Tone Production in Mandarin-speaking Children with Cochlear Implants: A Preliminary Study

Similar documents
Relationship Between Tone Perception and Production in Prelingually Deafened Children With Cochlear Implants

2/25/2013. Context Effect on Suprasegmental Cues. Supresegmental Cues. Pitch Contour Identification (PCI) Context Effect with Cochlear Implants

Tone perception of Cantonese-speaking prelingually hearingimpaired children with cochlear implants

Lexical tone recognition in noise in normal-hearing children and prelingually deafened children with cochlear implants

Study on Effect of Voice Analysis Applying on Tone Training Software for Hearing Impaired People

Perception and Production of Mandarin Tones in Prelingually Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants

Lexical Tone Perception with HiResolution and HiResolution 120 Sound-Processing Strategies in Pediatric Mandarin-Speaking Cochlear Implant Users

Speech Perception in Mandarin- and Cantonese- Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants: A Systematic Review

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 22 (1998) Indiana University

Hearing Research 242 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Hearing Research. journal homepage:

Mandarin tone recognition in cochlear-implant subjects q

Critical Review: Speech Perception and Production in Children with Cochlear Implants in Oral and Total Communication Approaches

A dissertation presented to. the faculty of. In partial fulfillment. of the requirements for the degree. Doctor of Philosophy. Ning Zhou.

Prelude Envelope and temporal fine. What's all the fuss? Modulating a wave. Decomposing waveforms. The psychophysics of cochlear

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 26 ( ) Indiana University

Results. Dr.Manal El-Banna: Phoniatrics Prof.Dr.Osama Sobhi: Audiology. Alexandria University, Faculty of Medicine, ENT Department

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

Understanding cochlear implants: a guide for parents and educators

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants

Production of Stop Consonants by Children with Cochlear Implants & Children with Normal Hearing. Danielle Revai University of Wisconsin - Madison

Speech perception and speech intelligibility in children after cochlear implantation

The REAL Story on Spectral Resolution How Does Spectral Resolution Impact Everyday Hearing?

752 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 51, NO. 5, MAY N. Lan*, K. B. Nie, S. K. Gao, and F. G. Zeng

===================================================================

PATTERN ELEMENT HEARING AIDS AND SPEECH ASSESSMENT AND TRAINING Adrian FOURCIN and Evelyn ABBERTON

Role of F0 differences in source segregation

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY Academic in Confidence data removed

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 25 ( ) Indiana University

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 26 ( ) Indiana University

Speech conveys not only linguistic content but. Vocal Emotion Recognition by Normal-Hearing Listeners and Cochlear Implant Users

An Auditory-Model-Based Electrical Stimulation Strategy Incorporating Tonal Information for Cochlear Implant

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 22 (1998) Indiana University. Speech Production by Users of Cochlear Implants:A Review 1

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds

Simulations of high-frequency vocoder on Mandarin speech recognition for acoustic hearing preserved cochlear implant

Analysis of post implantation speech recognition abilities of children with hearing impairment using cochlear implants

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: COCHLEAR IMPLANTS AND AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS

Acoustic cues to tonal contrasts in Mandarin: Implications for cochlear implants

ACOUSTIC ANALYSIS AND PERCEPTION OF CANTONESE VOWELS PRODUCED BY PROFOUNDLY HEARING IMPAIRED ADOLESCENTS

2014 European Phoniatrics Hearing EUHA Award

The Influence of Linguistic Experience on the Cognitive Processing of Pitch in Speech and Nonspeech Sounds

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Bilaterally Combined Electric and Acoustic Hearing in Mandarin-Speaking Listeners: The Population With Poor Residual Hearing

Who are cochlear implants for?

Measuring Auditory Performance Of Pediatric Cochlear Implant Users: What Can Be Learned for Children Who Use Hearing Instruments?

