete trials, interspersed trials, massed trials We thank Amy Caveney, Deidra King, Ellen Kong, Romelea Manucal, Kelly Slease, Betty Tia, Richard Del Pilar, Wendy Jacobo, Sarah Neihoff, and Wendy Sanchez for their assistance with this project. Address correspondence to Adel C. Najdowski, Center for Autism and Related Disorders, 19019 Ventura Blvd., Suite 300, Tarzana, California 91356 (e-mail: a.najdowski@ centerforautism.com). doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-827 One of the defining features of applied behavior analysis is the improvement of behavior in meaningful and socially important ways (Baer, Wolf, & Risley, 1968). The primary method for assessing behavior change is through repeated data collection. Thus, behavior analysts who deliver behavioral intervention programs to children with autism have often used continuous data-collection methods in which data are collected on each learning trial. However, in an attempt to decrease time spent collecting data, clinicians have recently begun to collect data on the first trial within a block of trials (cf. Dollins & Carbone, 2003; Sundberg & Hale, 2003). To date, one study has attempted to examine empirically the differences between all-trials and first-trial data-collection methods. Cummings and Carr (2009) collected data on either the first of 10 acquisition trials or on all 10 acquisition trials during discrete-trial instruction for 6 children with pervasive developmental disabilities. Results suggested that basing a child s progress on first-trial data led to determination that the child had met mastery levels sooner than basing progress on data that were collected on all 10 trials. By contrast, using data from all 10 trials better identified skill maintenance (i.e., retention of skill performance following initial mastery). A potential limitation of the methods used by Cummings and Carr (2009) was that data collection did not continue for the remaining nine trials in the first-trial data-collection procedure. Thus, it was not possible to determine if an all-trials data-collection procedure would have agreed with conclusions regarding the number of sessions to mastery (within targets) using the first-trial procedure. The current study replicated procedures described by Cummings and Carr but also collected data on the remaining nine trials in the first-trial data-collection procedure to compare whether the two methods of analyzing data would yield differential identification of skill mastery and maintenance. METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent 827

828 ADEL C. NAJDOWSKI et al. diagnosis of autism or pervasive developmental disorder (not otherwise specified) between the ages of 3 and 11 years. Participants were receiving 5 to 38 hr (M 5 20 hr) of one-onone behavioral intervention and had been receiving discrete-trial instruction for a mean of 13.2 months (range, 1 to 39 months). One participant primarily communicated via a picture exchange communication system, 3 communicated with one- to two-word utterances, and 7 communicated in full sentences. Therapists were individuals who typically implemented behavioral interventions with the participants, and who had provided such treatment to children with autism for a mean of 50.4 months (range, 17 to 144 months). All therapists were enrolled in a MA program in behavior analysis at the time of the study. Sessions were conducted in each participant s therapy room in his or her home or in the clinic. Data Collection and Interobserver Agreement The principal measure of participant behavior was the correct emission of each targeted skill. The skills that were targeted for intervention depended on each child s curricular needs across three areas (i.e., language, social skills, and academic skills) and four operant classes (i.e., imitation, receptive identification, tacts, and intraverbals). (A list of specific skills targeted for all participants is available from the first author.) A correct response was defined as the participant emitting the target skill within 3 s of the presentation of the initial instruction; emission of a response following any additional prompting was not scored as a correct response. Each session consisted of data collection on 10 acquisition trials. During some phases of teaching, interspersed trials of previously mastered skills were conducted, but data were not collected during these trials. Using the all-trials data-collection method, correct responses were converted to a percentage after dividing the number of acquisition trials scored as correct by the total number of acquisition trials. For the first-trial data-collection method, correct responses were converted to a percentage using the score on the first acquisition trial only (a 0 if incorrect and a 1 if correct). One datacollection method was randomly assigned to each of the participant s acquisition targets to evaluate differences in the identification of mastery across the two data-collection procedures. The mastery criterion was defined as three consecutive sessions above 80% correct responding (all-trials condition) or three consecutive sessions of 100% correct responding (first-trial condition). Performance of the target skills yielded four dependent variables to evaluate the two datacollection procedures: (a) the number of sessions to mastery; (b) the mean percentage of correct responses during maintenance probes; (c) agreement between data-collection methods regarding identification of mastery, defined as the percentage of targets in which the two methods agreed on the number of sessions to mastery; and (d) the percentage of targets in which mastery would have been identified sooner had the alternative data-collection procedure been used, which was assessed by summing the number of target behaviors for which use of the alternative data-collection method s criterion would have resulted in a decision of mastery in fewer sessions. Interobserver agreement was assessed by having a second observer independently collect data on 35% of sessions. Interobserver agreement scores were calculated by dividing agreements on correct responding by agreements plus disagreements and converting the resulting quotient to a percentage for each 10-trial session. Across participants, interobserver agreement for correct responses was 99% (range, 83% to 100%). Procedure Each participant received discrete-trial instruction for four target skills, 1 to 5 days per week. Two target skills were from one curricular lesson (Training Set 1), and two target skills were from

