Can We Have Thought without Metaphor?

Similar documents
Felden-WHAT? By Lawrence Wm. Goldfarb , All Rights Reserved

A PCT Primer. Fred Nickols 6/30/2011

PCT 101. A Perceptual Control Theory Primer. Fred Nickols 8/27/2012

ROADMAP FREEDOM FROM STUTTERING. Copyright 2017 Life Quality, Inc. All Rights Reserved

Obstacle- something that obstructs or hinders progress or action.

White Supremacy Culture perfectionism antidotes sense of urgency

2013 JadaCastellari.com all rights reserved

Spring - Restore Your Liver

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

HOW TO INSTANTLY DESTROY NEGATIVE THOUGHTS

Flat Belly Fix Review

The Power of Positive Thinking

#1. What is SAD and how will we resolve it?

RapidRapport. Action Journal. with Michael Bernoff

P O D C A S T Transcript. Dr. Gary Small. Author of 2 Weeks to a Younger Brain

Spring - Restore Your Liver

ORIENTATION SAN FRANCISCO STOP SMOKING PROGRAM

Subliminal Messages: How Do They Work?

Foundations for Success. Unit 3

Lee's Martial Arts. The Five Principles. Principle #1: Preventive Defense. Principle #2: Awareness

What You Will Learn to Do. Linked Core Abilities Build your capacity for life-long learning Treat self and others with respect

5 Quick Tips for Improving Your Emotional Intelligence. and Increasing Your Success in All Areas of Your Life

How to Manage Seemingly Contradictory Facet Results on the MBTI Step II Assessment

With Marie-Claire Ross The SELLSAFE Communication Mentor

The Recovery Journey after a PICU admission

The Top Seven Myths About Hypnosis And the real truth behind them!

Seven Questions to Ask Your Next Dentist

Cardio Blaster. for Wellness Warriors

Stay Married with the FIT Technique Go from Pissed off to Peaceful in Three Simple Steps!

2 INSTRUCTOR GUIDELINES

STEP IT UP Moderate intensity workout

Delirium. Script. So what are the signs and symptoms you are likely to see in this syndrome?

Motivation represents the reasons for people's actions, desires, and needs. Typically, this unit is described as a goal

Overcoming Subconscious Resistances

Tips on How to Better Serve Customers with Various Disabilities

What is Down syndrome?

The Attribute Index - Leadership

Leadership Practices Inventory: LPI

Sightech Vision Systems, Inc. Real World Objects

Planning for Physical

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

19 INSTRUCTOR GUIDELINES

Catalyst of the Mind

5 Ways To Get Rid Of The Baby fat 1

Smoking and Quitting Assessment

We don t see things as they are, we see things as we are!

Managing Your Emotions

The Secret Keys Masterpiece Mindset Principles Summary RealCoach Month One

Recording Transcript Wendy Down Shift #9 Practice Time August 2018

Changing Community Perceptions About Autism

2 Psychological Processes : An Introduction

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette

Week 3 How to Build a Super Confident Self-Image

Warm-up, Stretching & Cool-down. Guide

Intimacy Anorexia: The Book. By Douglas Weiss, Ph.D.

M.O.D.E.R.N. Voice-Hearer

Exploring the Possibility of the Unconscious Imitation of Others Desires 1

Motivational Interviewing. Calvin Miller, CADC, MAATP

Force, work, position, and move are words we hear every day: force work position Move force, work position

Section 4 Decision-making

An Escalation Model of Consciousness

These methods have been explained in partial or complex ways in the original Mind Reading book and our rare Risk Assessment book.

21 Insider Tips To Fitness Success Fitness Success Secrets Of Top Fitness Experts

The Golf Swing Speed Challenge (103) - First 3 Weeks

11 tips for real estate agents with ADHD

Piaget. Updates/Announcements. Overview. Cognitive Development. Jean Piaget. Try this

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) for Physical Health Conditions

OPWDD: Putting People First. Redefining the Role of the DSP Trainer s Manual. Developed by Regional Centers for Workforce Transformation

LPI : Leadership Practices Inventory

12 hours. Your body has eliminates all excess carbon monoxide and your blood oxygen levels become normal.