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: COCHLEAR IMPLANTS AND AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS. POLICY NUMBER: CATEGORY: Technology Assessment

Hearing in Noise Test in Subjects With Conductive Hearing Loss

A neural network model for optimizing vowel recognition by cochlear implant listeners

Performance outcome of paediatric pre-lingual cochlear implantation

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair

Normalizing talker variation in the perception of Cantonese level tones: Impact of speech and nonspeech contexts

Speech Perception and Oral Language Development of Deaf Children in Mainstream schools

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Spectrograms (revisited)

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants

TitleSimulation of Cochlear Implant Usin. Citation 音声科学研究 = Studia phonologica (1990),

Auditory Functionality and Early Use of Speech in a Group of Pediatric Cochlear Implant Users

The Deaf Brain. Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre

Music Perception in Cochlear Implant Users

The clinical use of cochlear implants has rekindled the historic controversy

Sound localization psychophysics

Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences. Discovery with delivery as WE BUILD OUR FUTURE

Kaylah Lalonde, Ph.D. 555 N. 30 th Street Omaha, NE (531)

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version***

Speech (Sound) Processing

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Kailey Leroux M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Production of word-initial fricatives of Mandarin Chinese in prelingually deafened children with cochlear implants

Factors Associated with Development of Speech Production Skills in Children Implanted by Age Five

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 23 (1999) Indiana University

Speech perception of hearing aid users versus cochlear implantees

Research Article The Relative Weight of Temporal Envelope Cues in Different Frequency Regions for Mandarin Sentence Recognition

ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES

Speech Cue Weighting in Fricative Consonant Perception in Hearing Impaired Children

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Cochlear Implants Int. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 March 15.

Home based Early Intervention on Auditory and Speech Development in Mandarin speaking Deaf Infants and Toddlers with Chronological Aged 7 24 Months

What is sound? Range of Human Hearing. Sound Waveforms. Speech Acoustics 5/14/2016. The Ear. Threshold of Hearing Weighting

Outcome of Cochlear Implantation at Different Ages from 0 to 6 Years

Auditory Perception and Verbal Intelligibility in Children with Cochlear Implant, Hearing Aids and Normal Hearing

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

COM3502/4502/6502 SPEECH PROCESSING

What you re in for. Who are cochlear implants for? The bottom line. Speech processing schemes for

A PROPOSED MODEL OF SPEECH PERCEPTION SCORES IN CHILDREN WITH IMPAIRED HEARING

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

SPEECH PERCEPTION IN A 3-D WORLD

Using limitations of the auditory system to optimize hearing-aid design

Hearing Research 242 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Hearing Research. journal homepage:

STUDY OF MAIS (MEANINGFUL AUDITORY INTEGRATION SCALE) SCORE POST UNILAT- ERAL COCHLEAR IMPLANTATION IN PRELINGUAL DEAF PATIENTS

REVISED. The effect of reduced dynamic range on speech understanding: Implications for patients with cochlear implants

Feedback and feedforward control in apraxia of speech: Noise masking effects on fricative production.

Mounting evidence suggests that cochlear implants

whether or not the fundamental is actually present.

Preliminary Results of Adult Patients with Digisonic SP Cohlear Implant System

Envelope Versus Fine Structure Speech Coding Strategy: A Crossover Study

CURRICULUM VITA. POSTDOCTORAL: Indiana University, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,

DOES EARLY SIGN LANGUAGE INPUT MAKE A DIFFERENCE ON DEAF CHILDREN WITH AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS?

Simulation of an electro-acoustic implant (EAS) with a hybrid vocoder

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 24 (2000) Indiana University

Transcription:

