The Intelligent Design controversy: lessons from psychology and education. forthcoming in Trends in Cognitive Science, February 2006

Similar documents
From magic to muons: why people believe in strange things

A review of research instruments assessing levels of student acceptance of evolution

WORKSHOP ON MORAL LEARNING. May th, Michigan League, Ann Arbor, MI

COURSE: NURSING RESEARCH CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION

Scenario. Scenario: Scientific Thinking and Evolution

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University

Tree-thinking: Do Pictorial Representations of Evolutionary Relationships Help or Hinder Museum Visitors' Understanding?

Lesson 6 Evolution, Emotion, and Reason

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY. Radwan Banimustafa

What is Psychology? Science versus Intuition 1 a. What is psychology? (from text) b. Note the revision from class:

Comments on Cohen, Mizrahi, Maund, and Levine * Alex Byrne, MIT

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, in press.

UNIT II: RESEARCH METHODS

Workshop on Dying Well Chinese University of Hong Kong Centre for Bioethics November 9-10, 2015

No Lack of Character: Social Psychology and Virtue Ethics. The view that virtues are character traits is commonplace. Both the status of

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Investigating Undergraduate Students Use of Intuitive Reasoning and Evolutionary Knowledge in Explanations of Antibiotic Resistance

Duke Law Journal Online

VIRGINIA LAW REVIEW IN BRIEF

CONCEPT OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR

The idea of an essentially contested concept is incoherent.

Co-Cultures co- cultural

Section 1: The Nature of Science

Final Exam: PSYC 300. Multiple Choice Items (1 point each)

EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY VERSUS INTELLIGENT DESIGN: RESOLVING THE ISSUE 1

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

Chapter 1: Thinking critically with. Introduction

Most scholars recognize that we are a combination Heredity and environmental factors interact with and affect one another

Empty Thoughts: An Explanatory Problem for Higher-Order Theories of Consciousness

Eliminative materialism

Because the Brain Agrees: The Impact of Neuroscientific Explanations for Belief

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

Child Mental Health: A Review of the Scientific Discourse

A Direct Object of Perception

How to Think Straight About Psychology

POLI 343 Introduction to Political Research

Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1

Psychology UNIT 1: PSYCHOLOGY AS A SCIENCE. Core

We Can Test the Experience Machine. Response to Basil SMITH Can We Test the Experience Machine? Ethical Perspectives 18 (2011):

Five Misconceptions of Behavior Analysis 1) It ignores consciousness, feelings, and states of mind. 2) It neglects innate endowment and argues all

624 N. Broadway, 3rd Floor, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA THE EMBRYO RESCUE CASE

The Rationality of Perception rewrites perception s rational role and its rational standing. This

Augmenting Minimalism: A Deep Prototype Theory. Strevens (2000) argues and defends (2001a and 2001b) causal minimalism, a

On A Distinction Between Access and Phenomenal Consciousness

Materialism and the Mind and Body Problem:

Socialization & the Life Cycle. Social Interaction & Everyday Life. Socialization. Nurture vs. Nature (1)

Psychological Approach to Comparative Education Aneela Farooq Afshan Nisar

Changing the Graduate School Experience: Impacts on the Role Identity of Women

PHLA The Philosophy of Mind - II

Evolutionary Psychology (Psych 459) Midterm Exam Winter 2010

The Importance of a Code of Ethics to the Practice of Public Relations

Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne Chapter 1. Chapter 1 An Introduction to Psychology

Response to the ASA s statement on p-values: context, process, and purpose

CHAPTER 2 FOUNDATIONS OF INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR

By Mats Alvesson & Jörgen Sandberg (Constructing Research Questions, Sage 2013)

A Conceptual Guide to Natural History Museum Visitors Understanding of Evolution

Intentional Action and Side Effects in Ordinary Language. The chairman of the board of a company has decided to implement

1 Qualitative Research and Its Use in Sport and Physical Activity

Behaviorists and Behavior Therapy. Historical Background and Key Figures

Thinking Like a Researcher

SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING

Request for Proposals: Junior Investigators in Genetics and Human Agency Letters of Intent Due March 1, 2016

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2

Philosophy of Physical Activity

Running Head: TEACHING EVOLUTION 1

Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research Steps in Nonexperimental Research

Experimental Psychology

Is it possible to gain new knowledge by deduction?

Psychology chapter 16 Test Notes Social Psychology Altruistic Behavior - helping behavior that is not linked to personal gain; recognition and reward

Functionalist theories of content

Test Bank. Chapter 2. Abrams, Sexuality and Its Disorders SAGE Publishing, 2017

Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application

Color Science and Spectrum Inversion: A Reply to Nida-Rümelin

The good ship Theseus: The effect of valence on object identity judgments

Identifying Identity. you is not the equivalence to me. You are different from me and I am different from you,

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Practice for Units 1 & 2

Choose an approach for your research problem

Preservice Secondary Science Teachers Acceptance of Evolutionary Theory and Factors related to Acceptance

Psychology's History and Approaches

Psychology - Linear A level Summer Homework task The study of behaviour and mental processes

Introduction to Attitudes

Stephen Madigan PhD madigan.ca Vancouver School for Narrative Therapy

8/17/2012. What Is Social Psychology? What Is Social Psychology? Chapter 1. Introducing Social Psychology

BLOCK S OVERFLOW ARGUMENT

Consciousness and Theory of Mind: a Common Theory?

Literature Henrich, Joseph, and Natalie Henrich Why Humans Cooperate A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press

Perspectivalism and Blaming

CHIP-2. 7/Oct/2013. Part 0: Concepts and history in psychology. Recap of lecture 1. Preferring theories that address all 3 kinds of data

Ian Rory Owen. Psychotherapy and Phenomenology: On Freud, Husserl and Heidegger. Lincoln, NE: iuniverse, Inc., 2006.

