Succumbing to instant gratification without the nucleus accumbens

Similar documents
Fronto-executive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Choosing delayed rewards: perspectives from learning theory, neurochemistry, Rudolf N. Cardinal, Trevor W. Robbins & Barry J.

Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort

Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Physical and Physiological Aspects of Drug Use and Abuse

The Contribution of Neuroscience to Understanding Human Behaviour

Implications of using rat models for ADHD research

Basic definition and Classification of Anhedonia. Preclinical and Clinical assessment of anhedonia.

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

Sponsored document from Clinical Psychology Review

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DELAY DISCOUNTING AND NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN RATS. Maggie M. Sweitzer. B.A., University of Akron, 2005

Local analysis of behaviour in the adjusting-delay task for assessing choice of delayed reinforcement

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP

FLORIDA INTERNATIONAL UNIVERSITY, MIAMI, FLORIDA B.A. in Psychology

The Role of Serotonin in the Regulation of Patience and Impulsivity

Title. Author(s)Takahashi, Taiki. CitationMedical Hypotheses, 65(4): Issue Date Doc URL. Type. File Information

Tobacco Addiction. Addiction Terminology. Jack E. Henningfield, PhD Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Pinney Associates

Chapter 5: General discussion

SPN102: Attention Deficit Disorders Handout

Similar Effects of the Selective Noradrenaline Reuptake Inhibitor Atomoxetine on Three Distinct Forms of Impulsivity in the Rat

Chapter 4. Role of the nucleus accumbens core and shell in Pavlovian instrumental transfer

Neurobiology of Addiction

DELAYED REWARD AND COST DISCOUNTING. JAMES G. MURPHY Auburn University. RUDY E. VUCHINICH and CATHY A. SIMPSON University of Alabama, Birmingham

Cortico-limbic-striatal circuits subserving different forms of cost benefit decision making

Mechanisms of impulsive choice: III. The role of reward processes

The orbitofrontal cortex, predicted value, and choice

Evidence-Based Treatment Approaches for Gambling Disorder

Joseph T. McGuire. Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences Boston University 677 Beacon St. Boston, MA

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

Computational Psychiatry: Tasmia Rahman Tumpa

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

By: Brian Zachariah M.D. PGY4 Lauren Derhodge D.O. Clinical Assistant Professor, Attending Physician

Academic year Lecture 16 Emotions LECTURE 16 EMOTIONS

The Neurobiology of Addiction

Interpersonal therapy adapted to borderline personality disorder: contributions to research in clinics and brain imaging

Effects of Drugs on the Brain and Behavior in Adolescents

Individual differences in decision-making

Still at the Choice-Point

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria

The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour

Serotonin Depletion Impairs Waiting but not Stop-Signal Reaction Time in Rats: Implications for Theories of the Role of 5-HT in Behavioral Inhibition

Modulation of the Neural Circuitry Underlying Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

ORIGINAL ARTICLES Two Models of Impulsivity: Relationship to Personality Traits and Psychopathology

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas

What are Substance Use Disorders?

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

Ratio and time requirements on operant schedules: effortrelated effects of nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions

Contrasting Roles of Basolateral Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex in Impulsive Choice

Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice

Top Peer Reviewed Journals Neuroscience & Behavior

25 Things To Know. Pre- frontal

Opioid Epidemic Update

Citation for published version (APA): Jónsdóttir, S. (2006). ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender. s.n.

