Attitudes, Labeling Bias, and Behavior Modification in Work Organizations

Similar documents
REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE

Abstract. Key words: bias, culture, Five-Factor Model, language, NEO-PI-R, NEO-PI-3, personality, South Africa

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I)

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal


Thinking Like a Researcher

Psychology Unit 1 Test

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question

Attitudes, Self- Concept, Values, and Ethics

IDEA Technical Report No. 20. Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators. Stephen L. Benton Dan Li

Overview of the Logic and Language of Psychology Research

CHAPTER 15 MOTIVATION

TLQ Reliability, Validity and Norms

Motivation and Emotion. AP Psychology

EFFECTS OF LEADER PERSISTENCE AND LEADER BEHAVIOR ON LEADERSHIP PERCEPTIONS

Job Choice and Post Decision Dissonance1

The State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI)

Title page. Ethical stance among senior business and marketing students at Macquarie University

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs

Organizational Behaviour

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

Examining of adaptability of academic optimism scale into Turkish language

Organizational Behaviour

The relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behavior

CHAPTER V. Summary and Recommendations. policies, including uniforms (Behling, 1994). The purpose of this study was to

STUDENT GUIDE NAVEDTRA A CHAPTER 9 BASIC NEEDS AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR

MC GREGOR S XY THEORY. Assoc. Prof. Zuzana KAPSDORFEROVÁ, PhD. Ing. Veronika HRDÁ, PhD., MBA

PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF CLINICAL PERFORMANCE RATINGS

Self-Determination in a Work Organization

Dikran J. Martin Psychology 111

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Attitude Measurement

Validity and Reliability of Sport Satisfaction

The Effects of Gender Role on Perceived Job Stress

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D.

Schools of Psychology

MGMT 301 Exam 3 Answer Key 2. You would use an Autocratic leadership style in a CS call center

Relationship of Stress Coping Strategies and Life Satisfaction among Students

Studying the Relation between Emotional Intelligence and Academic Achievement among the Students of Buinzahra Islamic Azad University

Linking Locus of Control with Demographic Attributes: An Empirical Study on Defence Manufacturing Company in Bangalore, India

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

Teacher satisfaction: some practical implications for teacher professional development models

Can Leaders Step Outside of the Gender Box? An Examination of Leadership and Gender Role Stereotypes

Effect of job characteristics on satisfaction and performance: A test in Egyptian agricultural extension system

Validity and reliability of physical education teachers' beliefs and intentions toward teaching students with disabilities (TBITSD) questionnaire

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Does the Selection of Sample Influence the Statistical Output?

What are the Relationships Between Transformational Leadership and Organizational Citizenship Behavior? An Empirical Study

Teachers Sense of Efficacy Scale: The Study of Validity and Reliability

Who is happy and why? Subjective well-being and associated thinking styles of US and Canadian students.

Macquarie University ResearchOnline

MALE AND FEMALE LEADERSHIP SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES

MHR Chapter 5. Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour

The relationship between, style leadership coaches and achievement motivation female athletes fitness field of Bojnoord city

Basis for Conclusions: ISA 230 (Redrafted), Audit Documentation

MODULE 5 Motivation Definition of Motivation Work Motivation Work Motivation Sources of Motivation

Psychological Approaches to Counseling. Mr. Lema, Isaac Clinical Psychologist (MSc.) 25 th November 2015

Type of Education, Locus of Control and Optimistic - Pessimistic. Attitude among Undergraduate College Students

Extension of the Children's Perceptions of Interparental Conflict Scale for Use With Late Adolescents

Empowerment At Higher Educational Organizations

Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1

Normative Outcomes Scale: Measuring Internal Self Moderation

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9.

Finding Cultural Differences and Motivation Factors of Foreign Construction Workers

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

The Role of Motivation in Health Education

The Stigma of Obesity in Training Procedures. Jenessa Shapiro. RUSP proposal draft

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

THE JOB SATISFACITON AND MOTIVATION. A Comparison between. Job Satisfaction of Librarians: Men and W oinen SUSANNE PATTERSON WAHBA I 45

PHASE 1 OCDA Scale Results: Psychometric Assessment and Descriptive Statistics for Partner Libraries

CHAPTER 6 BASIS MOTIVATION CONCEPTS

Dr. Adeniyi Mofoluwake. MPH 543 Leadership and Organizational Management. Week 3 Case Study Analysis 2. Instructor Dr.