EYV VOLUNTEERING MATTERS. Impact of Youth Volunteering in Tanzania. May - November 2011

Copyright 2014 FatBurningNation.com

Infinity-Valued Logic. A really powerful way to evaluate, grade, monitor and decide.

Managing Inflammatory Arthritis. What to Discuss with Your Health Care Team

Copyright 2014 FatBurningNation.com

DAY 2 RESULTS WORKSHOP 7 KEYS TO C HANGING A NYTHING IN Y OUR LIFE TODAY!

Wellbeing at Work NZ managing resilience in the workplace symposium. Dr Anne Messervy Dr Aaron Jarden 12 th September, 2016

Overcoming barriers. Our strategy for

We admitted that we were powerless over alcohol that our lives had become unmanageable.

After Soft Tissue Sarcoma Treatment

The Power Of Self-Belief

Mastering Emotions. 1. Physiology

STRETCHES. Diyako Sheikh Mohammadi Sport student at Kajaani University of Applied Sciences, Finland. 25 July 2012

PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSCIOUSNESS AND PHENOMENAL CONSCIOUSNESS. Overview

Phenomenal content. PHIL April 15, 2012

The first in a series of articles by Purplepatch Fitness founder Matt Dixon highlighting his fresh approach to triathlon training.

We admitted that we were powerless over alcohol that our lives had become unmanageable. Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) (2001, p. 59)

Transcript of teachings by Khen Rinpoche Geshe Chonyi

Interacting with people

Information on ADHD for Children, Question and Answer - long version

Practical Brain-Focused Strategies for Working with Depression

Reduce Tension by Making the Desired Choice Easier

Is it possible to give a philosophical definition of sexual desire?

Processing of Logical Functions in the Human Brain

Look to see if they can focus on compassionate attention, compassionate thinking and compassionate behaviour. This is how the person brings their

5 MUSTS FOR BEING MINDFULLY PRESENT SCOTT MAUTZ

Typecasting. Look Behind the Name. article. Stuart Atkins, Ph.D. continued

Introduction Fear Keeps You Small The Twelve Core Human Fears Why Fear? The Eight Tools That Cure Fear...

A Warts-Free Life Arnie Eshlerman

Transcription:

Can We Have Thought without Metaphor? by Zhou Bowen Our minds definitely know our bodies better than we do! Look at the way people drink. Instead of directing the bottle to the mouth, people turn their heads a bit to the left, and then drink from the bottle. You'll be amazed to see how cleverly our brains work in the execution of this simple drinking mechanism. Notice first that turning your head to accommodate the natural crook of your arm may expend less energy than moving your arm out of its natural resting position to accommodate the middle of your face. Secondly, notice that when you turn your head, the bottle won't block your front view. Try this yourself and see how automatic it is! If you have read Andy Clark's "Where Brain, Body, and World Collide," you'll better appreciate the above example. But don't worry if you haven't, because Clark's main idea can be summarized as follows: the main job of the body's brain mechanism is to build an interactive and dynamic system between the body and the environment, and it does this by exploiting both the givens of the body and the givens of the environment so as to achieve maximum efficiency for a specific task. This all sounds well and good for unconscious, automatic processes like drinking, but what if the brain is assigned a more complicated task such as understanding subjective concepts like importance, intimacy, help or time? According to George Lakoff and Mark Johnson, our brains make extensive use of primary metaphors to deal with such concepts--for example, when we reason that important is big (metaphor: "tomorrow is a big day") or time is motion (metaphor: "time flies"). So can we still see the kind of efficiency revealed in the drinking mechanism in our conceptual use of metaphor? The answer is yes. With Clark's brain model in mind, I will demonstrate the efficiency of using primary metaphor in conceptualizing subjective experiences, taking as my starting point some of Lakoff and Johnson's ideas. Primary metaphor, according to Lakoff and Johnson, is a way to conceptualize, reason and visualize subjective experience with reference to other domains of experience, mostly sensorimotor domains of incoming sensory experiences and our own body movements. Lakoff and Johnson have provided a logical account for establishing this metaphoric system based on two integrated theories. The theory of conflation explains the rise of primary metaphor: "For young children, subjective experiences and judgments and sensorimotor experiences are so regularly conflated--undifferentiated in experience--that for a time children do not distinguish between the two when they occur together" (46). For example, the metaphor "affection is warmth" is formed between the "subjective judgment--affection, and sensorimotor domain-- temperature, by the primary experience: feeling warm while being held affectionately" (50). Narayanan's theory is the neural version of this theory:

"'associations' made during the period of conflation are realized neurally in simultaneous activations that result in permanent neural connections being made across the neural networks [of both the subjective and sensorimotor domains]" (46). One of the more intriguing aspects of Lakoff and Johnson's theories is its insistence on the inevitability of primary metaphor: "You inevitably acquire an enormous range of primary metaphors just by going about the world constantly moving and perceiving" (57). That is the equivalent of saying that the brain automatically and unconsciously associates subjective and sensorimotor domains during the childhood conflation period, which directly leads to the formation of metaphor. The profound implication of such an account is that we do not really have a choice regarding the formation of metaphor, for the brain is not even attempting to build the subjective domain "separately." I shall discuss what I believe may be the reason behind this shortly. To do so, I will use Andy Clark's model to prove that metaphor is an extremely efficient solution offered by our bodily and environmentally situated brain to accomplish the task of building a subjective domain. Accordingly, I would like to simplify Clark's model into two rules so as to make my proof easier to understand. And for this proof, I too, will be speaking metaphorically, asking you to visualize that in the example below, "the Man" stands for the brain, "crossing the river" stands for accomplishing a task, and "the construction team" stands for body and environment: Facing a river, if a man had a choice as to whether to walk over a bridge, or first to build a bridge for walking over it, he would opt for the former. (Rule of taking the existent path) If a man had to build a bridge across the river, he would bring along a construction team to carry out the project. (Rule of achieving efficiency by cooperation with body and environment). I would now like to use two examples, one from Clark and the other my own, to explicate these two rules. Firstly, Clark presents the scientific finding that a single-finger movement engages more brain activities than a motion that involves the whole hand grasping--five fingers moving together. This intriguing phenomenon can be explained with reference to Rule 1: "the basic case, from an evolutionary perspective, is the case of whole hand grasping motions (used to grab branches, swings, acquire fruits)" (260). The brain has naturally adapted the whole hand motion, utilized some extra neurons to inhibit the motion of other fingers, and produced a single finger movement. This I call above the rule of taking the existent path (Rule 1). To illustrate the second rule, let's perform a simple experiment. If you type "hi, Bob" on your keyboard, it probably takes less than 2 seconds. Now you are to type exactly the same words, but in a slightly different way. You should first locate the key, say "h," on the keyboard, without thinking about the movement

of your fingers, and then click the key. You will probably find this method of typing slow and inefficient, for it is indeed difficult to visualize the location of "h," simply because "h" is very much likely to be registered as a movement of your index finger. In fact, the brain has cooperated so well with the body that typing becomes "a direct recipe for action" (263). Actually, the brain has another conventional choice for carrying out this typing task, by first locating the position of the key "h," deciding which finger to use, and finally clicking on "h." But as Rule 2 correctly predicts, the brain, rather than a lonely central processing unit that only gives out commands, exploits the body to construct this dynamic typing mechanism. Now that we have the two simple rules, we are in a better position to examine the anatomy of metaphor. I shall first address the question of why the brain doesn't attempt to build a subjective domain separately. Evolutionary theory suggests that in the history of human beings, a well-rounded system of subjective experience emerged much later than the almost innate sensorimotor experience. And we can see from our own experience that we master subjective experience not at infancy, but at a later age. Thus, it is not surprising that the brain decides to accommodate the subjective experiences in the capacious domain of sensorimotor experiences through the ingenious method of metaphor, simply because the latter is an existent path, and Rule 1 indicates that the brain favors an existent path. Furthermore, the sensorimotor domain reveals great prospects for the accommodation of subjective experience in early childhood. There it exhibits a rich potential for "inferential capacity" (59), informing the infant that warmth may be a good substitute for affection, and destination can depict purpose. In this light it seems quite natural that all the diligent neural connections built in early childhood are in fact active preparations for the emergence of the subjective domain. The most profound implication of this sensorimotor-centric model (meaning the sensorimotor domain is the foundation upon which subjective experience is established) is that massive data storage is reduced. You can imagine that inside the subjective domain, most of its elements may be hooked up with correspondents in the sensorimotor domain, such as "affection" with "warmth." These elements alone are merely representations and occupy minimum space. But their seemingly rich contents lie in the fact that they are able to maneuver over their partners. That is to say, if we are to describe the subjective domain as a rich reservoir, its abundance comes from the underground spring of the sensorimotor domain. With this "sensorimotor-centric" rather than "subjectivecentric" model in mind, Lakoff and Johnson's statement--"without metaphor, such concepts are relatively impoverished and have only a minimal, 'skeletal' structure" (58)--itself uses metaphor to make an excellent point. "Skeletal" precisely depicts the emptiness of subjective experience without its sensorimotor part, like a building without a concrete interior. light of the second Rule of cooperation, metaphor reveals another essential