Serotonin and the Evaluation of Future Rewards

EFFECTS OF ADHD ON EARLY LEARNING AND ACDEMIC PERFORMANCE 1

The neuroscience of impulsive and self-controlled decisions

Responses to DSM-5. DSM-5 and Malingering. DSM-5: Development and Implementation. Oxford Medicine Online

self-control in rats

BRAIN PLASTICITY: AGING AND NEUROPSYCHICATRIC DISORDERS:

TIME DISCOUNTING FOR PRIMARY AND MONETARY REWARDS *

Reinforcement learning and the brain: the problems we face all day. Reinforcement Learning in the brain

The National Methamphetamine Symposium

ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, PHYSICAL HEALTH, AND LIFESTYLE IN OLDER ADULTS

Behavioural Brain Research

Separate neural pathways process different decision costs

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers

The Neurobiology of Attention

A CONVERSATION ABOUT NEURODEVELOPMENT: LOST IN TRANSLATION

The Neurobiology of Drug Addiction

Emo$onal decision making systems and their role in addic$on. Antoine Bechara

Adolescent Brain Development and Drug Abuse. Ken C. Winters, Ph.D. Professor, Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota

Dissociable effects of monoamine reuptake inhibitors on distinct forms of impulsive behavior in rats

TIMING, REWARD PROCESSING AND CHOICE BEHAVIOR IN FOUR STRAINS OF RATS WITH DIFFERENT LEVELS OF IMPULSIVITY ANA I. GARCÍA AGUIRRE

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism

KANSAS STATE UNIVERSITY

SUPPORT INFORMATION ADVOCACY

Commentaries BREAKING UP WITH YOUR DRUG CAN BE HARD TO DO, BUT IS IT MAINLY BECAUSE COMPULSIVE BEHAVIOR INVOLVES SPECIFIC BRAIN REGIONS?

Addiction in the Brain - Latest Research. Gary M. Henschen, MD, LFAPA Chief Behavioral Health Officer Magellan Healthcare, Inc.

Instructions for use

Instrumental Conditioning VI: There is more than one kind of learning

Early-Life Maternal Separation and Social Isolation Produce an Increase in Impulsive Action but Not Impulsive Choice

EDUCATION FULL-TIME ACADEMIC EXPERIENCE

processes in the central nervous system (CNS), affecting many of the during the course of ethanol treatment. Ethanol stimulates the release of

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

Psychopharmacology of ADHD. Copyright 2006 Neuroscience Education Institute. All rights reserved.

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

SACTob Position Statement on Nicotine and Its Regulation in Tobacco and Non-Tobacco Products.

Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops

Brain Imaging Data of ADHD

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors

Associations between Willingness to Exert Cognitive Effort and Smoking Abstinence

Anatomy of the basal ganglia. Dana Cohen Gonda Brain Research Center, room 410

HHS Public Access Author manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 April 01.

The Role of Smoking in Cocaine. Addiction

Transcription:

Succumbing to instant gratification without the nucleus accumbens Rudolf N. Cardinal Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom. Telephone: +44 (0)1223 333587 Fax: +44 (0)1223 333564 E-mail: Rudolf.Cardinal@pobox.com Submitted for the Eppendorf and Science Prize for Neurobiology, 2003 http://www.sciencemag.org/feature/data/prizes/eppendorf/eppenprize.shtml Deadline: 31 July 2003 Word count (max 1,000): 991 Animals act to obtain rewards such as food, shelter, and sex. Sometimes, their actions are rewarded or reinforced immediately, but often this is not the case; to be successful, animals must learn to act on the basis of delayed reinforcement. They may also profit by choosing delayed reinforcers over immediate reinforcers, if the delayed reinforcers are sufficiently large. However, individuals differ in their ability to choose delayed rewards. Selfcontrolled individuals are strongly influenced by delayed reinforcement, and choose large, delayed rewards in preference to small, immediate rewards; in contrast, individuals who are relatively insensitive to delayed reinforcement choose impulsively, preferring the immediate, smaller reward in this situation (1). Impulsivity has long been recognized as a normal human characteristic (2) and in some circumstances it may be beneficial (3), but impulsive choice contributes to deleterious states such as drug addiction (4-8) and attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (9). Little is known about how the brain learns from or chooses delayed reinforcement. We studied three brain regions previously implicated in other kinds of reinforcement learning: the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and medial prefrontal cortex (mpfc) (10). The AcbC is a critical site where signals that predict reward have their motivational impact (11-15). All three structures are abnormal in humans with ADHD or in animal models of ADHD (16-20), and all three receive projections from dopaminergic and 1