Marisa Puckett and Geraldine E. Hynes, Ph.D. Sam Houston State University Huntsville, Texas

Role Ambiguity, Role Conflict, and Satisfaction: Moderating Effects of Individual Differences

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Employment Counseling Program (ECP) for Young Adults with Autistic Spectrum Disorders 3 rd Annual Penn Autism Network Conference March 28, 2008

Honours psychology students perceptions of job-related skills

LIMITATIONS, SUGGESTIONS AND APPLICATIONS. It is true to highlight that every research has its own limitations whether it is

An International Multi-Disciplinary Journal, Ethiopia Vol. 4 (1) January, 2010

MCORE TECHNICAL REPORT: DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER

ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY MA 204-1

Hindsight Bias: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. STEVEN J. HEINE and DARRIN R. LEHMAN University of British Columbia, Canada

Effect of sports participation on locus of control and personality traits and self -efficacy among interuniversity sportsperson

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND ETHICAL COMPETENCE: AN EMPIRICAL STUDY

6. Athletes often attribute their losses to bad officiating. This best illustrates A) an Electra complex. B) learned helplessness. C) the spotlight ef

What is Psychology? chapter 1

ATTITUDES PART 1: STRUCTURE AND MEASURES SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION ANNA SPAGNOLLI

Me 9. Time. When It's. for a. By Robert Tannenboum./ Industrial'Relations

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

The Art of Empowerment. Lynn E. Lawrence, CMSgt(ret), USAF CPOT, ABOC, COA, OSC Consultant

relationships (i.e., attribution schemal). The I-E Scale Peterborough, Ontario, Canada ROTTER S (1968) Internal-External Locus of Control (I-E) Scale

Epistemological Beliefs and Leadership Style among School Principals

Reliability and Exploratory Factor Analysis of Psychological Well-being in a Persian Sample

Chapter 2: Research Methods in I/O Psychology Research a formal process by which knowledge is produced and understood Generalizability the extent to

Transcription:

BEHAVIOR THERAPY 10, 129--136 (1979) Attitudes, Labeling Bias, and Behavior Modification in Work Organizations JULIAN BARLING AND TREVOR WAINSTEIN University of the Witwatersrand The present study investigated a labeling bias against behavior modification in work organizations. Groups of individuals in favor or opposed to the use of behavior modification in industry were formed. The identical videotape of a performance appraisal interview was shown to these subjects after half of each group had received a humanistic, and the other half a behavior modification, description of the videotape's contents. The 2 2 (Label Attitude) analyses of variance revealed no evidence of a labeling bias. Although both method and leadership characteristics were rated more positively when the videotape was said to be descriptive of humanistic techniques, a significant Label Attitude interaction emerged. The failure to yield any labeling bias is contrasted with previous research, while the need for a more "humanistic" terminology in describing behavioral programs is emphasized. Behavior modification has struggled to achieve popular acceptance within psychology (Lazarus, 1977). This issue is of some importance, since it may influence the extent to which professionals in different settings utilize behavior modification techniques, regardless of their empirically demonstrated efficacies. Two examples of a possible "labeling bias" against behavior modification have been demonstrated in different settings. First, Reppucci and Saunders (1974) documented an attempted implementation of a behavior modification program which was not accepted by the staff members concerned until the terminology of the program was changed, "behavior modification" becoming the less value-charged term "social learning." Second, Woolfolk, Woolfolk, and Wilson (1977) found that a humanistic description of an educational strategy was accepted more readily than a behavior modification description, although prior attitudes to behavior modification in general were not controlled. The authors express their thanks to Ian M. Evans, Alma Hannon, and Chris Orpen for their constructive suggestions regarding earlier versions of this manuscript; to Mike Saling for statistical advice; and to Prof. W. Musgrove for his kind permission in allowing the Behavior Modification Attitude Scale to be revised. Reprints may be obtained from Julian Barling, Department of Psychology, University of the Witwatersrand, 1 Jan Smuts Avenue, Johannesburg 2001, South Africa. 129 0005-7894/79/010129-08501.00/0 Copyright 1979 by Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

