NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES HOW WAS THE WEEKEND? HOW THE SOCIAL CONTEXT UNDERLIES WEEKEND EFFECTS IN HAPPINESS AND OTHER EMOTIONS FOR US WORKERS

Similar documents
NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES WEEKENDS AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING. John F. Helliwell Shun Wang. Working Paper

An Assessment of Life Satisfaction Responses on Recent Statistics Canada Surveys

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES VALUATION OF THE RISK OF SARS IN TAIWAN. Jin-Tan Liu James K. Hammitt Jung-Der Wang Meng-Wen Tsou

STATISTICS 8 CHAPTERS 1 TO 6, SAMPLE MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

Those Who Tan and Those Who Don t: A Natural Experiment of Employment Discrimination

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES INTERNATIONAL EVIDENCE ON THE SOCIAL CONTEXT OF WELL-BEING

The U-Shape without Controls. David G. Blanchflower Dartmouth College, USA University of Stirling, UK.

OECD work on Subjective Well-being

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS

Cancer survivorship and labor market attachments: Evidence from MEPS data

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

Motherhood and Female Labor Force Participation: Evidence from Infertility Shocks

A Blueprint for Exploring International Differences in Well-Being

Are we having a good time, boys and girls? The need for time and affect methodologies in understanding gendered wellbeing

The Impact of Weekend Working on Well-Being in the UK

DAZED AND CONFUSED: THE CHARACTERISTICS AND BEHAVIOROF TITLE CONFUSED READERS

Developing a Comprehensive and One-Dimensional Subjective Well-Being Measurement: Evidence from a Belgian Pilot Survey

Preliminary Draft. The Effect of Exercise on Earnings: Evidence from the NLSY

What Your Wellness Programs Aren't Telling You. Dan Witters Gallup March 18, 2014

Write your identification number on each paper and cover sheet (the number stated in the upper right hand corner on your exam cover).

EMPIRICAL STRATEGIES IN LABOUR ECONOMICS

NIGHT CRIMES: An Applied Project Presented to The Faculty of the Department of Economics Western Kentucky University Bowling Green, Kentucky

CASE STUDY 2: VOCATIONAL TRAINING FOR DISADVANTAGED YOUTH

LIFE SATISFACTION ANALYSIS FOR RURUAL RESIDENTS IN JIANGSU PROVINCE

Helmut Farbmacher: Copayments for doctor visits in Germany and the probability of visiting a physician - Evidence from a natural experiment

BLACK RESIDENTS VIEWS ON HIV/AIDS IN THE DISTRICT OF COLUMBIA

The impact of workplace smoking bans: results from a national survey

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES TRUST AND WELL-BEING. John F. Helliwell Shun Wang. Working Paper

Version No. 7 Date: July Please send comments or suggestions on this glossary to

Discussion Papers. A Note on the High Stability of Happiness: The Minimal Effects of a Nuclear Catastrophe on Life Satisfaction. Eva M.

Validating the American Time Use Survey: Does anybody really know what they were doing yesterday? by Alan B. Krueger, Princeton University 1/28/07

Published online: 10 January 2015 The Author(s) This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com

Time-Use, Unemployment, and Well-Being: An Empirical Analysis Using British Time-Use Data

ECON Microeconomics III

Recreational marijuana and collision claim frequencies

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction

Dylan Small Department of Statistics, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania. Based on joint work with Paul Rosenbaum

The question of whether money buys happiness comes up frequently

October 5, 2006 Stress and Mind/Body Health Hispanics. Summary

Smoking, Wealth Accumulation and the Propensity to Plan

Moral, Trust and Happiness -Why Does Trust Improves Happiness?-

The Effect of the Smoke- Free Illinois Act on Casino Admissions and Revenue

Measuring Subjective Well-Being for Policy and Individual Use

Identifying Endogenous Peer Effects in the Spread of Obesity. Abstract

Americans Current Views on Smoking 2013: An AARP Bulletin Survey

c. Construct a boxplot for the data. Write a one sentence interpretation of your graph.

Changes in Subjective Well-Being and Their Implications

World Happiness 2013

A Cross-State Comparison of Measures of Subjective Well-Being. Younghwan Song. January Abstract

TRUST AND WELL-BEING

Lecture II: Difference in Difference and Regression Discontinuity

A cross-state comparison of measures of subjective wellbeing

Dissatisfied with life, but having a good day: time-use and well-being of the unemployed

Young Women s Marital Status and HIV Risk in Sub-Saharan Africa: Evidence from Lesotho, Swaziland and Zimbabwe

Instrumental Variables I (cont.)

A Probability Puzzler. Statistics, Data and Statistical Thinking. A Probability Puzzler. A Probability Puzzler. Statistics.

Econometric Game 2012: infants birthweight?

Introduction: Statistics, Data and Statistical Thinking Part II

A Cross-State Comparison of Measures of Subjective Well-Being

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Time as an Ingredient in Meal Production and Consumption

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AND TAX DIFFERENTIALS BETWEEN BEER, WINE AND SPIRITS. Working Paper No. 3200

1 Online Appendix for Rise and Shine: The Effect of School Start Times on Academic Performance from Childhood through Puberty

How Much Should We Trust the World Values Survey Trust Question?

Lecture (chapter 1): Introduction

Generalized Estimating Equations for Depression Dose Regimes

Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy Research

Human Capital and Economic Opportunity: A Global Working Group. Working Paper Series. Working Paper No.