Acta Otolaryngol 2004; 124: 363/367 Tone Production in Mandarin-speaking Children with Cochlear Implants: A Preliminary Study LI XU 1, YONGXIN LI 2, JIANPING HAO 1, XIUWU CHEN 2, STEVE A. XUE 1 and DEMIN HAN 2 From the 1 School of Hearing, Speech and Language Sciences, Ohio University, Athens, OH, USA and 2 Department of Otolaryngology, Beijing Institute of Otorhinolaryngology, Beijing Tongren Hospital, Beijing, People s Republic of China Xu L, Li Y, Hao J, Chen X, Xue SA, Han D. Tone production in Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants: a preliminary study. Acta Otolaryngol 2004; 124: 363/367. Objective*/More than a quarter of the world s population speak tone languages, such as Mandarin Chinese. In those languages, the pitch or tone pattern of a monosyllabic word conveys lexical meaning. The purpose of this study was to investigate tone production in Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants (CIs). Material and Methods*/Speech samples were recorded from seven normal-hearing and four CI children aged 4 /9 years. All subjects were native Mandarin speakers. The speech samples were used for acoustic analysis of the tone patterns, i.e. the fundamental frequency contours. In addition, a tone intelligibility test was carried out in which four normal-hearing native Mandarin-speaking adults listened to the speech materials and judged the intelligibility of the children s tone production. Results*/The tone production for the seven normal-hearing children was considered to be perfect in the intelligibility test. Acoustic analysis of the speech materials of the normal-hearing children produced the four typical tone patterns of Mandarin Chinese: (i) high and flat; (ii) rising; (iii) low and dipping; and (iv) falling. The tone patterns produced by the children with CIs tended to be flat, with some other patterns being irregular. The results of the tone intelligibility tests also showed degraded intelligibility of tone patterns. Conclusion */A potential speech development deficit was documented in prelingually deafened children with CIs whose native language is a tone language. The imperfect tone production of the implant children, which can be attributed to the paucity of pitch information delivered via the current CI stimulation, may have significant implications for communication using tone languages. Further research is warranted to determine factors that may affect tone development in children with CIs. Key words: cochlear prosthesis, pitch, tone language. INTRODUCTION Contemporary multichannel cochlear implants (CIs) have provided benefit to thousands of patients with profound hearing loss throughout the world. The number of implant recipients in China and Southeast Asia has increased dramatically in recent years. At Beijing Tongren Hospital alone, the total number of cochlear implantees has reached 350 since the CI program was started only 8 years ago. The number of new implantees is expected to be :/ 140 per year, 90% of whom will be children. In China and Southeast Asia, tone languages are most prevalent. These include Mandarin Chinese, Cantonese, Vietnamese, Thai, etc. In these tone languages, the tone pattern (or simply tone), as defined by the fundamental frequency (F0) of the voiced part of a syllable, varies. In Mandarin Chinese, a language spoken by more people than any other, there are four tone patterns. The pitch contours of tones 1/4 are (i) flat and high, (ii) rising, (iii) low and dipping and (iv) falling, respectively. The unique characteristic of the tone language is that the variations of the tone patterns of each syllable convey lexical meaning. For instance, tones 1 /4, when associated with the Chinese syllable /qi/ (pronounced chee ), can mean (i) seven, (ii) flag, (iii) get up and (iv) air, respectively. Given the current understanding of electrical hearing and pitch coding, pitch information of complex acoustic stimuli, such as speech and music, is unlikely to be strong and is probably lacking in contemporary CI systems [for a review, see Moore (1)]. Sporadic reports concerning a limited number of native Chinese-speaking patients have shown that the tone recognition performances of these patients vary tremendously, ranging from chance performance to :/ 80% correct (2/5). Two recent studies on tone recognition in Cantonese children with CIs have shown that they perform just slightly above chance levels, suggesting that children with CIs have great difficulty in extracting the pitch information needed to identify the lexical tones (6, 7). Auditory input is essential for normal speech and language development. How do children with CIs develop tone production under conditions of impoverished auditory inputs? This study was designed to explore tone development in children with CIs whose native language is Mandarin Chinese. MATERIAL AND METHODS Four prelingually deafened children (S1/S4; age range 4.0 /8.75 years) in whom hearing loss had been identified at birth and who received Nucleus 24M CIs at Beijing Tongren Hospital participated in the # Taylor & Francis 2004. ISSN 0001-6489 DOI 10.1080/00016480410016351

