The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

Similar documents
University of Warwick institutional repository:

The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees

NEO-PI-R Factor Structure in College Students

ABSTRACT. Field of Research: Academic achievement, Emotional intelligence, Gifted students.

GENDER AND AGE DIFFERENCES ON EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE SCALES OF CHILDREN YEARS OLD: PARENTS REPORT ABSTRACT

Emotional Intelligence Assessment Technical Report

Big five personality traits and coping styles predict subjective wellbeing: A study with a Turkish Sample

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

Personality and Individual Differences

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Psychometric Properties of Farsi Version State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (FSTAXI-2)

Incremental Validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Short Form (TEIQue SF)

Ability and personality correlates of general knowledge

Personality and Individual Differences

Key words: State-Trait Anger, Anger Expression, Anger Control, FSTAXI-2, reliability, validity.

M.A. Alhad & S.S. Turnip Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Emotional Intelligence and Leadership

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Self Assessed Intelligence and Academic Performance

Personality and Individual Differences

Self Compassion and Emotional Intelligence of Engineering and Dental College Students

The role of emotional intelligence and personality in moral reasoning

Personality Traits And Emotional Intelligence As Predictors Of Learning English And Math Alireza Homayouni a *

An application of belief-importance theory with reference to the Big Five and trait emotional intelligence

Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school

Individual Conceptions of Well-Being Predict Psychological and Subjective Well-Being: Beyond the Big Five (Book Chapter)

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE A GATEWAY TO SUCCESS FOR MANAGEMENT STUDENTS

Personality and Individual Differences

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method

The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space

Keywords: Emotional Intelligence, Irrational Beliefs, emigrant, aboriginal.

Trait emotional intelligence and decision-related emotions

Affect, Behavior, Cognition, and Desire in the Big Five: An Analysis of Item Content and Structure

Bahria Journal of Professional Psychology, January 2014, Vol-13, 1, 44-63

A comparison between three and ve factor models of Pakistani personality data

The Role of Emotional Intelligence in Academic Performance of Male and Female Students in the University of the Punjab

Personality and Individual Differences

Client Personality and Preference for Counseling Approach: Does Match Matter?

The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Personality Traits and Life Satisfaction among Working Men and Women

When Getting Angry Is Smart: Emotional Preferences and Emotional Intelligence

Emotional Intelligence as a Credible Psychological Construct: Real but Elusive A Conceptual Interpretation of Meta-Analytic Investigation Outcomes

The relationship of five personality factors with the feeling of happiness among students

A study of association between demographic factor income and emotional intelligence

Little more than personality: Dispositional determinants of test anxiety (the Big Five, core self-evaluations, and self-assessed intelligence)

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI

Can trait Emotional Intelligence. predict differences in attainment and progress. in secondary school?

The Doctrine of Traits. Lecture 29

APS Interest Group for Coaching Psychologists (QLD)

Figural Creativity, Creative Potential, and Personality among Taiwanese Fashion Design Undergraduates

Relationship between Teachers' Personality Traits and Self Efficacy: An Empirical Analysis of School Teachers in Karaikal Region (Puducherry)

Is low Emotional Intelligence a primary causal factor in drug and alcohol addiction?

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

Trait Meta-Mood and Subjective Happiness: A 7-week Prospective Study

Study of relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Social Adjustment

The present study investigated the location of trait

The Potential of Emotional Intelligence in Personality Space

Affectivity, Achievement Motivation, and Academic Performance in College Students

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

Multiple Act criterion:

Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties

Personality Traits and the Feeling of Loneliness of Post-Graduate University Students

Running head: EXAMINATION OF THE BIG FIVE 1

Role of Positive Psychological Strengths and Big five Personality Traits in Coping Mechanism of University Students

Management Science Letters

On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence

Rosenthal, Montoya, Ridings, Rieck, and Hooley (2011) Appendix A. Supplementary material

Relationship between Personality Traits and Happiness in Patients with Thalassemia

Running head: INCREMENTAL VALIDITY OF THE TEIQue 1. The Incremental Validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue):

Are dimensions of psycho-social well-being different among Latvian and Romanian University students?