significance. It represents a mechanism in which the brain exploits physical and environmental logic to press for greater efficiency. Bear in mind that the brain initially has two choices for handling subjective concepts: it can either assign that task to a specialized department, the subjective domain, or engage the relatively simpler physical or environmental domain to accomplish the mission "metaphorically." In order to illustrate the efficiency and simplicity of the second, collaborative approach, I shall start off by showing how complicated things may get if the brain were to attempt to understand subjective concepts within the subjective domain only. Allow me to dwell on the primary metaphor "affection is warmth" offered by Lakoff and Johnson one more time. Instead of using a metaphor, if we apply purely subjective reasoning, affection may be described as fondness. But then what exactly is fondness? For fondness is nothing but another subjective concept. After some chucking and humming, the brain decides that fondness is to be depicted as liking, which is best represented by love, which in turn can be explained as adoration. No matter how hard the the brain tries, it always comes up with yet another subjective concept to be addressed. Sadly, while the brain restlessly "kicks the ball" from one subjective concept to another, this exhausting process has not yielded any constructive results at all. Thus we come to the conclusion that it is indeed immensely difficult, if not impossible, to grasp subjective understanding within the subjective domain alone. However, by resorting to metaphor, subjective concepts are tremendously simplified. In the actual workings of the brain, the concept "affection" automatically activates the sense of "warmth" in the sensorimotor domain, which is straightforwardly settled by physical logic (we all know what warmth feels like). In other words, the exhausting part, that involving logic, is disentangled and performed outside the subjective domain. Generally speaking, the premise of this simple and trouble-free solution is that the brain views itself, together with the body and the environment, as an interanimated unity, which is essentially what Rule 2 is all about. Moreover, Rule 2 states that the brain achieves efficiency by exploiting the body and the environment. So there must be metaphors that engage environmental logic, which Lakoff and Johnson have not really mentioned, since most of their examples (such as "affection and warmth") are based on physical, and particularly biological, logic. Sure enough, we can indeed find metaphors that exploit the environment, like "he wants to network with you" or "he tries to upgrade his knowledge by taking night classes." "Network" and "upgrade" originally come from the language of computers, and are purely environmental. It is amazing that the brain is able to utilize these words, which are totally independent of us, to express subjective concepts like "communication" and "improvement" in such a precise and efficient way. In conclusion, Clark's model of the brain has helped us gain an insight into metaphor, a brainchild that exhibits both the brain's favor for "incremental

processes" (259) and its dynamic collaboration with body and environment, resulting in enormous efficiency. Taking this a step further, if we look carefully at the working mechanism of the brain, whether in simple tasks like drinking or in complicated ones like understanding subjective concepts, we may even want to investigate whether our entire biological system builds upon a natural tendency towards efficiency. 1 Certainly, this tendency for efficiency appears to be present in almost every aspect of the body's daily life. But it is important to see that it is not just the brain, and not just the body, but the incredibly powerful brain-body-environment alliance, which has endowed us with this competence that helps ease our complex journey in an even more complex world. Note Thanks to the writing assistant from the USP Writing Centre who helped me clarify my conclusion. Works Cited Clark, Andy. "Where Brain, Body, and World Collide." Daedalus 127 (1998): 256-277.Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books, 1999. About the writer: Zhou Bowen is an Environmental Engineering major who joined the USP in 2004. This paper was written for Don Favareau's Multidisciplinary Perspectives on "Mind." From http://usp.nus.edu.sg/writing/folio/vol4/bowen1.html