364 L. Xu et al. Acta Otolaryngol 124 Table I. Demographic information for the four CI subjects Subject No. Age (years) Sex Etiology of deafness Age at activation (years) Duration of CI use (years) S1 8.75 F Unknown 3.75 5.0 SPEAK S2 5.91 F Unknown 3.58 2.33 ACE S3 5.91 F Ototoxicity 3.83 2.08 ACE S4 4.0 F Congenital 3.0 1.0 SPEAK Speech-processing strategy study. Demographic information for the subjects is shown in Table I. As controls, seven normal-hearing children (N1/N7; age range 3.0 and 8.5 years) were also recruited. All subjects were from native Mandarin-speaking families, with Mandarin Chinese being the only language used. Speech samples from both normal-hearing children and children with CIs were recorded in a soundtreated room. Speech was elicited by asking the subjects to count from 1 to 10 in Mandarin Chinese. Imitated speech was also elicited by asking the children to repeat after an adult native Mandarin speaker the 4 tone patterns of the 10 sets of Mandarin syllables (/fu/, /ji/, /ma/, /qi/, /shi/, /wan/, /xi/, /yan/, / yang/ and /yi/) that have all 4 tone patterns. This resulted in 40 speech samples (4 tone patterns /10 syllables). The speech materials were stored on tape and then transferred to the hard disk of a computer with a sampling rate of 22 050 Hz in a 16-bit format. Acoustic analysis of the speech materials focused on the extraction of F0s, i.e. the tone patterns. The 40 imitated speech samples were subjected to F0 extraction. For 1 of the CI subjects, S4, the speech sample obtained by counting from 1 to 10 was used instead, because it was not possible to elicit imitated speech from her. First, the consonant part of the syllables was removed from the speech sample using a sound processing program (CoolEdit 2000; Syntrillium Software, Scottsdale, AZ). Next, the F0s were extracted from the vowel part of the syllables using an autocorrelation method (8) in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Finally, a post-processing procedure was undertaken to fix any errors in F0 measured with the autocorrelation method, as F0 doubling or halving did occur. In this procedure, the F0s obtained with the autocorrelation analysis were plotted over the narrowband spectrograms. Corrections were necessary when the F0 plotted was not consistent with the F0 shown on the spectrograms. In addition to the acoustic analysis of the F0s of the speech materials, a tone-pattern intelligibility test was performed by four normal-hearing native Mandarinspeaking adult subjects. These adult subjects listened to the imitated speech and the speech of counting and then provided a subjective score for the intelligibility of the tone patterns for each of the child subjects. The scores were integers between 0 and 10, with 0 being no tonality at all and 10 being a perfect score. RESULTS Examples of the spectrograms of the four tone patterns of the vowel part of the Chinese syllable /qi/ are shown in Fig. 1. The upper and lower panels represent speech data recorded from a normal-hearing subject (N7) and a child with a CI (S3), respectively. The F0s extracted by means of the autocorrelation analysis are plotted as white dotted lines on the spectrograms, confirming the accuracy of the F0 measurements. The tone patterns can be seen from the contours of the F0s and more clearly from the contours of the higher harmonics. The F0s for the normal-hearing child showed the typical flat and high (tone 1), rising (tone 2), low and dipping (tone 3) and falling (tone 4) contours. It is noteworthy that tone 3 in our sample of normal-hearing children tended to show the low and dipping rather than the falling and then rising contours that we commonly saw in adults. In either case, tone 3 was readily perceived. The CI child s production of tone 4 was similar to that of the normal subjects but tones 1 /3 were essentially indistinguishable. Tone production in children with CIs showed great individual differences. Fig. 2 plots the F0 contours of tones 1/4 in one of the normal-hearing subjects (N7) and all four children with CIs (S1 /S4). Each line shows a tone contour for the vowel part of one of the Mandarin syllables. As stated previously, 10 syllables were recorded for each tone pattern for all the subjects except for S4, whose speech data were obtained only by counting from 1 to 10 in Mandarin Chinese. Of the Chinese words for 1 to 10, 4 are in tone 1, 1 in tone 2, 2 in tone 3, and 3 in tone 4. The tone contours in the upper group of panels are plotted using the actual time of speech production; therefore, the duration of the pronunciation of the vowels and the rate of change of a particular tone pattern can be seen from these plots. On the other hand, when the duration of the vowels was normalized to the mean duration of all vowels (318 ms in our sample), the plot of tone contours allowed easy comparison between different subjects and between different tones (Fig. 2, lower panels).

