Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements

Similar documents
1 Running head: Strength and gait perception. Sex difference in attractiveness perceptions. of strong and weak male walkers

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

Differential Viewing Strategies towards Attractive and Unattractive Human Faces

When facial attractiveness is only skin deep

I Can Dance: Further Investigations of the Effect of Dancing Ability on Mate Value

Judging body weight from faces: The height ^ weight illusion

Jillian J. M. O Connor

PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE

Sexually Selective Cognition: Beauty Captures the Mind of the Beholder

Adaptive allocation of attention: effects of sex and sociosexuality on visual attention to attractive opposite-sex faces

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men

This paper is in press (Psychological Science) Mona Lisa s Smile Perception or Deception?

JAIME M. CLOUD Ph.D. University of Texas at Austin Individual Differences and Evolutionary Psychology

1 Short title: Strength and personality. Handgrip Strength and the Big Five Personality Factors in Men and Women

Intersexual Competition

JAIME M. CLOUD Ph.D. University of Texas at Austin Individual Differences and Evolutionary Psychology

Virtual Shapers & Movers: Form and Motion affect Sex Perception

Facial symmetry and the Ôbig-fiveÕ personality factors

The disengagement of attentive resources from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional. infidelity

Received January 19, 2004; revision received March 23, 2004; accepted April 8, 2004

Arianne Elmy November 28, Final Project Summary

Evolutionary Psychology

Do Women Pretend Orgasm to Retain a Mate?

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

David J. Lick and Kerri L. Johnson University of California, Los Angeles. Simone V. Gill Boston University

Jealousy: Unconscious processes

Female Condition Influences Preferences for Sexual Dimorphism in Faces of Male Humans (Homo sapiens)

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts EFFECTS OF ACOUSTICAL MANIPULATIONS ON PERCEPTIONS OF

BIRTH CONTROL, KISSING, AND ASSESSMENT OF MATE QULAITY 1. Running head: BIRTH CONTROL, KISSING, AND ASSESSMENT OF MATE

Alec Beall, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada. (Corresponding author).

Women s Oral Sex Behaviors and Risk of Partner Infidelity. Michael N. Pham, Todd K. Shackelford, and Yael Sela. Oakland University

Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, Michigan 3 Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, Michigan

SHORT REPORT Facial features influence the categorization of female sexual orientation

The Relationship between Objective Sperm Competition Risk and Men s Copulatory Interest is. Moderated by Partner s Time Spent with Other Men

Sensation seeking and men's face preferences

SEX-DIFFERENCES IN PREFERENCE FOR LOOKING AT THE FACE OR BODY IN SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM MATING CONTEXTS

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Sex-Specificity in the Reward Value of Facial Attractiveness. Amanda C Hahn 1, Lisa M DeBruine 1, Benedict C Jones 1. Corresponding author

Why Sex? Mating. Disadvantages of Sex. Advantages of Sex. What Would We Expect? Sex Differences in Parental Investment

The Influence of Women s Self-Esteem on Mating Decision Making

Facial Perceptions: Age s influence on People s Perceptions of Facial Modifications

Running head: MATE RETENTION STRATEGIES p. 1. Solving the Problem of Partner Infidelity:

Partner Status Influences Women s Interest in the Opposite Sex

Personality and Individual Differences

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

MENSTRUAL INFLUENCES ON PERSON PERCEPTION: MALE SENSITIVITY TO FLUCTUATING FEMALE FERTILITY

Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression

The impact of visual exposure to a physically attractive other on self-presentation

Swagger, Sway, and Sexuality: Judging Sexual Orientation From Body Motion and Morphology

Running head: RESOURCE SCARCITY & RACE 1. Does this Recession Make Me Look Black? The Effect of Resource Scarcity on the

UNCORRECTED PROOF ARTICLE IN PRESS

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

EXAMINING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ORGANIZATIONAL JUSTICE AND EFFECTIVENESS OF STRATEGY IMPLEMENTATION AT FOOD INDUSTRIES IN ARDABIL PROVINCE

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective

Can t Take My Eyes off You: Attentional Adhesion to Mates and Rivals

Sex-specific relationship between digit ratio (2D:4D) and romantic jealousy. The Netherlands

CURRICULUM VITAE. Department of Psychology, Oakland University 654 Pioneer Dr., Rochester, MI

Sex Ratio and Mate Preferences: A Cross-Cultural Investigation. Emily A. Stone and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University. David M.