Depression Screening: An Effective Tool to Reduce Disability and Loss of Productivity

Collecting & Making Sense of

The OECD Guidelines on Subjective Well-Being

Modeling Interviewer Effects in a Large National Health Study

Creating an Index to Measure Wellbeing and Predict Life Satisfaction in Athens, Georgia Series 1: January 2018

SELECTED FACTORS LEADING TO THE TRANSMISSION OF FEMALE GENITAL MUTILATION ACROSS GENERATIONS: QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS FOR SIX AFRICAN COUNTRIES

Sample selection in the WCGDS: Analysing the impact for employment analyses Nicola Branson

Collecting & Making Sense of

AnExaminationoftheQualityand UtilityofInterviewerEstimatesof HouseholdCharacteristicsinthe NationalSurveyofFamilyGrowth. BradyWest

11/1/2013. Depression affects approximately 350 million people worldwide, and is the leading cause of disability globally (WHO, 2012)

What is Multilevel Modelling Vs Fixed Effects. Will Cook Social Statistics

Dissatisfied with life, but having a good day: time-use and well-being of the unemployed

Volume 35, Issue 4. Are people satisfied with their time use? Empirical evidence from German survey data

Addendum: Multiple Regression Analysis (DRAFT 8/2/07)

Households: the missing level of analysis in multilevel epidemiological studies- the case for multiple membership models

SAMPLE Health Culture Survey

Reading and maths skills at age 10 and earnings in later life: a brief analysis using the British Cohort Study

THE IMPACT OF PERSONALITY TRAITS ON WAGE GROWTH AND THE GENDER WAGE GAP

Do Danes and Italians rate life satisfaction in the same way?

Dissatisfied with Life, but Having a Good Day: Time-Use and Well-Being of the Unemployed

Unemployment, Social Capital, and Subjective Well-Being

Carrying out an Empirical Project

Practical propensity score matching: a reply to Smith and Todd

Dissatisfied with life, but having a good day: time-use and well-being of the unemployed

Rapid decline of female genital circumcision in Egypt: An exploration of pathways. Jenny X. Liu 1 RAND Corporation. Sepideh Modrek Stanford University

GAMBLING AND INDIVIDUALS WELLBEING:

Is Happiness Different From Flourishing? Cross-Country Evidence from the ESS

Behavioral probabilities

STAT 503X Case Study 1: Restaurant Tipping

A Survey Method for Characterizing Daily Life Experience: The Day Reconstruction Method

Early Release from Prison and Recidivism: A Regression Discontinuity Approach *

Well-Being on Planet Earth

Transcription:

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES HOW WAS THE WEEKEND? HOW THE SOCIAL CONTEXT UNDERLIES WEEKEND EFFECTS IN HAPPINESS AND OTHER EMOTIONS FOR US WORKERS John F. Helliwell Shun Wang Working Paper 21374 http://www.nber.org/papers/w21374 NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH 1050 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138 July 2015 Helliwell s research is supported by the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR). Wang gratefully acknowledges financial support from the Korea Development Institute (KDI) School of Public Policy and Management. We thank the Gallup Organization for access to data from the Gallup/ Healthways US Daily Poll. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Bureau of Economic Research. NBER working papers are circulated for discussion and comment purposes. They have not been peerreviewed or been subject to the review by the NBER Board of Directors that accompanies official NBER publications. 2015 by John F. Helliwell and Shun Wang. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that full credit, including notice, is given to the source.

How was the Weekend? How the Social Context Underlies Weekend Effects in Happiness and other Emotions for US Workers John F. Helliwell and Shun Wang NBER Working Paper No. 21374 July 2015 JEL No. I31,J81 ABSTRACT In this paper we estimate the size of weekend effects for seven emotions and then explore their main determinants for the working population in the United States, using the Gallup/Healthways US Daily Poll 2008-2012. We first find that weekend effects exist for all emotions, and that these effects are not explained by sample selection bias. Full-time workers have a larger weekend effects than do parttime workers for all emotions except sadness, for which weekend effects are almost identical for all workers. We then explore the sources of weekend effects and find that workplace trust and workplace social relations, combined with differences in social time spent with family and friends, together almost fully explain the weekend effects for happiness, laughter, enjoyment and sadness, for both full-time and part-time workers, with significant but smaller proportions explained for the remaining three emotions - worry, anger and stress. John F. Helliwell Vancouver School of Economics University of British Columbia 997-1873 East Mall Vancouver BC V6T 1Z1 CANADA and NBER john.helliwell@ubc.ca Shun Wang KDI School of Public Policy and Management 263 Nansejong-ro Sejong, Korea 339-007 swang@kdis.ac.kr

1. Introduction Emotions, both positive and negative, are key measures of subjective well-being (Durand and Smith, 2013; Helliwell and Wang, 2014; Kahneman and Krueger, 2006). Recent empirical studies on emotions find evidence of variations through the week, often called a day-of-week effect. Taylor (2006) finds that those who are interviewed on Fridays report lower levels of mental stress than those interviewed in the middle of the week, using the British Household Panel Survey (BHPS) data. Helliwell and Wang (2014) find that people experience more positive emotions and fewer negative emotions during weekends and statutory holidays than on weekdays in the Gallup/Heathways Daily Poll. A few other studies also find similar weekend effects (Kennedy-Moore et al., 1992; Ryan et al., 2010; Stone et al., 1985; Stone et al., 2012; Young and Lim, 2014). Although there are significant weekend effects for all emotions, when assessed for a specific day, there are no weekend effects for life evaluations (Helliwell & Wang 2014). This difference helps to validate both emotional reports and life evaluations, since the former are intended to reveal day-to-day changes, while the latter are intended to look beyond the day-to-day variations in experiences to provide a broader measure of subjective wellbeing. In this paper we focus more specifically on emotional weekend effects for the working population. In this way we are able to assess more directly, for a comparable set of respondents, the emotional effects of the social context, both on and off the job. Consistent with the findings in Young and Lim (2014), we find that proportionate weekend effects are larger for negative than for positive emotions: from weekends to weekdays, the average levels of stress, anger, worry, and sadness decrease by 32.5%, 24.4%, 24.3%, and 9.6%, while the levels of enjoyment, happiness, and laughter increase by 6.8%, 4.1%, and 3.3% respectively. However, this difference is largely due to the greater general prevalence of positive emotions. When viewed as share of the respondent population who feel any given emotion on weekdays and on the weekend, the outlier is stress, which is felt by 15% more of the sample on weekdays than on weekends. None of the other differences apply to more than 8% of respondents, and average about 4% of respondents for both positive and the remaining negative emotions. We find that full-time 1