Author's personal copy

Conscientiousness and Eysenckian psychoticism as predictors of school grades: A one-year longitudinal study

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE: THE EFFECT ON SOCIAL MEDIA USE, GENDER, AND INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE. Presented by: Gail Grabczynski April 21, 2018

IJPSS Volume 2, Issue 7 ISSN:

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

Facility for sustained positive affect as an individual difference characteristic

The Relationship between Personality Traits and Reading Proficiency

Relationship of Stress Coping Strategies and Life Satisfaction among Students

The Role of Self-discipline in Predicting Achievement for 10th Graders

Constructing a Three-Part Instrument for Emotional Intelligence, Social Intelligence and Learning Behavior

A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains q

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Original Papers. Psychometric properties of the Polish version of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form

The Relationship between Intrinsic Motivation and Happiness with Academic Achievement in High School Students

Study of the Relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Self Efficacy among School Going Adolescents

Chinese Personality Traits and Mental Health: Mediating Effect of Coping Style

Personality Down Under: Perspectives from Australia

Relation between emotional intelligence and behavioral symptoms in delinquent adolescents

Personality and Individual Differences

Abstract. Asia-pacific Journal of Convergent Research Interchange Vol.4, No.2 June 30 (2018), pp

USING PERSONALITY TESTS IN RESEARCH: ARE LONGER TESTS NECESSARILY BETTER?

Investigating the Relationship between Spiritual Intelligence and Happiness and Quality of Life for Girl Students

BODY IMAGE IN DANCERS

Internet Addiction and Personality Traits among Youths of Rajkot District

IJBARR E- ISSN X ISSN ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND ACADEMIC MOTIVATION IN SCHOOL GIRLS

Transcription:

Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Short Communication The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic a, *, Emily Bennett a, Adrian Furnham b a Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross, London SE14 6NW, United Kingdom b University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, United Kingdom Received 1 August 2006; received in revised form 18 October 2006; accepted 26 October 2006 Available online 6 December 2006 Abstract This study examined the relationship between the Big Five personality traits (Gosling et al., 2003), trait emotional intelligence (EI) (Petrides & Furnham, 2001) and happiness (Argyle et al., 1989) in a sample of 112 (61 female) student and non-student participants. Strong dispositional determinants of happiness were identified. In line with previous findings, four of the Big Five, namely stability, extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness, were positively correlated with both happiness and trait EI, which explained 18% of unique variance (over and above age and the Big Five) in happiness. Furthermore, a significant amount of shared variance between happiness and the Big Five was explained by trait EI, which partly mediated the paths from stability and conscientiousness to happiness, and fully mediated the link between agreeableness and happiness. Limitations and implications are discussed. Ó 2006 Published by Elsevier Ltd. Keywords: Happiness; Trait emotional intelligence; Big Five; Personality * Corresponding author. Dr. Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, New Cross, SE14 6NW, United Kingdom. Tel.: +44 207 919 7885; fax: +44 207 919 7873. E-mail address: t.chamorro-premuzic@gold.ac.uk (T. Chamorro-Premuzic). 0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2006 Published by Elsevier Ltd. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.029