How Does the Gender of Spokesperson Affect the Audience s Perception

Gaze Properties of Women Judging the Attractiveness of Masculine and Feminine Male Faces

Is Cunnilingus-Assisted Orgasm a Male Sperm-Retention Strategy?

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplemental Material. Romance, Risk, and Replication: Nisha Hickin, Amanda J. F. Tamman, and Lara M. C. Puhlmann

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

EDUCATIONAL CAREGIVERS PAIN PERCEPTION 1

COMPONENTS OF SELF-PERCEIVED MATE VALUE

It s Not All about Her: Men s Mate Value and Mate Retention. Emily J. Miner. Florida Atlantic University. Valerie G. Starratt

The Effect of Facial Attractiveness on Recognition Memory

Back to the USSR: How Colors Might Shape the Political Perception of East versus West

British Journal of Psychology (2010), 00, 1 13 q 2010 The British Psychological Society

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Final Exam PS 217, Fall 2010

The Mere Presence of Opposite-Sex Others on Judgments of Sexual and Romantic Desirability: Opposite Effects for Men and Women

Sex ratio and mate preferences: A cross-cultural investigation

Motivation IV Sexual Motivation Sexual Reproduction Reproduction is necessary for the survival of the species. Some organisms (e.g., bacteria) reprodu

Hungry Men Prefer More Mature Women: A Field Test of the Environmental Security Hypothesis

Cognition xxx (2011) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

No prior entry for threat-related faces: Evidence from temporal order judgments


Goal-Driven Cognition and Functional Behavior: The Fundamental-Motives Framework

Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES IN EMOTION JUDGMENT

Discrimination of Attractiveness and Health in Men s Faces: the Impact of Color Cues and Variation in Relation to Sex and Age of Rater

Exploring the Neural Substrates of Self-Ownership and Memory. Dave Turk University of Aberdeen

The human body has long been a source of inspiration for

Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box. Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Author's personal copy

Physical Attractiveness as a Persuasive Tactic - 1. Exploiting the Beauty in the Eye of the Beholder:

Morbid Jealousy from an Evolutionary Psychological Perspective JUDITH A. EASTON. University of Texas at Austin LUCAS D. SCHIPPER TODD K.

Assortative mating for perceived facial personality traits

Manuscript submitted to Evolutionary Psychology

Evolution and Human Behavior

The Role of Stimulus Specificity on Infidelity Reactions: Seeing is Disturbing

Disjunction Effect in Prisoners Dilemma: Evidences from an Eye-tracking Study

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

Affective Objectification as the Perception of Intent To Be or Have an Object. Abstract. Author Summary. Introduction.

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Transcription:

1 [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, May 2016] Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements Susanne Röder 1,2*, Claus-Christian Carbon 1,2,3, Todd K. Shackelford 4, Katarzyna Pisanski 5, Bettina Weege 6, Bernhard Fink 6 1 Department of General Psychology and Methodology, University of Bamberg, Germany; 2 Forschungsgruppe EPÆG (Ergonomie, Psychologische Æsthetik, Gestalt), Bamberg, Germany; 3 Bamberg Graduate School of Affective and Cognitive Sciences (BaGrACS), Bamberg, Germany; 4 Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, MI, USA; 5 Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Poland; 6 Institute of Psychology and Courant Research Center Evolution of Social Behavior, University of Göttingen, Germany. Word count: 2972 *Corresponding author: Susanne Röder, Department of General Psychology and Methodology, University of Bamberg, Markusplatz 3, D-96047 Bamberg, Germany, Phone: ++49 951 863 1961, Fax: ++49 951 601 511, email: susanne.roeder@unibamberg.de