DATA COLLECTION 829 a different curricular lesson (Training Set 2). At the inception of the study, one target skill from each training set was randomly paired with the all-trials condition, and the other target skill from each training set was randomly paired with the first-trial condition. Each day, one to four target skills were assessed one to three times each. For each target skill, the experiment proceeded in the following phases for 10 of the 11 participants: baseline, massed trials, interspersed trials, and maintenance (procedural details may be obtained from the first author). The massedtrials phase was omitted for 1 participant. One baseline session was conducted per target skill, in which 10 trials of that skill were interspersed with previously mastered skills (for every acquisition trial, there were approximately one to two interspersed trials). For each trial, the instruction was vocally presented, followed by a 3-s intertrial interval. No differential consequences were arranged for the emission of a target skill, whereas correct emission of previously mastered skills resulted in brief praise. Massed-trials sessions consisted of 10 consecutive trials in which the therapist presented only the target skill, using a most-to-least intrusive prompt hierarchy (Riley & Heaton, 2000). Correct emission of the target skill following the initial instruction resulted in praise and brief (e.g., 5 s) access to stimuli that had been previously identified as preferred during brief preference assessments (DeLeon et al., 2001). Acquisition target skills progressed to the interspersed-trials phase after two to three sessions above 80% correct responding (alltrials condition) or 100% correct responding (first-trial condition). The interspersed-trials phase was conducted to ensure that participants were able to respond to acquisition stimuli in the presence of distracter stimuli (i.e., previously mastered skills). During this phase, the therapist interspersed 10 trials of the target skill with one to two trials of previously mastered skills. Correct responding during acquisition trials produced praise and brief (e.g., 5 s) access to a preferred stimulus. This phase continued until the respective mastery criteria were achieved. Thereafter, the therapist conducted maintenance probes for each target skill once per week for 3 weeks. Each maintenance probe consisted of 10 trials of the target skill that were interspersed with previously mastered skills. The therapist provided praise for correct responses (for targeted and mastered skills); all other responses produced no differential social consequences. Experimental Design and Data Analysis An alternating treatments design was used to compare the number of sessions to mastery and maintenance for each participant s two training sets across the two data-collection procedures. However, for the purpose of evaluating differences in the four dependent measures across the first-trial and all-trials data-collection methods, data were summarized across all participants for the respective training sets. Session-by-session data are available from the first author. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Figure 1 (top) depicts the number of sessions to mastery across participants. Using the alltrials and first-trial methods, the mean number of sessions to mastery were 7.45 and 7.77, respectively, indicating little difference between the two methods. Figure 1 (middle) depicts the mean percentage of correct responding on the target skills during the maintenance probes for all participants in the all-trials and first-trial methods, yielding 95% and 97%, respectively, again indicating no differentiation between the two methods. Figure 1 (bottom) depicts the percentage of target skills for which (a) there was agreement across both methods on the number of sessions required to observed mastery level performance (39%), (b) mastery would have been identified sooner had the alltrials method been used (43%), and (c) mastery would have been identified sooner had the firsttrial method been used (18%).

830 ADEL C. NAJDOWSKI et al. Figure 1. Mean number of sessions to mastery (top) and mean percentage of correct responses during maintenance probes (middle) for all-trials and first-trial data-collection methods across all participants. Percentage of acquisition targets in which there was agreement between all-trials and first-trial data collection and in which mastery would have been achieved sooner had the alternative data-collection method been used (bottom).