1634 T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 0. Introduction Although few goals are more valued in society than happiness, psychologists have traditionally focused on human unhappiness (e.g., depression, anxiety and emotional disorders) (Argyle, 2001; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). However, recent years have seen an upsurge in studies on happiness (e. g., Argyle, 2001; Diener, 2000; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), which is defined in terms of the average level of satisfaction over a specific time period, the frequency and degree of positive affect manifestations, and the relative absence of negative affect (Argyle, Martin, & Crossland, 1989). Although happiness depends on situational factors, reflected in within-individual variations of affect, the fact that some individuals are consistently happier than others suggests dispositional causes underlying the pursuit and experience of happiness. Indeed, personality traits are arguably the most robust predictors of happiness, if not the major determinant. Hence Eysenck s (1983) famous assertion that happiness is a thing called stable extraversion (p. 87). The most compelling evidence for the strong associations between personality traits and happiness derived from DeNeve and Cooper s (1998) meta-analysis, which indicated that four of the so-called Big Five personality factors, namely emotional stability (ES), conscientiousness (C), extraversion (E), and agreeableness (A) usually in that order predispose individuals towards happiness. There is also wide consensus on the fact that ES and E, linked to temperamental differences in positive and negative affect, provide the biological basis of happiness, with A providing the social, and C the achievement, components of happiness (Carver & Scheier, 2004; Furnham & Cheng, 1997; Hayes & Joseph, 2003). In recent years, dispositional explanations of happiness have also emphasized the importance of emotional intelligence (EI), which, among other things, refers to the ability to identify and manage one s and others emotions (Palmer et al., 2002; Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Unlike cognitive ability, EI is most reliably assessed via self-report inventories, rather than objective performance tests. Therefore the label trait EI has been put forward to reflect its taxonomic position within the realm of personality. Given the conceptual and empirical overlap between trait EI and other personality dimensions, notably ES, E, and A (Petrides & Furnham, 2001) it is particularly important to test whether trait EI may explain variance in happiness beyond other personality traits. Accordingly, a recent study (Furnham & Petrides, 2003) found trait EI predicted happiness over and above the Big Five personality dimensions. Furthermore, the significant links between the Big Five and happiness were fully accounted for by trait EI. Thus the present study set out to explore the mediational role of trait EI in the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and happiness. To our knowledge, no other studies have simultaneously examined the Big Five and Petrides trait EI as predictors of happiness. However, a growing number of studies suggest that despite the considerable overlap between trait EI and the Big Five personality factors, trait EI explains additional and unique variance in a number of important outcomes. For instance, Saklofske, Austin, and Minski (2003) found that trait EI (assessed via Schutte et al., 1998 inventory) predicted life satisfaction and depression-proneness beyond the Big Five. Petrides and Furnham (2003) reported that, although trait EI was substantially related to extraversion, agreeableness, emotional stability, and autonomy, EI-facets predicted additional variance over and above the Big Five in competency to support. Petrides, Frederickson, and Furnham (2004) showed that

T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 1635 trait EI explained several educational outcomes (notably truancy and attendance) even when major personality factors this time assessed via Eysenck s Giant Three (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985) were considered. Last, but not least, a recent paper (Petrides, Perez-Gonzalez, & Furnham, in press) showed that in two studies trait EI significantly predicted rumination, life satisfaction, depression, dysfunctional attitudes, and coping, even when controlling for Big Five variance. Thus one may expect trait EI to explain happiness even when major personality factors are taken into account. In the present study, it was predicted that: H1: Four of the Big Five personality traits, namely ES, E, C and A, would be significantly and positively correlated with both happiness and trait EI. H2: Trait EI would be significantly and positively correlated with happiness. H3: The relationship between the Big Five and happiness would be fully mediated (accounted for) by trait EI. As the sample comprised a similar number of female and male participants, as well as a wide range of age, possible effects of age and gender were also examined to ensure that any relationship between the central variables of the study was not a function of these demographic factors. 1. Method 1.1. Participants One hundred and twelve (61 female) participants, aged 13 to 59 (M = 25.1, SD = 9.4) years, took part in this study. Approximately 50% of the sample was composed of undergraduate students (arts, media, psychology, and computer sciences) from the University of London, with the rest being drawn from the general population (mostly family members or friends of the participating students, as well as people approached in the city of London). 2. Measures The Oxford Happiness Inventory (OHI; Argyle et al., 1989): This is a widely used, 29-item, happiness scale, where participants respond on a 7-point Likert-type scale (ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree ). The OHI has been shown to have adequate test-retest reliability (7-week period =.78; 5-months period =.67) and moderate to high internal consistency (typical Cronbacha between.64 and.87) (see Argyle et al., 1989), and has been translated into several languages, including Chinese and Hebrew. The Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003): This is a quick measure of the five factors of personality. The authors reported adequate levels of convergent and discriminant validity, as well as test-retest reliability. The inventory begins with the stem I see myself as: followed by ten pairs of two trait descriptors, which participants rate on a 7-point Likert-type scale (ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree ).