2 Abstract Sexual selection may have shaped male visual sensitivity to characteristics that provide information about female mate quality. Indeed, men judge certain facial and bodily configurations of women to be attractive, possibly because those configurations signal health and fertility. Most of this evidence derives from the study of women s facial and body photographs. We tested the hypothesis that attractive female dancers receive greater visual attention from men than do unattractive dancers. Twenty-nine men viewed video pairs of pre-categorized high and low attractive female dancers. Their eye gaze was tracked and they also provided ratings of attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony. High attractive dancers received greater visual attention than did low attractive dancers and men s visual attention correlated positively with their judgments of attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony. We discuss our findings in the context of the beauty captures the mind of the beholder hypothesis and the role of dance movements in human mate selection. Keywords: body movement, dance attractiveness, visual attention, eye tracking, dance movement harmony.

3 Highlights Men s visual attention to female dance movements Men spend higher visual attention to dance movements of attractive female dancers Men judged attractive dancer higher on femininity and dance movement harmony Men s visual attention correlates positively with attractiveness assessments

4 1. Introduction Men across cultures prioritize health and youth in a prospective opposite-sex partner and judge these characteristics as attractive, possibly because these traits indicate mate quality (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). Studies investigating men s attractiveness perceptions of women have primarily used facial and body photographs, but recent research suggests that women s body movements may also be associated with similar mate quality information (Fink, Weege, Neave, Pham & Shackelford, 2015). Grammer and colleagues (2003) reported that men judged women s gait recorded during high fertility as more attractive than gait recorded during low fertility. Miller, Tybur, and Jordan (2007) found that female lap dancers received higher tip earnings during high fertility days than during low fertility days, and Fink, Hugill, and Lange (2012) showed, that men judged female dancers as more attractive on fertile (relative to non-fertile) cycle days. These findings suggest that men are sensitive to cyclic variations in women s body movements, which in turn affect their attractiveness assessments. Additional evidence is provided by research investigating the effect of mating context on men s perception of women s dance movements. Röder, Weege, Carbon, Shackelford, and Fink (2015) found that high attractive female dancers were rated as more promiscuous than low attractive dancers, especially when male judges were instructed to assess these women as short-term sexual partners. High attractive dancers were rated higher on dance movement harmony and as healthier than their less attractive counterparts. Specifically, men s promiscuity judgments of female dancers predicted ratings of the dancers attractiveness as a short-term sexual partner, whereas movement harmony judgments predicted ratings of dancers attractiveness as a long-term partner.

5 These findings suggest that context-related differences in men's perceptions of women's dance attractiveness exist and may be produced by adaptations that motivate increased attraction toward healthy and fertile women. Eye-tracking research has shown that certain physical characteristics capture men s visual attention and that men look longer and more often at female faces (Maner, DeWall & Gailliot, 2008) and bodies (Dixson, Grimshaw, Linklater & Dixson, 2009) that they consider attractive. Thus, researchers suggested that mating-related motives may guide selective visual attention to and processing of attractive and unattractive faces ( beauty captures the mind of the beholder hypothesis: Maner, Kenrick, Becker, Delton, Hofer, Wilbur & Neuberg, 2003). Weege, Lange, and Fink (2012) found that women devoted greater visual attention to dance movements of men they judged as more attractive. They concluded that a cognitive bias towards attractiveness, similar to that proposed for face perceptions (Maner et al., 2003), may exist for women s perception of men s body movements. Here, we investigated men s visual attention to and assessments of women s dance movements and hypothesized that dance movements of high attractive female dancers would receive greater visual attention than movements of low attractive dancers. Moreover, we expected positive relationships between measures of men s visual attention and their assessments of attractiveness, femininity, and movement harmony of these same female dancers. 2. Materials and methods 2.1 Stimuli