DATA COLLECTION 831 The current results suggest that there was little difference in terms of the number of sessions in which a participant displayed mastery performance and in the percentage of correct responding during maintenance probes using either the all-trials or the first-trial datacollection method. These results differ slightly from those obtained by Cummings and Carr (2009), who noted that mastery was identified in fewer sessions using the first-trial method. However, it should be mentioned that data collection continued on the remaining nine trials during the first-trial condition in the current investigation. Thus, the current analysis also compared the two methods within each target skill. This additional analysis allowed us to see that, had the alternative data-collection method been used to identify mastery for each target, mastery would have been suggested earlier in more cases when the all-trials method was applied (43% of targets) than when the first-trial method was applied (18% of targets). Thus, the first-trial method was a slightly more conservative measure of length of time to achieve mastery-level performance. Despite this finding, the current results should be interpreted with caution because they might have been an artifact of using a mastery criterion of greater than 80% during the alltrials condition. Given that this mastery criterion is less stringent than the 100% criterion used for the first-trial condition, it is not surprising that mastery would be suggested sooner in more cases using a lower criterion level. The above 80% criterion was used in the current investigation because it has been used to evaluate response mastery (Anderson, Taras, & Cannon, 1996). Nevertheless, future research could compare the identification of masterylevel performance using ranges of criterion levels (e.g., comparing mastery at 80%, 90%, and 100% during all trials to 100% during the first trial) to determine the impact of different criterion levels on evidence of mastery. Future research should also evaluate the extent to which data collection on all trials or only a subset of trials decreases time requirements associated with the implementation of discrete-trial instruction. Considering the results of this study combined with the results reported by Cummings and Carr (2009), it appears that first-trial data collection might be a promising option for assessing behavior change during discrete-trial instruction. However, additional research is needed to evaluate the utility of this method. Specifically, all-trials data collection may have potential advantages in that it may help supervisors to monitor therapist behavior during sessions (i.e., procedural integrity). All-trials data collection also may help supervisors to identify errors related to prompts, presentation of stimuli, and movement through teaching phases, and to monitor how many trials are implemented. Several limitations warrant discussion. First, no procedural integrity data were collected on implementation of discrete-trial instruction; however, the therapists in the current study were evaluated regularly for procedural integrity according to standards that have been described elsewhere (e.g., Crockett, Fleming, & Doepke, 2007; LeBlanc, Ricciardi, & Luiselli, 2005). Second, only one baseline session was conducted prior to treatment. However, all participants responded 0% to 40% correct during baseline, indicating inadequate responding. Third, the current participants may not be representative of the population of children with pervasive developmental disorders (e.g., several of the current participants had relatively well-developed verbal behavior repertoires). Thus, it is unknown if there would be a difference between data-collection methods with children who do not share these characteristics. REFERENCES Anderson, S. R., Taras, M., & Cannon, B. O. (1996). Teaching new skills to children with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & S. C. Luce (Eds.), Behavioral intervention for children with autism: A manual for parents and professionals (p. 192). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

832 ADEL C. NAJDOWSKI et al. Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91 97. Crockett, J. L., Fleming, R. K., & Doepke, K. J. (2007). Parent training: Acquisition and generalization of discrete trials teaching skills with parents of children with autism. Developmental Disabilities, 28(1), 23 36. Cummings, A. R., & Carr, J. E. (2009). Evaluating progress in behavioral programs for children with autism spectrum disorders via continuous and discontinuous measurement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 57 71. DeLeon, I. G., Fisher, W. W., Rodriguez-Catter, V., Maglieri, K., Herman, K., & Marhefka, J. (2001). Examination of relative reinforcement effects of stimuli identified through pretreatment and daily brief preference assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 463 473. Dollins, P., & Carbone, V. J. (2003, May). Using probe data recording methods to assess learner acquisition of skills. In V. J. Carbone (Chair), Research related to Skinner s analysis of verbal behavior with children with autism. Symposium conducted at the 29th annual convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco. LeBlanc, M. P., Ricciardi, J. N., & Luiselli, J. K. (2005). Improving discrete trial instruction by paraprofessional staff through an abbreviated performance feedback intervention. Education and Treatment of Children, 28(1), 76 82. Riley, G. A., & Heaton, S. (2000). Guidelines for the selection of a method of fading cues. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10(2), 133 149. Sundberg, M. L., & Hale, L. (2003, May). Using textual stimuli to teach vocal-intraverbal behaviors. In A. I. Petursdottir (Chair), Methods for teaching intraverbal behavior to children. Symposium conducted at the 29th annual convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco. Received March 3, 2008 Final acceptance August 25, 2008 Action Editor, Henry Roane