workers have larger weekend effects than do part-time workers for all emotions, except for sadness where the two effects are almost the same. The effects for full-time workers are about twice as large as for part-time workers for happiness, enjoyment, anger, and stress. We then show that weekend effects are not driven by selection bias. Some recent studies argue that the day-of-week or weekend effects appearing in some datasets might be due to possible selection bias in the choice of interview days (Taylor, 2006; Tumen and Zeydanli, 2013). In other words, the subjective well-being responses might be correlated with some observed or unobserved characteristics that affect individuals decisions to take interviews on specific days. Using the British Household Panel Survey, Taylor (2006) deals with the selection bias and draws the conclusion that the day-of-week pattern of job satisfaction is not substantially affected by the potential selection bias, although the day-of-week pattern for mental well-being does become less significant after adjusting for the likely bias. Tumen and Zeydanli (2013), using the same data, emphasize the potential selection bias originating from unobservables, and suggest that the day-ofweek patterns may vary across countries. Seeing these possibilities, we also deal with the selection issue in this paper. Since we are studying weekend effects rather than day-ofweek effects, we are mostly concerned about whether individuals answers depend on whether they were interviewed on weekends or weekdays. Our tests show that selection bias has no impact on the weekend effects we estimate. To establish the risk of selection bias we first compare the social-demographic characteristics of weekend and weekday respondents. Some variables have statistically equivalent means while the others have small differences that are rendered statistically significant by the very large sample size. In these latter cases we check the standardized differences of means, which are often used to test the sample balance in the Propensity Score Matching method. We find that all the variables are quite balanced. We also illustrate the distribution of propensity scores of being selected into weekends for respondents who are actually interviewed on weekends and those who are interviewed on weekdays. The two distributions are well matched. 2

Next we make a direct test for selection bias by exploiting the nice feature of our data that people report yesterday s emotions. The emotions for Sunday, Monday and Tuesday, which are reported on Monday, Tuesday, and Wednesday respectively in the survey, should not be affected by the weekend selection bias. The difference of emotions reported for Sunday and Monday should therefore represent a non-biased weekend effect, while the difference between the emotions reported for Tuesday and Monday should be very close to zero. Our results show that the latter is zero or very close to zero, while the former is much larger and always significantly greater than zero. This suggests that weekend effect we observe is not primarily or even importantly driven by selection bias. We further compare the weekend effect measured by the difference between emotions on Sunday and Monday with the regular weekend effect based on all data and find that they are very close to each other for every emotion. We know that the former is not subject to selection bias, but the latter could be affected; the fact that they are very close to each other shows that selection bias is approximately zero. Having established the validity of the weekend effect data, we then explore the determinants of weekend effects. Stone et al. (2012) show that weekend effects for emotions are smaller among older workers, particularly with respect to negative emotions. Helliwell and Wang (2014) find that the weekend effect varies with gender, marriage status, age, and working status. Young and Lim (2014) and Helliwell and Wang (2014) both find that the variation of social time across days of week is an important determinant of weekend effects. The quality of the social context at work has previously been shown to influence both life evaluations and emotions (Helliwell and Huang, 2010; Helliwell and Wang, 2014). We further confirm, as illustrated previously (Helliwell and Wang, 2014), that emotional reports (which refer to a particular day) will show weekend effects, while life evaluations, which refer to life as a whole these days, do not show weekend effects. Workers with better social contexts in their workplaces will show smaller weekend effects. Our reasoning is that in a favorable workplace environment workers tend to experience more positive emotions and fewer negative emotions than do other workers, thus making weekend effects smaller for them than for workers in less happy workplaces. In this paper, we examine the magnitude of the social context and social time 3

influences in explaining weekend effects for each of the seven emotions. In our data we find that social time significantly increases on weekends and drops during weekdays. Daily social hours on weekends and weekdays average 7.7 and 5.1 hours respectively for full-time workers, and 7.5 and 6 hours for part-time workers. We find that workplace environment and social time together can almost fully explain the weekend effects for happiness, laughter, enjoyment and sadness, for both full-time and part-time workers, while the explained part of the weekend effects for worry, anger, and stress ranges between 17.9% and 79.5%. We conclude that unfavorable workplace environments and variations of social time are the major driving forces for the weekend effects for the emotional well-being of the working population in the United States. The rest of paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, we describe the data and research methodology. In section 3, we report the estimates of weekend effect and show that our results are not likely to be driven by the selectivity bias. In section 4 we explore the determinants of weekend effects. Section 5 concludes. 2. Data and Methodology The data we use for this study come from the Gallup/Healthways US Daily Poll. From the beginning of 2008, Gallup has randomly interviewed about 1,000 American adults each day in the United States. By the end of 2012, the total number of respondents accumulated in the data is 1.77 million. The Daily Poll includes a set of questions on emotional well-being: for positive emotions we have happiness, enjoyment, and laughter, and for negative emotions we have worry, sadness, anger, and stress. Laughter is a zero to one scale response to the question Did you smile or laugh a lot yesterday? Other six emotions are zero-to-one scale responses to the question Did you experience the following feelings during a lot of the day yesterday? Emotion questions were asked on every survey day, except for the stress question, which was not asked in 2011 and 2012. The survey includes the labor force status for each respondent, so that we can a sample based on the working population. There are about 0.94 million working respondents, accounting for 58% of all respondents. Among the working respondents, nearly 80% are full-time paid workers. 4