serotonergic neurons; systemically-administered drugs that influence either of these neuromodulator systems affect animals propensity to make impulsive choices (3, 21-24). To establish whether abnormalities in these regions might cause impulsive choice, we used a task in which hungry rats regularly choose between two levers. Responding on one lever led to the immediate delivery of a small food reward; responding on the other led to a much larger food reward, but this reward was delayed for between 0 and 60 seconds (24, 25). Once they had been trained on this task, we selectively destroyed neurons of the AcbC, ACC, or mpfc, and re-tested the rats (10). Rats with AcbC lesions became, and remained, impulsive; they began to choose the immediate, small reward much more often than sham-operated controls. They persisted in choosing impulsively, even though they were made to experience the larger, delayed alternative at regular intervals. Why did they do this? In theory, impulsive choice might arise for a variety of reasons (1, 21). Take an abstaining smoker, offered a cigarette. His choice is between a small, immediate reward (a cigarette) and a large, delayed reward (better health in the future). If he acts impulsively where another does not, it could be because he does not perceive the larger reward to be as worthwhile as his self-controlled counterpart does, or because he is simply less influenced by outcomes that are delayed considerably. Which is true of rats whose AcbC has been destroyed? Our findings (10, 26) and others (27, 28) suggest the latter. Even those AcbC-lesioned rats who showed an extreme preference for the larger reward when it was not delayed were incapable of choosing it as often as normal rats when it was delayed (10, 26). In fact, AcbC-lesioned rats appear just as sensitive to the magnitude of reward as normal rats (28), suggesting that their impulsive choice arises not because the large reward is subjectively too small to compensate for the normal effects of the delay, but because they would have to wait too long for it. In addition to being impulsive, AcbC-lesioned rats are also hyperactive (10, 29), but they do not appear to be inattentive (30, 31). Destruction of the AcbC does not, therefore, 2

mimic ADHD in full, but our findings suggest that the behaviour of rats with AcbC damage resembles that of humans with the hyperactive impulsive subtype of ADHD (32). In contrast, we found that damage to the ACC or mpfc did not produce impulsive choice (10); thus, although the ACC and mpfc have been shown to be abnormal in disorders of impulsivity (16-18), our findings suggest that dysfunction of these regions is not an important contributor to impulsive choice. The abnormalities of structure or function observed in these regions in ADHD brains may therefore be responsible for other features of the disorder (such as inattention or motoric disinhibition) (33), or these regions may have altered as a consequence of a disease process beginning elsewhere. A clearer understanding of the neurochemical and neuroanatomical basis of the symptoms and signs of ADHD may lead to more effective therapy. This study (10) examined the role of the AcbC in choosing delayed rewards, but did not address whether the AcbC is a critical structure in learning from delayed reinforcement. In order to learn which actions are the correct ones that eventually lead to reinforcement, and which are not, some mechanism must bridge the delay between action and outcome. We recently took two groups of hungry rats, one with AcbC lesions and one without, and presented them with two levers; one did nothing, while every press on the other lever delivered a single food pellet. For some rats, this pellet was delivered immediately; for others, it was delayed. Normal rats took longer to learn to press the lever when the reinforcement was delayed, which is not surprising (34), but they learned successfully with delayed reinforcement. Rats with AcbC lesions were perfectly able to learn this task when there was no delay, but were profoundly impaired when there was a delay between action and outcome (35). Taken together, these results indicate that the AcbC is a reinforcement learning structure specialized for the difficult task of learning with, and choosing, delayed reinforcement. Further understanding of the mechanism by which it does so, or fails to do so, would provide insight into the pathology of a number of neuropsychiatric disorders. 3