1636 T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 The Trait Emotional-Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF; Petrides & Furnham, 2006): This scale is a 30-item instrument designed to assess individuals emotional self-efficacy or ability to identify and manage their own and others emotions. It is based on the theory of trait EI, which regards the construct as a personality disposition at the lower hierarchical level of the Big Five personality traits. The scale is based on the full, 153-item TEIQue, which covers the trait EI sampling domain in its entirety. Adequate internal consistencies and broad coverage of the sampling domain of the construct have been reported (Petrides & Furnham, 2006). Items are responded to according to a 7-point Likert-type scale (ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. 3. Procedure Participants were tested individually. Order effects were controlled by randomly counterbalancing the order of administration. Battery completion time took approximately 12 minutes. All participants were fully de-briefed upon completion and provided with individual feedback if requested. 4. Results Descriptive statistics (M and SD) and inter-correlation coefficients (Pearson s r) for all measures are reported in Table 1. As shown, happiness correlated positively with ES, E, C, and A, fully supporting H1. The high and positive correlation between trait EI and happiness yielded support for H2. In the first regression, age and gender accounted for 11% of the variance in happiness scores for the overall sample, with age (but not gender) as a significant predictor in the model. When the big five where added as predictors in block 2, the percentage of variance accounted for significantly increased by 23%, and stability, conscientiousness and agreeableness (as well as age) were significant predictors. In block 3, trait EI was added as predictor and the overall variance explained Table 1 Descriptive statistics and inter-correlations between the primary variables M SD a 1 2 3 4 5 6 (1) Happiness 153.1 23.6.80 (2) Trait EI 145.2 18.7.72.70 ** (3) Stability 4.59 1.3.68.31 **.30 ** (4) Extraversion 4.95 1.0.59.21 *.25 **.00 (5) Conscientiousness 4.82 1.3.67.44 **.37 **.10.11 (6) Agree 4.49 1.4.67.32 **.42 **.20 *.01.33 ** (7) Openness 5.13 1.1.56.16.15.25 **.03.19 *.03 Note: N = 112 (61 females). p <.05. ** p <.01.

T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 1637 significantly increased by 18%, which represents an increase of 78% (18/23*100). The most significant predictor of this model was trait EI, with conscientiousness, stability and age still significant predictors. Agreeableness however, was no longer significant suggesting that trait EI mediates the link between agreeableness and happiness. The same analysis was repeated for female and male participants, separately. For females, the percentage of variance explained by each model was nearly identical to that explained in the overall sample. However, there were some noticeable differences in regards to specific predictors, notably extraversion, which was a significant predictor of females happiness scores in block 2, and agreeableness, which was not significant. When trait EI was added as predictor in block 3, only conscientiousness and trait EI were significant predictors, suggesting that, for female participants, the effects of age, stability, and extraversion on happiness are mediated by trait EI. When the analysis was repeated for males, the models accounted for slightly less variance in happiness than in the analyses for females and the overall dataset. Individual predictors showed some differences, too. Of the big five, only conscientiousness was a significant predictor (in block 2), though this effect disappeared when trait EI was added to the equation in block 3. 5. Discussion In all, results suggest strong dispositional influences on happiness. Although established personality traits (notably ES, A, and C) explained a considerable amount of variance in happiness, some of these paths (partly in the case of ES and C, and fully in the case of A) were explained by trait EI, which was a much stronger predictor than the Big Five (and age) of happiness. One possible explanation for the higher predictive validity of trait EI compared to the Big Five is that the former was assessed via a 30-item questionnaire, compared to a very short, 10-item measure of the Big Five (which leaves only 2-items per factor). Thus the comparative measurement fidelity may have over-estimated the impact of trait EI and under-estimated that of the Big Five. However, this short personality scale has shown more than adequate reliability and validity in the past (Gosling et al., 2003), and in the present study, its correlations with both happiness and trait EI were consistent with the previous literature (Argyle et al., 1989; DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; Petrides & Furnham, 2001). Needless to say, there are obvious practical advantages of employing such an economical measure of the Big Five as opposed to, say, the full 240-items of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). There is growing interest in short measures of personality and recent research has shown that even single item measures of the Big Five have adequate reliability and validity (Woods & Hampson, 2005). Leaving aside the psychometric issue of bandwidth fidelity, a more theoretical interpretation of results would indicate that the well-established dimensions of ES and A, which have been traditionally associated with happiness (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998), may largely be related to happiness because they are associated with individual differences in trait EI. Why C effects happiness, however, can only partly be explained by trait EI. Thus individuals high in C would not only be more able to identify and regulate their and others emotions more effectively, but also work harder to accomplish their life goals, which in turn would lead to higher achievement levels. Although the present findings point in the direction of strong dispositional influences on happiness, the criterion variable was itself operationalized in terms of aggregated or trait levels, and