The key explanatory variables are social hours and two measures of the quality of the workplace social context. The social hours variable is a response to the question Approximately, how many hours did you spend, socially, with friends or family yesterday? Please include telephone or e-mail or other online communication. This question was asked of all the respondents from 2008 to 2010, but in 2011 and 2012 only 10% and 5% of total respondents, randomly selected, were asked this question. Respondents report numbers between 0 and 24. Among the answers, there are about 5% of respondents reporting more than 16 social hours. To make this social hours variable more reliable, we replace any value greater than 16 by 16. There are two questions on workplace environment. One is Does your supervisor always create an environment that is trusting and open, or not? The answer to this question is binary, 1 for yes and 0 for no. 80% of respondents answer that the environment is trusting. Another question is Does your supervisor at work treat you more like he or she is your boss or your partner? The answer to this question is also binary, 1 for partner and 0 for boss. 62% report having a partner-like boss. The survey also includes a number of socio-demographic variables, such as gender, age, marital status, level of education, number of children under 18, monthly household income, health insurance coverage and importance of religion. Monthly household income refers to before-tax income from all sources, including wages and salaries, remittances from family members living elsewhere, farming, and others. The response is categorical, in which zero to ten stands for no income, under $60, $60 to $499, $500 to $999, $1,000 to $1,999, $2,000 to $2,999, $3,000 to $3,999, $4,000 to $4,999, $5,000 to $7,499, $7,500 to $9,999, and $10,000 and over, respectively. We construct the numerical household income by replacing the categorical response by the mean of each non-top category, and $18,000 for the top income category. There are about 0.78 million working respondents reporting income. To reduce the impact of missing income on the number of observations, we assign a zero value to log income when income is missing, and a dummy variable which equals to 1 if the income is missing will be used together with log income in regressions. The summary statistics for all the variables are reported in Appendix Table A1. 5

To show the size of weekend effects, we estimate the following model for each emotion for full-time and part-time workers separately: (1) emotion = α+ βweekend + X Γ+ X Ω+ ε, it it it ct it where i indexes individuals. The variable emotion denotes one of the seven emotions. it weekend is an indicator variable that equals to one if the emotions are for weekends or it statutory holidays. The vector X it denotes a set of individual- and household-level covariates, which include respondent s gender, age, age squared divided by 100, marital status, education levels, household income, number of children, frequency of church attendance, an indicator variable that equals to one if having health insurance, and a dummy variable indicating the importance of religion in life. X ct is a vector for stateyear fixed effects. ε it is the error term. To see how the weekend effect is varying with each respondent s workplace environment, we estimate the following model for full-time and part-time workers respectively: (2) emotion = α+ βweekend + γweekend work + θwork + X Γ+ X Ω+ ν. The variable it it it it it it ct it work denotes the quality of the workplace environment. We have two it measures for workplace quality: one is a dummy variable that equals to one if respondents report having a workplace is trustworthy and open, and the second is a dummy variable that equals to one if respondents report having a supervisor who is more like a partner than a boss. The two measures will first be included in the regression individually and then together. ν it is the error term. This is a typical difference-indifference (DID) approach, where γ captures the difference in weekend effect by workplace environment. Next we add the social time variables into Equation (2) to check how well weekend effects are further explained by social hours. The equation is as follows: (3) emotion = α+ βweekend + γweekend work + θwork + S Ψ+ X Γ+ X Ω+ ν, it it it it it it it ct it where S it denotes a set of variables on social time, including log of social hours, a dummy variable for zero social hour, and a dummy for zero to one social hour. 6

3. The Size of Weekend Effects for Positive and Negative Emotions 3.1. Descriptive results Table 1 gives a detailed summary of the three positive emotions - happiness, enjoyment, and laughter, and four negative emotions - worry, sadness, anger, and stress. In the table we report the number of observations, estimated means and standard errors for each emotion on weekends and weekdays, and weekend effects measured by the mean difference of each emotion between weekends and weekdays, and the percentage of change of emotion from weekdays to weekends. We can see that individuals generally have higher levels of positive emotions and lower levels of negative emotions. Positive emotions are higher on weekends than weekdays, with the reverse applying to negative emotions. The weekend effect is statistically significant for each emotion. Moreover, the effect is sizable, with absolute values ranging from 0.014 to 0.150, and the percentage change ranging from 3.3% to 32.5%. The percentage improvements for negative emotions from weekdays to weekends are generally larger than for positive emotions. For example, the reductions in stress, worry, and anger are -32.5%, -24.3%, -24.4% respectively, much larger than the improvement in enjoyment, 6.8%, the most improved among the positive emotions. These differences primarily reflect the fact that the average frequency is much less for negative than for positive emotions. 3.2. Regression results In this section we show the estimates of weekend effects using Equation (1). We run OLS regressions for each emotion, separately for full-time and part-time workers. For each group, we run two models, one is the model only controlling for state-year fixed effects, another including the state-year fixed effects and the full set of covariates described in Equation (1): gender, age, age squared divided by 100, marital status, education levels, household income, number of children, frequency of church attendance, an indicator variable that equals to one if having health insurance, and a dummy variable indicating the importance of religion in life. We report the coefficients of the weekend dummy in Table 2. We find that the weekend coefficients for each emotion are almost the same in the two models, which indicates that other covariates of weekend and weekday samples 7

are well balanced, given the random sampling procedure. Moreover, the weekend effect for full-time workers is larger than for part-time workers. For happiness, enjoyment, anger and stress, the weekend effect for part-time workers is approximately half that for full-time workers. The relative size is two-thirds for laughter, three-quarters for worry, while almost equal for sadness. 3.3. Testing for selection effects As shown in Taylor (2006) and Tumen and Zeydanli (2013), the size of the day-ofthe-week effect may be affected by the potential bias of respondents self-selection into specific days within a week. In this section we run a few tests to show that the impact of selection bias in our data is fairly small, if not zero 1. In Table 3 we test the balance of social-demographic variables for respondents being surveyed on weekends and weekdays. Columns (1) and (2) report the mean of each variable for respondents being surveyed on weekends and weekdays respectively. Columns (3) and (4) show the mean difference between Weekends and Weekdays and the corresponding standard error. We will find some small difference for some variables, some of which are rendered statistically significant by the large sample size. In this case we may check the standardized differences of means, which is a common way to check sample balance in propensity score matching methods, following Lee (2013) and Rosenbaum and Rubin (1985). Column (5) reports the standardized differences of means calculated by the formula (4), where and are the sample means for weekends and weekdays, and and are the corresponding sample variances. We can see that the absolute value of each standardized difference is smaller than 2.2. This is much smaller than the often-used cutoff value 10. This confirms the similarity of the weekend and weekday samples. We also illustrate in Figure 1 the propensity scores for being selected for weekend interviews. We use a probit model to estimate the propensity score. The variables used to 1 We report the results only for paid workers, but the tests for full samples yield similar results. 8