Acta Otolaryngol 124 Tone production by CI children 365 Fig. 1. Spectrograms of speech recordings from a normalhearing child (N7; upper panels) and a child with a CI (S3; lower panels). Only the vowel parts are plotted in the spectrograms, in which red or dark red colors represent the energy associated with time (abscissa) and frequency (ordinate). The F0s extracted by means of autocorrelation analysis are plotted using white dotted lines. The tone patterns of the normal-hearing subject showed the typical flat and high (tone 1), rising (tone 2), low or low and dipping (tone 3) and falling (tone 4) contours for all the syllables. The CI children made various errors in tone production. S1 was the best performer, her tone contours being nearly identical to those of a normal-hearing subject. For tone 3, she tended to use glottal stops rather than low and dipping contours, which is considered normal (9). S2 made a lot more tone-production errors than S1. Most of these errors consisted of an inability to produce a rising tone 2. Also, the F0 patterns for tone 3 varied among syllables and tended to be greatly prolonged, reflecting an added effort in producing this tone pattern. S3 produced rapidly falling tone patterns for Fig. 2. Tone patterns produced by a normal-hearing child (N7) and the four children with CIs (S1 /S4). Each line represents the F0 of the vowel part of a Mandarin syllable produced by a subject. The upper group of panels show the F0 contours of the original durations. The lower group of panels show the F0 contours using a normalized duration that is the mean duration of the vowel parts of all recordings (318 ms). tone 4 (Fig. 2, upper panels). Except for one of the syllables, her tones 1/3 were all monotonic, showing a flat or sometimes slightly rising pattern (Figs. 1 and 2). Producing the falling tone pattern of tone 4 appeared to be easiest for the children with CIs. Despite all kinds of errors seen in S1/S3, their production of tone

366 L. Xu et al. Acta Otolaryngol 124 4 was quite similar to that of the normal-hearing subjects. S4, however, demonstrated great difficulty in producing all tone patterns, including that for tone 4. The subjective scores for the tone-pattern intelligibility test are shown in Fig. 3. All seven normalhearing subjects received perfect scores for the intelligibility of their tone production. Therefore, only one bar is shown in Fig. 3 to represent the whole group of normal-hearing children. The mean subjective intelligibility scores for the four CI children ranged from 0.25 to 8.5. The differences in intelligibility scores between the normal-hearing children and the CI children and the differences in the scores among the CI children were found to be statistically significant (p B/0.05; ANOVA). The variability of the scores provided by the four normal-hearing adult judges was small, as shown by the error bars (SDs) and the scatter of the original data. DISCUSSION CIs have provided significant benefit in terms of both receptive and expressive language skills for hearingimpaired children. It has been demonstrated in a number of studies that speech produced by Englishspeaking children with multichannel CIs is usually more accurate than that produced by children with comparable hearing loss but without CIs (10 /14). The auditory feedback provided by the CIs is the critical Fig. 3. Subjective intelligibility scores. The bars show the means and SDs of the intelligibility scores given by four normal-hearing adult subjects after listening to the speech materials from the normal-hearing and CI children. As all seven normal-hearing children received perfect scores for the intelligibility of their tone production, only one bar is plotted to represent the whole group of normal-hearing children. The dots show the individual scores given by the four adult judges. factor attributed to the unprecedented achievement in language skills of hearing-impaired children. Tone production in children with CIs was not studied in the previous research. Tone production is particularly important for native tone-language speakers, because the pitch of a syllable conveys lexical meaning. In the present study we evaluated tone production in a small group of native Mandarinspeaking children with CIs. All CI children showed imperfect tone production, making their speech intelligibility scores lower than those of the normal controls. Such a deficit in tone development is probably due to inadequate pitch information delivered via the CI stimulation. Contemporary speechprocessing strategies for CIs generally have a limited number of frequency bands. Therefore, the F0 and higher harmonics cannot be resolved for pitch perception. Whereas the fine structure information within each band can potentially be useful for pitch perception (15), current speech-processing strategies emphasize the temporal envelope information and discard the fine structure information. We have shown (16) that tone perception using only the temporal envelope information in the acoustic signals is only moderately good. The question is whether such weak pitch information will be sufficient to sustain normal tone production development in children. The preliminary data from the present study suggest that it is not. Our data also showed enormous individual differences in tone production in the children with CIs. Many factors may have contributed to those differences. Factors that affect English speech production skills in children should be considered. Tobey et al. (13) found that English speech production performance in children with CIs is influenced by non-verbal intelligence, gender, implant characteristics and educational programs. Although not found to be major contributors to English speech production performance in that study, other factors, such as age of onset of deafness, hearing aid usage, duration of deafness, age of implantation and speech-processing strategy used in the CI system, should also be considered in future studies of tone production development in children with CIs. Our preliminary data from the present study cannot resolve any of those factors due to the small number of subjects involved but hopefully will open a new area of research. REFERENCES 1. Moore BCJ. Coding of sounds in the auditory system and its relevance to signal processing and coding in cochlear implants. Otol Neurootol 2003; 24: 243/54.