6 Our stimuli comprised 10 dance characters, selected from a set created as part of a larger-scale project on human body movement (e.g., Hufschmidt, Weege, Röder, Pisanski, Neave & Fink, 2015; Fink, Weege, Neave, Ried & do Lago, 2014; Fink et al., 2015; Weege, Pham, Shackelford & Fink, 2015). Dance movements of 84 British women aged 18 to 41 years (M = 20.6 years, SD = 3.8) were collected using 3D-optical motion capture technology (Vicon, Oxford, UK) and applied to a shape-standardized, featureless, gender-neutral, humanoid character using Autodesk MotionBuilder (Autodesk Inc., San Rafael, CA, USA). A 10-second sequence was extracted from the middle of each dance recording and rendered into a video with a resolution of 784 x 640 pixels at a frame rate of 24 fps. In a pre-study, 49 heterosexual men aged 19 to 30 years (M = 23.7 years, SD = 3.8) judged the attractiveness of the 84 dance characters on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = very unattractive, 7 = very attractive) using MediaLab software (Empirisoft Inc., New York, USA). On the basis of mean attractiveness ratings, the five most attractive and the five least attractive dancers were selected for presentation in the main study. Attractiveness ratings differed significantly between the two sets (high attractive: M = 5.03, SD = 0.17; low attractive: M = 1.92, SD = 0.14; independent samples t-test, one-tailed t(8) = 32.02, p <.001, d = 20.18). 2.2 Participants Twenty nine men, aged 20 to 39 years (M = 24.4 years, SD = 4.7) were recruited mainly from the local university campus. Participants completed a standard Snellen eye

chart test confirming good visual acuity. They provided written consent and were debriefed following both tasks. 7 2.3 Procedure A set of 25 video pairs was created by combining one video from the group of high attractive dancers with one video from the group of low attractive dancers, counterbalanced with regard to the side of presentation. Video pairs were presented on a 22 monitor (1680 x 1050 pixels resolution) at a size of 1280 x 1024 pixels. For each video pair, two areas of interest (AOIs) were defined (SR ExperimentBuilder software, SR Research, Canada), covering the entire size of the video of the high attractive and the low attractive dancer, respectively (see for a similar approach Weege et al., 2012). Within these AOIs, visual attention was measured as cumulative dwell time (in ms) and number of fixations (EyeLink 1000, SR Research, Canada). Subsequent to an automatic calibration sequence the 25 video pairs were presented to participants in randomized order. Prior to each video pair, a blank screen (10 s) and a fixation cross (1.5 s) were presented to ensure a constant starting position of gaze. After completing the eye-tracking task, participants rated the dancers on attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all attractive/feminine/harmonic, 7 = very attractive/feminine/harmonic). Videos were presented individually, blocked by attribute and in a randomized order. Means of visual attention measures and attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony ratings were calculated for the five high and the five low attractive dancers separately (descriptive statistics Table 1). To ascertain differences in visual

8 attention and ratings between high and low attractive dancers, difference scores were calculated by subtracting the means of high attractive dancers from those of low attractive dancers, and tested against zero (i.e., the assumption of no significant difference between the groups). All statistical tests were performed one-tailed and with an error level set to.05. 3. Results One-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov goodness of fit tests indicated that the difference scores were normally distributed (Zs(29).10, ps.16, n.s.). Cronbach s alpha coefficients for men s attractiveness, femininity, and harmony judgments of female dancers were all.98. One-sample t-tests revealed a significant result for dwell time (t(28) = 8.72, p <.001, d = 2.96) and number of fixations (t(28) = 8.39, p <.001, d = 3.17). We also found significant effects for ratings of attractiveness (t(28) = 13.53, p <.001, d = 5.11), femininity (t(28) = 17.21, p <.001, d = 6.50), and dance movement harmony (t(28) = 10.40, p <.001, d = 3.93). Thus, men devoted greater visual attention to high attractive dancers and judged them higher on attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony, compared to low attractive dancers. Men s visual attention correlated positively with their ratings of attractiveness (dwell time: r(27) =.35, p <.05; number of fixations: r(27) =.45, p <.01) and dance movement harmony (dwell time: r(27) =.41, p <.05; number of fixations: r(27) =.44, p <.01). Ratings of femininity correlated positively with number of fixations (r(27) =.32, p <.05) and dwell