estimate the propensity score of being selected into weekend include respondent s gender, age, age squared divided by 100, marital status, education levels, household income, number of children, frequency of church attendance, an indicator variable that equals to one if having health insurance, a dummy variable indicating the importance of religion in life, and respondent s full-time or part-time working status. The upper panel of Figure 1 shows the distribution of propensity scores for people who were actually interviewed on weekends and weekdays, and the lower panel shows the distribution of propensity scores for people who report emotions for weekends (who were interviewed on Sunday and Monday) and weekdays (who were interviewed on Tuesday to Saturday). In both cases, the distribution of propensity scores is quite similar for the two groups, which suggests that each respondent has almost the same probability of being selected into weekends or weekdays. The nice feature of the data is that people report yesterday s emotions. Therefore the emotions relating to Sunday, Monday and Tuesday which are reported on Monday, Tuesday, and Wednesday, respectively, are not affected by possible selection bias. Column (1) of Table 4 shows the difference in emotions between Sunday and Monday and column (2) shows the difference in emotions between Tuesday and Monday. The values in column (2) are all equal to or very close to zero, suggest that emotions are stable across weekdays. The large differences between column (1) and column (2) suggest that real weekend effects exist. Thus the weekend effect we observe is not driven by selection effects. We then compare the weekend effect measured by the difference between emotions on Sunday and Monday in column (1), with the regular weekend effect based on all data in column (3). We know that the former is not subject to selection bias, but the latter may be subject to certain level of selection bias. However, since the two results are so similar, the bias must be negligibly small. 4. Exploring the Determinants of Weekend Effects In this section we explore the determinants of weekend effects. We first run OLS regressions following Equation (2) to examine how weekend effects vary with workplace social context, measured by reported workplace trust and type of supervisor (boss-like or 9

partner-like). We further control social time variables following Equation (3). We report the estimated weekend effect for happiness in Tables 5 and 6 for full-time and part-time workers respectively. In each table, Models (1), (3) and (5) follows Equation (2) and Models (2), (4) and (6) follows Equation (3). We control the dummy for non-trusting workplace and its interaction with the weekend dummy in Models (1) and (2), the dummy for boss-like supervisor and its interaction with the weekend dummy in Models (3) and (4), and both workplace dummies and their interactions with the weekend dummy in Models (5) and (6). From Model (1) in Table 5 we see that the weekend effect of happiness for full-time workers in high-trust workplaces is 0.025, while for those reporting non-trusting workplace environment it is 0.070, which equals to 0.025 plus the 0.045 coefficient on the interaction term. This implies that the weekend effect is almost three times larger for those working in a low-trust environment. Results in Model (3) using the alternative workplace environment variable confirm the finding: full-time workers reporting bosslike supervisor have weekend effects twice as do workers with partner-like supervisors. If we include both indicators of workplace social context, as shown in Model (5), the weekend effect of happiness for full-time workers reduces to 0.022, and that for unfavorable environment (both indicators equal to 1) rises to 0.073. Workers with a bosslike supervisor in a low trust workplace thus have weekend effects more than three times as large as for those having a partner-like supervisor in a higher trust work environment. If we control also for the social time variable, the weekend effect for those full-time workers reporting favorable workplace environment is reduced to zero, as shown in Models (2) and (4), or even slightly negative (-0.003) as in Model (6). This suggests that weekend effects for respondents with favorable workplace environments are only due to the differing amounts of social time on weekends and weekdays. The weekend effect for those reporting unfavorable workplace environments is now 0.041, 0.039, and 0.034 in Models (2), (4) and (6) respectively, which is much smaller than the effect without accounting for the difference in social time between weekends and weekdays. We observe similar patterns for part-time workers for the weekend effect of happiness in Table 6. Without controlling for social time variable, the weekend effect for those 10

reporting unfavorable workplace environments is always larger than for those reporting favorable environments (by about 0.013). Moreover, accounting for social time differences between weekends and weekdays reduces the weekend effect for those parttime workers reporting favorable workplace environment to a level not significantly different from zero. The results for happiness in Tables 5 and 6 are summarized in Panel A of Table 7, in which we report weekend effects in different scenarios. To save space, we follow the format of Panel A to report the results for the remaining positive emotions in Panels B and C of Table 7, and for the four negative emotions in Panels A to D of Table 8. The weekend effect for enjoyment is reported in Panel B of Table 7. Without controlling for the social time variable, the weekend effect for full-time workers is 0.042 and 0.140 for those reporting favorable environment and unfavorable workplace social contexts. Holding social time constant, the two corresponding values reduce to 0.011 and 0.103. Thus controlling for social time reduces the weekend effect by about threequarters for those with good workplace environments, and by one-quarter for those with socially unfavorable workplaces. If we compare the weekend effect for those who report unfavorable workplace environments without accounting for social time, 0.140, with the effect for those who report favorable environment after allowing for social time, 0.011, the reduction is over 90%. The proportionate reduction is almost 90% (0.055 to 0.006) for part-time workers. In Panel C of Table 7 we summarize the weekend effects for laughter. We see that social time and the workplace social context together fully explain the weekend effect. In the four panels of Table 8 we report the weekend effects for worry, sadness, anger, and stress respectively. Compared to our previous results for positive emotions, the explanatory power of workplace environment and social time is lower for all negative emotions except sadness. Specifically, in the case of full-time workers, social time and workplace environment together explain about half of the weekend effects for worry (- 0.110 to -0.056), 80% for anger (-0.088 to -0.018) and 40% for stress (-0.202 to -0.123). For part-time workers the part of the weekend effect explained by the social context 11