The Discovery/Justification Distinction

Emotions and Moods. Robbins & Judge Organizational Behavior 13th Edition. Bob Stretch Southwestern College

The Science of Psychology. Chapter 1

Prevention for Positives with Motivational Interviewing

Wason's Cards: What is Wrong?

Social-semiotic externalism in Can robot understand values?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Chapter 1 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science

Review of Tim Bayne s The Unity of Consciousness

Transcription:

The Intelligent Design controversy: lessons from psychology and education Tania Lombrozo 1, Andrew Shtulman 1 and Michael Weisberg 2 1 Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138 2 Department of Philosophy, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104 forthcoming in Trends in Cognitive Science, February 2006 Corresponding author: Tania Lombrozo Department of Psychology Harvard University 33 Kirkland Street Cambridge, MA 02138

The current debate over whether to teach Intelligent Design creationism in American public schools provides the rare opportunity to watch the interaction between scientific knowledge and intuitive beliefs play out in courts rather than cortex. While it s easy to believe the controversy stems only from ignorance about evolution, a closer look confirms what decades of research in cognitive and social psychology have already taught us: that the relationship between understanding a claim and believing a claim is far from simple. Research in education and psychology confirms that a majority of college students fail to understand evolutionary theory, but also finds no support for a relationship between understanding evolutionary theory and accepting it as true [1, 2]. We believe the intuitive appeal of Intelligent Design owes as much to misconceptions about science and morality as it does to misconceptions about evolution. To support this position we present a brief tour of misconceptions: evolutionary, scientific, and moral. Numerous studies have shown that students hold highly systematic misconceptions about evolution [3]. These misconceptions conform to internally consistent alternative theories of evolution (akin to Lamark s) and are remarkably impervious to instruction [2, 3]. In particular, most people construe evolution as the simultaneous adaptation of individuals rather than the collective adaptation of a population. On this view, populations evolve because the environment increases the probability that organisms will bear offspring with adaptive traits rather than increasing the probability that organisms with adaptive traits will bear offspring [2]. This misconception may result from a poor understanding of random processes coupled with a tendency to overgeneralize properties of artifact design to evolution [4, 5]. Alternatively, biological essentialism, the commonsense assumption that the outward appearance of a species is determined by some kind of a hidden cause or essence, may predispose individuals to

construe evolution as the transformation of each species member s essence rather than the selective propagation of individual traits [6, 7]. Because people fail to understand the mechanisms of evolution, they also fail to appreciate how empirical evidence bears on evolutionary claims. For example, most college students fail to understand that the presence of an adaptation implies past differential reproduction, or that the absence of within-species variation precludes the operation of natural selection altogether [8]. If a theory is scientific in virtue of its ability to generate testable hypotheses, then individuals who fail to understand how evolutionary claims can be tested may not appreciate why evolutionary theory has a stronger claim to scientific legitimacy than Intelligent Design, which purports to provide a scientific alternative to evolution. Misunderstanding the content and scientific status of evolutionary theory may seem sufficient to account for the general public s ambivalence, but a surprising finding reveals that this cannot be the whole story. As alluded to previously, understanding the rudiments of natural selection is not correlated with accepting natural selection as a mechanism of evolution [1, 2]. Perhaps people are reluctant to accept natural selection because they believe it has undesirable implications. Brem, Ranney, and Schnidel (2003) found that the overwhelming majority of their participants believed evolution to have negative social consequences, like justifying racism and selfishness, and negative philosophical consequences, like denying free will and a purpose to life. These views presumably stem from mistaken beliefs about biology (e.g. that race is a biologically meaningful category or that ultimate explanations reveal proximate intentions) coupled with the naturalistic fallacy (i.e. the belief that one can derive how we ought to behave from a description of how the world actually is). But their findings reinforce the lesson that

understanding a claim and believing a claim are at best fair-weather friends: knowing more about evolution actually strengthened students perceptions that evolution has negative consequences. So what leads some people to accept evolutionary theory while others reject it? Brem, Ranney, and Schnidel (2003) found that students who accepted evolution were exposed to antievolution messages as often as creationists, but to pro-evolution messages more often than creationists. They were also more likely than creationists to believe that evolution has no social or moral consequences, positive or negative. The data don t provide a definite answer, but they do suggest that beliefs about evolution cannot be regarded in isolation. A proper understanding of evolutionary theory and its consequences requires more than a few lessons in biology. It also requires lessons from philosophy of science about what constitutes a scientific theory and an empirical test, and lessons from moral philosophy about the difference between empirical claims and moral claims. Perhaps this is what ought to be taught alongside evolution in America s public schools. 1 Brem, S. K., Ranney, M. & Schindel, J. E. (2003). The perceived consequences of evolution: College students perceive negative personal and social impact in evolutionary theory. Science Education, 87, 181-206. 2 Shtulman, A. (in press). Qualitative differences between naïve and scientific theories of evolution. Cognitive Psychology. 3 Cummins, C.L., Demastes, S.S., & Hafner, M.S. (1994). Evolution: Biological education s under-researched unifying theme. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(5), 445-448.

4 Evans, E. M. (2000). Beyond Scopes: Why creationism is here to stay. In K. Rosengren, C. Johnson, & P. Harris (Eds.) Imagining the impossible: Magical, scientific and religious thinking in children. (pp. 305-331) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 5 Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children Intuitive Theists? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15(5), 295-301. 6 Medin, D. & Atran, S. (2004). The native mind: Biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychological Review, 111(4), 960-983. 7 Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. 8 Lombrozo, T. (2005). Why adaptationist explanations are so seductive. Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 2516.