Behavioural neuroscientific techniques may make it possible to distinguish the brain regions that underlie different types of impulsivity (3), and to segregate the neural abnormalities that contribute to complex disorders such as ADHD and drug addiction. References 1. G. Ainslie, Psychological Bulletin 82, 463-496 (1975). 2. Aristotle, Nicomachean Ethics [translated by W.D. Ross] (Clarendon Press, Oxford, 350 BC / 1925). 3. J. L. Evenden, Psychopharmacology 146, 348-361 (1999). 4. C. X. Poulos, A. D. Le, J. L. Parker, Behavioural Pharmacology 6, 810-814 (1995). 5. W. K. Bickel, A. L. Odum, G. J. Madden, Psychopharmacology 146, 447-54 (1999). 6. J. L. Evenden, Journal of Psychopharmacology 13, 180-192 (1999). 7. G. M. Heyman, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 561-610 (1996). 8. S. H. Mitchell, Psychopharmacology 146, 455-64 (1999). 9. T. Sagvolden, J. A. Sergeant, Behavioural Brain Research 94, 1-10 (1998). 10. R. N. Cardinal, D. R. Pennicott, C. L. Sugathapala, T. W. Robbins, B. J. Everitt, Science 292, 2499-2501 (2001). 11. J. A. Parkinson, R. N. Cardinal, B. J. Everitt, Progress in Brain Research 126, 263-285 (2000). 12. J. Hall, J. A. Parkinson, T. M. Connor, A. Dickinson, B. J. Everitt, European Journal of Neuroscience 13, 1984-92. (2001). 13. R. N. Cardinal, J. A. Parkinson, J. Hall, B. J. Everitt, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 26, 321-352 (2002). 14. J. R. Taylor, T. W. Robbins, Psychopharmacology 84, 405-12 (1984). 15. C. L. Wyvell, K. C. Berridge, Journal of Neuroscience 20, 8122-8130 (2000). 16. M. Ernst, A. J. Zametkin, J. A. Matochik, P. H. Jons, R. M. Cohen, Journal of Neuroscience 18, 5901-5907 (1998). 17. G. Bush, et al., Biological Psychiatry 45, 1542-1552 (1999). 18. K. Rubia, et al., American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 891-896 (1999). 19. V. Russell, A. Devilliers, T. Sagvolden, M. Lamm, J. Taljaard, Brain Research 676, 343-351 (1995). 20. A. G. Sadile, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24, 161-169 (2000). 21. M. Y. Ho, S. Mobini, T. J. Chiang, C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, Psychopharmacology 146, 362-72 (1999). 22. J. Bizot, C. Le Bihan, A. J. Puech, M. Hamon, M. Thiébot, Psychopharmacology 146, 400-412 (1999). 23. T. R. Wade, H. de Wit, J. B. Richards, Psychopharmacology 150, 90-101 (2000). 24. R. N. Cardinal, T. W. Robbins, B. J. Everitt, Psychopharmacology 152, 362-375 (2000). 25. J. L. Evenden, C. N. Ryan, Psychopharmacology 128, 161-70 (1996). 26. R. N. Cardinal, T. W. Robbins, B. J. Everitt, in Choice, Behavioral Economics and Addiction N. Heather, R. Vuchinich, Eds. (Elsevier, in press). 27. B. Balleine, S. Killcross, Behavioural Brain Research 65, 181-93 (1994). 4

28. V. J. Brown, E. M. Bowman, European Journal of Neuroscience 7, 2479-85 (1995). 29. J. A. Parkinson, M. C. Olmstead, L. H. Burns, T. W. Robbins, B. J. Everitt, Journal of Neuroscience 19, 2401-2411 (1999). 30. A. Christakou, Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Cambridge (2001). 31. B. J. Cole, T. W. Robbins, Behavioural Brain Research 33, 165-79 (1989). 32. APA, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC, 2000). 33. J. L. Muir, B. J. Everitt, T. W. Robbins, Cerebral Cortex 6, 470-81 (1996). 34. A. Dickinson, A. Watt, W. J. H. Griffiths, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section B - Comparative and Physiological Psychology 45, 241-258 (1992). 35. R. N. Cardinal, T. H. C. Cheung, (unpublished data). 5