variables is one-third for worry (-0.079 to -0.051), 50% for anger (-0.026 to -0.013), and 20% for stress (-0.095 to -0.078). 5. Conclusion In this paper we estimate the size and sources of weekend effects for seven emotions for the working population in the United States, using the Gallup/Healthways US Daily Poll 2008-2012. We first find that weekend effects, measured as the difference in fractions of the population reporting each emotion between weekends and weekdays, are statistically and economically significant for all seven emotions. Moreover, full-time workers have larger weekend effects than do part-time workers for all emotions except for sadness, where the effects are similar for fulltime and part-time workers. We also show that the weekend effects we find are not driven by selection bias. We then explore the sources of weekend effects and find that they are much smaller for workers with good workplace social contexts, as indicated by high workplace trust and a partner-like boss. Moreover, social time can largely or even entirely explain the remaining weekend effects for positive emotions and sadness, for workers with favorable workplace social contexts. The workplace social environment and social time together almost completely account for the weekend effects for happiness, laugher, enjoyment and sadness, for both full-time and part-time workers. The explanatory power is lower for the remaining negative emotions. Taken together, the quality of the social contexts on and off the job are the primary forces behind weekend effects in the subjective well-being of the working population of the United States. 12

References Durand, M. and Smith C. (2013). The OECD Approach to Measuring Subjective Well- Being. In Helliwell J.F., Layard, R., and Sachs, J. (eds.) World Happiness Report 2013, UN Sustainable Development Solutions Network, 112-137. Helliwell, J.F. and Huang, H. (2010). How s the Job? Well-Being and Social Capital in the Workplace. Industrial and Labor Relations Review 63(2), 205-227. Helliwell, J.F. and Wang, S. (2014). Weekends and Subjective Well-Being. Social Indicators Research 116(2), 389-407. Kahneman, D. and Krueger A. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives 20(1), 3-24. Kennedy-Moore, E., Greenberg, M.A., Newman, M.G. and Stone, A.A. (1992). The Relationship Between Daily Events and Mood: The Mood Measure May Matter. Motivation and Emotion 162(2), 143-155. Lee W. (2013). Propensity Score Matching and Variations on the Balancing Test. Empirical Economics 44(1), 47-80. Rosenbaum, P. and Rubin D. (1985). Constructing a Control Group Using Multivariate Matched Sampling Methods that Incorporate the Propensity Score. American Statistician 39(1), 33-38. Ryan, R.M., Bernstein, J.H. and Brown, K.W. (2010). Weekends, Work, and Well-being: Psychological Need Satisfactions and Day of the Week Effects on Mood, Vitality, and Physical Symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 29(1), 95-122. Stone, A.A., Hedges, S.M., Neale, J.M. and Satin, S. (1985). Prospective and Cross- Sectional Mood Reports Offer No Evidence of A Blue Monday Phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49(1), 129-134. Stone, A.A., Schneider, S. and Harter, J.K. (2012). Day-of-Week Mood Patterns in the United States: On the Existence of Blue Monday, Thank God It s Friday and Weekend Effects. Journal of Positive Psychology 7(4), 306-314. Taylor, M.P. (2006). Tell Me Why I Don t Like Mondays: Investigating Day of the Week Effects on Job Satisfaction and Psychological Well-being. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society) 169(1), 127-142. 13

Tumen, S. and Zeydanli T. (2013). Day-of-the-Week Effects in Subjective Well-Being: Does Selectivity Matter? Social Indicators Research. DOI: 10.1007/s11205-013- 0477-6 Young, C. and Lim C. (2014). Time as a Network Good: Evidence from Unemployment and the Standard Workweek. Sociological Science 1, 10-27. DOI: 10.15195/v1.a2 14

Table 1: Weekend Effects on Emotions for the Working Population Variable Weekends Weekdays Weekend Effect N Mean N Mean Absolute Relative Happiness 296,623 0.922 636,477 0.893 0.030*** 3.3% (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Enjoyment 296,656 0.902 636,549 0.844 0.058*** 6.8% (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Laughter 295,975 0.871 634,774 0.837 0.034*** 4.1% (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Worry 296,878 0.248 637,188 0.328-0.079*** -24.3% (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Sadness 296,923 0.130 637,332 0.144-0.014*** -9.6% (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Anger 296,965 0.111 637,415 0.146-0.036*** -24.4% (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Stress 173,762 0.311 (0.001) 364,162 0.460 (0.001) -0.150*** (0.002) -32.5% Notes: Absolute weekend effect is the difference between emotion on weekends and weekdays. Relative weekend effect is 100% times the absolute weekend effect divided by the average value on weekdays. Standard errors for means and differences are reported in parentheses. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. Table 2: Weekend Effects for Full-Time and Part-Time Workers Happiness Enjoyment Laughter Worry Sadness Anger Stress Panel A: Full-time paid worker State-year fixed 0.033*** 0.065*** 0.037*** -0.085*** -0.014*** -0.041*** -0.168*** effect Full set of controls 0.033*** 0.065*** 0.037*** -0.085*** -0.015*** -0.040*** -0.167*** from Equation (1) Panel B: Part-time paid worker State-year fixed 0.017*** 0.034*** 0.026*** -0.063*** -0.013*** -0.017*** -0.087*** effect Full set of controls 0.017*** 0.034*** 0.026*** -0.063*** -0.014*** -0.018*** -0.087*** from Equation (1) Notes: Each cell of the table reports OLS estimates of the weekend effect. In one model, only state-year fixed effects are controlled. In another model, the covariates include the full set of controls from Equation (1). Panel A and B is for full-time and part-time paid worker respectively. In both regressions, the sample sizes are the same. Standard errors (not reported in the table) to calculate the significance level are clustered within counties. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. 15