9 time (r(27) =.21, p =.14), although the latter correlation did not reach statistical significance. Dwell time and number of fixations correlated positively (r(27) =.81, p <.001). --- Insert Table 1 about here --- 4. Discussion Our results show that men devote more visual attention (looked longer and more frequently) to pre-categorized attractive female dancers compared to those precategorized as less attractive, and judge them higher on attractiveness, femininity and dance movement harmony (although the relationship between dwell time and femininity judgments did not reach statistical significance). This suggests that dance movements of high attractive female dancers differ in certain properties from those of low attractive dancers, and men are not only visually attracted to the moves of high attractive dancers, but also judge them more positively. These findings sit comfortably alongside other research suggesting that body movements convey certain quality information that influences men s and women s mate preferences. Johnson and colleagues (2007) reported that gender-atypical gait movements affect perceptions of sexual orientation of women and, the accuracy of sexual orientation assessments. Computer-generated walkers with lower waist-to-hip ratios and displaying more pronounced hip sways were categorized as heterosexual women, whereas the same walkers displaying shoulder swaggers were categorized as homosexual women. Thus, body movement seems to contain gender-typical cues that vary within and between the sexes. Hufschmidt et al. (2015) presented virtual characters, animated with the dance movements of men and women to children and adults. Although gender-identification performance was higher than expected by chance for both groups,

10 physical strength predicted performance only in adults, suggesting that information about a sexually dimorphic feature (strength) is also conveyed through dance movements. Women s dance movements may signal properties of mate quality, and we speculate that it is primarily those qualities predicted by the Sexual Strategies Theory (Buss & Schmitt, 1993), i.e., fertility and health. Research on ovulatory-cycle dependent variation in men s responses to women s body movements provides support for the notion of a relationship between women s body movements and fertility (Miller et al., 2007; Fink et al., 2012). With regard to health, the situation is less clear. However, we reported previously that movement harmony judgments of women s dances correlated with health perceptions (Röder et al., 2015). Moreover, perceived dance movement harmony predicted attractiveness ratings, especially in a long-term mating context, whereas promiscuity ratings predicted the short-term attractiveness of dancers. We found that men devote greater visual attention to female dancers whose body movements they judge as more harmonic. Although male observers seem to share a certain taste in their preferences, it is not clear which characteristics of female body movements elicit judgments of dance movement harmony. Perhaps this assessment captures aspects of body movements that indicate physical health and emotional wellbeing (Hanna, 2006). To quantify female dance movements objectively through, for example, a kinematic/biomechanical analysis is an avenue for future research. This would enable researchers to disentangle aesthetic cues from sexual cues, as dance movements may convey both, and to investigate their relationships with health and wellbeing, in addition to sexual information that men derive from women s dance movements.

11 Previous research on female face and body attractiveness suggests that sexual selection may have shaped men s mate preferences and perceptual mechanisms to be sensitive to certain quality cues of a potential mate. Maner et al. (2003) demonstrated that observers were selectively attuned to physically attractive individuals. Men and women exhibited higher visual attention to attractive compared to unattractive faces of women, whereas only women showed this bias for male faces. Considering the results of the present study and those of Weege et al. (2012), we suggest that a cognitive bias in the perception of dance movements exists in both sexes and is informed by mating-related motives. What might be the evolutionary benefits of visual sensitivity and attention to sexspecific quality cues that men derive from female body movements? People seem to be quick in their initial assessment of what they consider attractive or unattractive (which is consistent with the results of cognitive as well as neurobiological studies), and look longer on what they evaluate as positive. Despite the short presentation time of each pair of dancers, it is likely that observers made a quick initial decision on the quality of the dancers body movements. However, whether humans rely on initial brain processes during mate selection, which may be biased in the form of selective attention to opposite sex-typical cues, remains an open question. An interesting question in this context is whether dance movements can be regarded as honest cues to an individual s mate quality, as has been proposed for faces and bodies (Thornhill & Grammer, 1999). If this were so, we would expect that the quality information men derive from women s body movements is reliable, in that body movement cues cannot be easily faked. Thus, dance movements should comprise developmental