1638 T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 assessed via a self-report inventory. This design limitation arguably inflated the association between happiness and the trait predictors examined, undermining the effects of situational variables. There is also the issue of the relatively small and unrepresentative sample size; whilst the present study examined not only students, 50% of the sample did consist of university undergraduates and the overall sample was predominantly British. Future research should attempt to include both dispositional and situational factors and examine larger and more representative samples to overcome these limitations and further explore and assess the comparative importance of different predictors of happiness. Table 2 Hierarchical regressions of happiness onto age, gender, Big Five, and trait EI Overall N = 112 Females N = 61 Males N =51 b t b t b t 1 Age.36 3.99 **.35 2.91 **.37 2.85 ** Gender.05.58 F (2, 109) = 7.96 ** (1, 59) = 8.41 ** (1, 49) = 8.17 ** Adj. R 2.11.11.12 2 Age.27 3.37 **.28 2.69 **.26 2.03 * Gender.07.91 Stability.23 2.87 **.28 2.56 **.14 1.12 Extraversion.12 1.54.21 1.94 *.01.10 Conscientiousness.29 3.40 **.29 2.44 *.37 2.79 ** Agreeableness.16 1.97 *.09.79.12.97 Openness.03.44.01.09.08.65 F (7, 104) = 9.15 ** (6, 54) = 6.75 ** (6, 44) = 4.07 ** Sig. F change 8.53 ** 5.72 ** 2.93 ** Adj. R 2.34.36.27 3 Age.14 2.09 *.14 1.59.17 1.55 Gender.00.00 Stability.12 1.66 *.11 1.14.10.93 Extraversion.03.47.11 1.22.09.85 Conscientiousness.19 2.63 **.22 2.17 *.18 1.50 Agreeableness.00.11.04.38.07.59 Openness.00.11.01.11.02.26 Trait EI.54 6.40 **.53 4.58 **.58 4.30 ** F (8, 103) = 16.21 ** (7, 53) = 10.92 ** (7, 43) = 7.52 ** Sig. F change 40.97 ** 20.98 ** 18.54 ** Adj. R 2.52.54.48 * p <.05. ** p <.01.

T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 1639 Despite the methodological limitations of this brief report, the present results (particularly those reported in Table 2) provide useful information for a prospective hierarchical integration of the different dispositional determinants of happiness, and emphasize the independent contribution of the rather young construct of trait EI in the prediction of happiness. Although further replication, adjusting for bandwidth fidelity and including situational variables, is needed to further examine the incremental validity of trait EI over personality and other individual difference factors, trait EI appears to be an important determinant of happiness. References Argyle, M. (2001). The psychology of happiness (2nd ed.). London: Routledge. Argyle, M., Martin, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Happiness as a function of personality and social encounters. In J. P. Forgas & M. Innes (Eds.), Recent advances in social psychology: an international perspective. North Holland: Elsevier. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2004). Perspectives on personality (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI): professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197 229. Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: the science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55, 34 43. Eysenck, H. J. (1983). I do: your guide to a happy marriage. London: Century. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences. New York: Plenum. Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (1997). Personality and happiness. Psychological Reports, 80, 761 762. Furnham, A., & Petrides, K. V. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence and happiness. Social Behaviour and Personality, 31, 815 824. Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr., (2003). A very brief measure of the Big Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504 528. Hayes, N., & Joseph, S. (2003). Big Five correlates of three measures of subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 723 727. Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N., & Furnham, A. (2004). The role of trait emotional intelligence in academic performance and deviant behavior at school. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 277 293. Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: psychometric investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15, 425 448. Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: behavioural validation in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. European Journal of Personality, 17, 39 57. Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2006). User manual of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue). Institute of Education, University of London. Petrides, K. V., Perez-Gonzalez, J. C., & Furnham, A. (in press). On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion. Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., & Minski, P. S. (2003). Factor structure and validity of a trait emotional intelligence measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1091 1100. Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185 211. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167 177. Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology. American Psychologist, 55, 5 14. Woods, S. A., & Hampson, S. E. (2005). Measuring the Big Five with single items using a bipolar response scale. European Journal of Personality, 19, 373 390.