Acta Otolaryngol 124 Tone production by CI children 367 2. Huang TS, Wang NM, Liu SY. Nucleus 22-channel cochlear mini-system implantations in Mandarin-speaking patients. Am J Otol 1996; 17: 46/52. 3. Sun JC, Skinner MW, Liu SY, Wang FNM, Huang TS, Lin T. Optimization of speech processor fitting strategies for Chinese-speaking cochlear implantees. Laryngoscope 1998; 108: 560 /8. 4. Wei WI, Wong R, Hui Y, Au DKK, Wong BYK, Ho WK, et al. Chinese tonal language rehabilitation following cochlear implantation in children. Acta Otolaryngol 2000; 120: 218 /21. 5. Wei CH, Zeng FG, Cao KL, Wang ZZ. Rate discrimination and tone recognition in Chinese cochlear implant patients. Abstracts of the 24th Association for Research in Otolaryngology Midwinter Meeting, St. Petersburg Beach, FL, 4 /8 February, 2001: 293. 6. Ciocca V, Francis AL, Aisha R, Wong L. The perception of Cantonese lexical tones by early-deafened cochlear implantees. J Acoust Soc Am 2002; 111: 2250/6. 7. Lee KYS, van Hasselt CA, Chiu SN, Cheung DMC. Cantonese tone perception ability of cochlear implant children in comparison with normal-hearing children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2002; 63: 137 /47. 8. Kent RD, Read C. Acoustic analysis of speech, 2nd edn. Albany, NY: Singular; 2002. 9. Duanmu S. The phonology of standard Chinese. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2002. 10. Svirsky MA, Robbins AM, Kirk KI, Pisoni DB. Miyamoto RT. Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants. Psychol Sci 2000; 11: 153/8. 11. Matthies ML, Svirsky M, Perkell J, Lane H. Acoustic and articulatory measures of sibilant production with and without auditory feedback from a cochlear implant. J Speech Hear Res 1996; 39: 936/46. 12. Geers A, Brenner C, Davidson L. Factors associated with development of speech perception skills in children implanted by age five. Ear Hear 2003; 24: 24S/35S. 13. Tobey EA, Geers AE, Brenner C, Altuna D, Gabbert G. Factors associated with development of speech production skills in children implanted by age five. Ear Hear 2003; 24: 36S/45S. 14. Uchanski RM, Geers AE. Acoustic characteristics of the speech of young cochlear implant users: a comparison with normal-hearing age-mates. Ear Hear 2003; 24: 90S/105S. 15. Xu L, Pfingst BE. Relative importance of temporal envelope and fine structure in lexical-tone perception. J Acoust Soc Am 2003; 114: 3024/7. 16. Xu L, Tsai Y, Pfingst BE. Features of stimulation affecting tonal-speech perception: implications for cochlear prostheses. J Acoust Soc Am 2002; 112: 247 /58. Address for correspondence: Li Xu, MD, PhD School of Hearing, Speech and Language Sciences Grover Center*/W224 Ohio University Athens, OH 45701 USA Tel.: /1 740 593 0310 Fax: /1 740 593 0287 E-mail: xul@ohio.edu