12 information and be linked to an individual s health. In this view, attractive dances are displayed by individuals who can afford the production of a complex motor behavior such as dance, which requires the coordination of physical, biomechanical and neurological structures. Whether personal information conveyed through dance movements can be consciously altered to elicit a different response in observers (e.g., a more positive attractiveness assessment) has yet to be demonstrated. Acknowledgements This project was funded by the Internal Research Funding, University of Bamberg (FNK), grant number 02060257, awarded to S.R., and by the German Research Foundation (DFG), grant numbers FI 1450/4-1 and FI 1450/7-1, awarded to B.F.

13 5. References Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204-232. Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Linklater, W. L., & Dixson, A. F. (2009). Eyetracking of men s preferences for waist-to-hip ratio and breast size in women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 43-50. Fink, B., Hugill, N., & Lange, B. P. (2012). Women s body movements are a potential cue to ovulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 759-763. Fink, B., Weege, B., Neave, N., Ried, B., & do Lago, O.C. (2014). Female perception of male body movement. In V. Weekes-Shackelford & T.K. Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Sexuality and Behavior (pp. 299-324). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. Fink, B., Weege, B., Neave, N., Pham, M.N., & Shackelford, T.K. (2015). Integrating body movement into attractiveness research. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 220. Grammer, K., Keki, V., Striebel, B., Atzmueller, M., & Fink, B. (2003). Bodies in motion: A window to the soul? In: E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds), Evolutionary Aesthetics. Heidelberg, Springer. Hanna, J.L. (2006). Dancing for health. Conquering and preventing stress. Lanham, MD: AltaMira. Hufschmidt, C., Weege, B., Röder, S., Pisanski, K., Neave, N., & Fink, B. (2015). Physical strength and gender identification from dance movements. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 13-17.

14 Johnson, K. L., Gill, S., Reichman, V., & Tassinary, L. G. (2007). Swagger, sway, and sexuality: Judging sexual orientation from body motion and morphology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 321-334. Maner, J. K., DeWall, C. N., & Gailliot, M. T. (2008). Selective attention to signs of success: Social dominance and early stage interpersonal perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 488-501. Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Delton, A. W., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C., & Neuberg, S. (2003). Sexually selective cognition: Beauty captures the mind of the beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1107-1120. Miller, G., Tybur, J. M., & Jordan, B. D. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earning by lap dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 375-381. Röder, S., Weege, B., Carbon, C. C., Shackelford, T. K., & Fink, B. (2015). Men s perception of women s dance movements depends on mating context, but not men s sociosexual orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 172-175. Thornhill, R., & Grammer, K. (1999). The body and face of women: one ornament that signals quality? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 105-120. Weege, B., Lange, B., & Fink, B. (2012). Women s visual attention to variation in men s dance quality. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 236-240. Weege, B., Pham, M. N., Shackelford, T. K., & Fink, B. (2015). Physical strength and dance attractiveness: further evidence for an association in men, but not in women. American Journal of Human Biology, 27, 728-730.

15 Table Table 1: Descriptive statistics of men s visual attention toward and ratings of women s dance movements. High attractive dancers Low attractive dancers M SD M SD Dwell time (ms) 5,597.4 892.3 2,791.4 913.9 Number of fixations 10.94 2.24 6.67 1.91 Attractiveness 4.83 0.76 2.32 0.88 Femininity 5.32 0.75 2.34 0.74 Movement harmony 4.99 0.88 2.61 0.97 Note: All means (M) and standard deviations (SD) are based on n = 29 male observers.