Table 3: Balancing Test Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) Weekend Weekdays Difference s.e. Standardized Difference Male 0.539 0.540-0.001 0.001-0.155 Age 42.668 42.468 0.201*** 0.039 1.424 Age squared/100 20.190 20.027 0.164*** 0.033 1.304 Married or living with partner 0.614 0.625-0.010*** 0.001-2.100 Separated, divorced or widowed 0.140 0.136 0.004*** 0.001 1.133 Education High school or vocational school degree/diploma 0.316 0.323-0.007*** 0.001-1.482 Some college 0.241 0.237 0.003** 0.001 0.796 College graduate 0.208 0.210-0.002 0.001-0.409 Post graduate work or degree 0.169 0.161 0.008*** 0.001 2.143 Log household income 9.265 9.195 0.070*** 0.011 1.749 Indicator for missing income 0.149 0.155-0.006*** 0.001-1.685 Church attendance weekly 0.085 0.086-0.001 0.001-0.370 monthly 0.124 0.122 0.002 0.001 0.469 seldom 0.268 0.268 0.000 0.001-0.012 never 0.207 0.204 0.003** 0.001 0.770 Having health insurance 0.848 0.845 0.004** 0.001 1.006 Number of children 0.852 0.869-0.017*** 0.003-1.449 Importance of religion in life 0.623 0.625-0.002 0.001-0.431 Full-time paid worker 0.800 0.795 0.005*** 0.001 1.241 Notes: +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. Standardized difference is calculated by Equation (3). 16

Table 4: Test for Selection Bias (1) (2) (3) Sun-Mon Tue-Mon Weekend effect Happiness 0.035*** 0.001 0.030*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) Enjoyment 0.066*** 0.001 0.058*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) Laughter 0.040*** 0.002 0.034*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) Worry -0.084*** 0.004+ -0.079*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) Sadness -0.015*** 0.002-0.014*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) Anger -0.035*** 0.007** -0.036*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) Stress -0.151*** 0.015*** -0.150*** (0.003) (0.003) (0.002) Notes: The first column shows the difference in emotions between Sunday (being surveyed on Monday) and Monday (being surveyed on Tuesday). The second column shows the difference in emotions between Tuesday (being surveyed on Wednesday) and Monday (being surveyed on Tuesday). The third column shows the difference in emotions between weekends and weekdays. The emotions on Sunday, Monday and Tuesday were reported on Monday, Tuesday, and Wednesday respectively. If there is no real weekend effect, in other words, weekend effect is solely driven by respondents self-selection (into weekends or weekdays), we should not observe large difference between columns (2) and (3), because these effects are calculated from emotions reported in weekdays. Moreover, since column (1) reports weekend effect under potential selection bias (if any) while column (2) reports weekend effect without selection bias, the two results are still very similar suggest that the section bias is minimal, if not zero. Standard errors in parentheses are clustered at counties. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. 17

Table 5: Determinants of Happiness (Yesterday) for Full-Time Workers (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) Weekend 0.025*** 0.000 0.025*** -0.000 0.022*** -0.003* (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Dummy for non-trusting -0.097*** -0.087*** -0.082*** -0.073*** workplace (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) Dummy for non-trusting 0.045*** 0.041*** 0.039*** 0.034*** *Weekend (0.003) (0.003) (0.003) (0.004) Dummy for boss-like -0.058*** -0.055*** -0.031*** -0.030*** supervisor (0.001) (0.002) (0.001) (0.002) Dummy for boss-like 0.025*** 0.025*** 0.012*** 0.014*** supervisor*weekend (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) (0.003) Log social hours 0.053*** 0.054*** 0.053*** (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Dummy for zero social -0.108*** -0.109*** -0.108*** hour (0.006) (0.005) (0.005) Dummy for zero to one -0.057*** -0.059*** -0.057*** social hour (0.005) (0.005) (0.005) Number of observations 611,585 371,519 608,171 369,620 603,706 366,881 Number of counties 3,123 3,116 3,123 3,116 3,123 3,116 Adjusted R-squared 0.031 0.064 0.024 0.059 0.032 0.065 Notes: The odd columns follow Equation (2) and the even columns follow Equation (3). Variables in the vector Xit and state-year dummies are controlled in all models, but coefficients are not reported. The Standard errors in parentheses are clustered within counties. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. 18

Table 6: Determinants of Happiness (Yesterday) for Part-Time Workers (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) Weekend 0.014*** -0.001 0.013*** -0.001 0.013*** -0.002 (0.002) (0.003) (0.003) (0.003) (0.003) (0.003) Dummy for non-trusting -0.081*** -0.078*** -0.073*** -0.070*** workplace (0.005) (0.006) (0.005) (0.006) Dummy for non-trusting 0.010 0.018+ 0.010 0.017 workplace*weekend (0.007) (0.009) (0.008) (0.010) Dummy for boss-like -0.037*** -0.036*** -0.019*** -0.017*** supervisor (0.003) (0.004) (0.003) (0.004) Dummy for boss-like 0.006 0.010 0.004 0.005 supervisor*weekend (0.005) (0.006) (0.005) (0.007) Log social hours 0.054*** 0.054*** 0.053*** (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) Dummy for zero social -0.137*** -0.142*** -0.141*** hour (0.013) (0.013) (0.013) Dummy for zero to one -0.057*** -0.062*** -0.057*** social hour (0.012) (0.013) (0.012) Number of observations 117,302 72,836 116,752 72,559 115,566 71,777 Number of counties 3,004 2,903 3,004 2,903 3,001 2,899 Adjusted R-squared 0.037 0.075 0.031 0.071 0.038 0.076 Notes: The odd columns follow Equation (2) and the even columns follow Equation (3). Variables in the vector Xit and state-year dummies are controlled in all models, but coefficients are not reported. The Standard errors in parentheses are clustered within counties. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. 19

Table 7: OLS Estimates of Weekend Effects for Positive Emotions Not controlling social time Panel A. Dependent Variable: Happiness Full-time workers Controlling social time Not controlling social time Part-time workers Controlling social time Trusting workplace 0.025*** 0.000 0.014*** -0.001 Non-trusting workplace 0.070*** 0.041*** 0.024*** 0.017 + Partner-like supervisor 0.025*** -0.000 0.013*** -0.001 Boss-like supervisor 0.050*** 0.025*** 0.019*** 0.008 Trusting workplace & 0.022*** -0.003* 0.013*** -0.002 partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor 0.073*** 0.045*** 0.026*** 0.020* Panel B. Dependent Variable: Enjoyment Trusting workplace 0.048*** 0.018*** 0.029*** 0.010*** Non-trusting workplace 0.133*** 0.097*** 0.051*** 0.044*** Partner-like supervisor 0.048*** 0.017*** 0.028*** 0.008* Boss-like supervisor 0.097*** 0.066*** 0.042*** 0.030*** Trusting workplace & 0.042*** 0.011*** 0.026*** 0.006 partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor 0.140*** 0.103*** 0.055*** 0.048*** Panel C. Dependent Variable: Laughter Trusting workplace 0.026*** -0.004** 0.020*** -0.000 Non-trusting workplace 0.081*** 0.043*** 0.038*** 0.020 + Partner-like supervisor 0.027*** -0.005** 0.018*** -0.001 Boss-like supervisor 0.057*** 0.025*** 0.031*** 0.010 Trusting workplace & 0.023*** -0.008*** 0.017*** -0.002 partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor 0.085*** 0.048*** 0.043*** 0.023* Notes: Standard errors in parentheses are clustered within counties. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. 20

Table 8: OLS Estimates of Weekend Effects for Negative Emotions Not controlling social time Panel A. Dependent Variable: Worry Full-time workers Controlling social time Not controlling social time Part-time workers Controlling social time Trusting workplace -0.077*** -0.054*** -0.063*** -0.050*** Non-trusting workplace -0.110*** -0.079*** -0.080*** -0.070*** Partner-like supervisor -0.080*** -0.057*** -0.066*** -0.052*** Boss-like supervisor -0.091*** -0.062*** -0.066*** -0.052*** Trusting workplace & -0.078*** -0.056*** -0.059*** -0.051*** partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor -0.110*** -0.078*** -0.079*** -0.067*** Panel B. Dependent Variable: Sadness Trusting workplace -0.010*** -0.000-0.010*** -0.002 Non-trusting workplace -0.032*** -0.018*** -0.031*** -0.017 Partner-like supervisor -0.010*** 0.000-0.011*** -0.001 Boss-like supervisor -0.023*** -0.010*** -0.018*** -0.009 Trusting workplace & -0.008*** 0.001-0.010*** -0.001 partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor -0.034*** -0.019*** -0.033*** -0.021 + Panel C. Dependent Variable: Anger Trusting workplace -0.029*** -0.021*** -0.011*** -0.012*** Non-trusting workplace -0.085*** -0.074*** -0.026** -0.033** Partner-like supervisor -0.030*** -0.021*** -0.013*** -0.015*** Boss-like supervisor -0.058*** -0.050*** -0.015** -0.014* Trusting workplace & -0.025*** -0.018*** -0.012*** -0.013*** partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor -0.088*** -0.077*** -0.026** -0.031*** Panel D. Dependent Variable: Stress Trusting workplace -0.158*** -0.123*** -0.087*** -0.073*** Non-trusting workplace -0.201*** -0.163*** -0.100*** -0.086*** Partner-like supervisor -0.162*** -0.126*** -0.093*** -0.078*** Boss-like supervisor -0.179*** -0.142*** -0.079*** -0.067*** Trusting workplace & -0.158*** -0.123*** -0.093*** -0.078*** partner-like supervisor Non-trusting workplace & boss-like supervisor -0.202*** -0.164*** -0.095*** -0.081*** Notes: Standard errors in parentheses are clustered within counties. +, *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, 1, 0.1% levels respectively. 21

Figure 1. Propensity Score Notes: The upper panel of Figure 1 shows the distribution of propensity scores for people who were actually interviewed on weekends and weekdays, and the lower panel shows the distribution of propensity scores for people who report emotions for weekends (who were interviewed on Sunday and Monday) and weekdays (who were interviewed on Tuesday to Saturday). 22

Appendix Table A1. Summary Statistics of Explanatory Variables Variable N Mean Std. Dev. Min Max Male 935,010 0.540 0.498 0 1 Age 921,566 42.529 14.105 18 99 Married or living with partner 925,843 0.621 0.485 0 1 Separated, divorced, or widowed 925,843 0.137 0.344 0 1 Education High school 925,451 0.321 0.467 0 1 Some college 925,451 0.238 0.426 0 1 College 925,451 0.210 0.407 0 1 Graduate 925,451 0.164 0.370 0 1 Log household income 935,012 9.216 4.008 0 12.283 Dummy for zero or missing income 935,012 0.153 0.360 0 1 Church attendance weekly 905,127 0.086 0.280 0 1 monthly 905,127 0.123 0.328 0 1 seldom 905,127 0.268 0.443 0 1 never 905,127 0.205 0.404 0 1 Having health insurance 934,102 0.846 0.361 0 1 Number of children 933,392 0.864 1.198 0 15 Importance of religion in life 930,609 0.624 0.484 0 1 Full-time paid worker 935,012 0.797 0.402 0 1 Social time with family or friends 558,667 6.024 4.529 0 16 Dummy for zero social hour 558,667 0.038 0.191 0 1 Dummy for zero to one social hour 558,667 0.028 0.164 0 1 Dummy for trustworthy and open workplace 774,382 0.797 0.403 0 1 Dummy for partner-like supervisor 769,743 0.620 0.485 0 1 23