130 BARKING AND WAINSTEIN Recently, behavior modification has been applied within work organizations (cf. Franks & Wilson, 1977; Luthans & Kreitner, 1975; Saad & Barling, 1977). The importance of attitudes toward this implementation is acknowledged (Dubno, Hillburn, Robinson, Sandier, Trani, & Weingarten, 1978). Interestingly, both Hammer (1971) and Fry (1974) have criticized behavior modification in work organizations on the grounds that it is based largely on operant conditioning using animal studies; indeed, Hammer (1971) refers to organizational behavior modification as "applied ratamorphism." Both these authors, however, largely ignore any research concentrating on the application of operant conditioning principles to human behavior in work organizations (cf. Franks & Wilson, 1976; Schneier, 1974). It is feasible that the term "organizational behavior modification" itself may elicit unfavorable reactions, largely on the basis of the label and terminology rather than the particular technology involved. The present study was therefore a constructive replication of the earlier study of Woolfolk et al. (1977), in that the possible influence of the label "organizational behavior modification" on subsequent attitudes was examined. It was predicted that a negative labeling bias would exist against behavior modification in the industrial and organizational milieu. METHOD Subjects. The present study was divided into two sections. In the first, 112 subjects completed a revised edition of _Musgrove's (1974) Behavior Modification Attitude Scale. The subjects (X age = 21.78 years, SD = 1.68) comprised third-year undergraduate students (n = 90) and first-year postgraduate students (n = 6) involved in separate year-long courses in industrial psychology and organizational behavior and final-year undergraduate students majoring in psychology (n = 16). The second part of the study involved 32 of these subjects ()~ age = 21.47 years, SD = 1.41; 25 males, 7 females; 24 third-year industrial and organizational behavior students, 5 final-year psychology undergraduates, and 3 psychology postgraduates) who had been contacted on the basis of their scores on the revised version of the Behavior Modification Attitude Scale; all had consented to participate in this section of the study. All subjects had expressed an interest in using the knowledge gained about organizational behavior modification in the above courses once assuming full-time employment. Assessment. To assess attitudes to the use of behavior modification in work organizations, all subjects (n = 112) initially completed a revised version of Musgrove's (1974) scale; the original scale assessed overall attitudes to behavior modification in the educational setting. For the purposes of the present study, the 20 items had been reworded to reflect the industrial context. The mean score of 66.73 on this revised scale (SD = 13.96; minimum = 32, maximum = 89) was extremely similar to that of the original (,( = 64.28, SD = 13.31; minimum = 20, maximum = 96). In

ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION 131 its revised format, the scale had a Cronbach's a reliability coefficient of.86 and a split-half reliability of.91. Parker' s (1976) shortened form of Taylor and Bowers' (1972) Survey of Organizations was administered to assess attitudes concerning supervisory leadership. This comprised four dimensions, viz., managerial support (3 items), goal emphasis (2 items), work facilitation (3 items), and interaction facilitation (2 items), each of which was rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale. In addition, all the items comprising the organizational climate factor were administered (cf. Franklin, 1975). This consisted of four different subscales, viz., decision-making practices (4 items), human resources primacy (3 items), communication flow (3 items), and motivational conditions (3 items). The 18-item Likert-type scale and 15-item semantic differential of Woolfolk et al. (1977) were slightly reworded to reflect the work context (e.g., "teacher warmth" became "manager warmth;... teacher was a good model for me" became "manager was a good model for me"). The eight factors generated from their study, viz., manager evaluation (6 items), method efficacy (5 items), interest in method (4 items), manager flexibility (2 items), method quality (7 items), emotionality (4 items), tedium (2 items), and idealism (1 item) were thus administered. The 16 subscales administered characterized numerous different aspects of organizational behavior. To establish any possible commonality among these 16 different dimensions, a principal-components factor analysis with varimax rotation was performed. Three factors with an eigenvalue greater than 1 emerged, while the formula 3/X/-N (Mauer, Note l) was used to define the meaning of the various factors (see Table 1). This formula allows for the relatively small sample size utilized in this part of the study. Factor 1 was labeled method characteristics and defined by subscales 1 through 9. Factor 2, labeled leadership characteristics, comprised subscales 10 through 13. Finally, Factor 3, comprising items 14 and 15, was labeled perceived organizational climate. 1 Only the first two factors served as dependent variables. The subscale "work facilitation" did not load significantly on any of the factors. In summing the relevant subscales to obtain separate factor indexes, work facilitation was thus excluded. A 6-min, 38-sec videotape depicting a 3-month performance appraisal session between a manager and his line supervisor regarding the latter's performance was shown. During the interview, the manager contingently reinforced his line supervisor's self-monitored performance whenever their mutually predetermined goals had been attained. The line supervisor The possible importance of this factor is minimized for three reasons. First, only two subscales loaded significantly on this factor: it is doubtful, however, whether "doublets" are sufficiently robust to constitute a separate factor. Second, this factor only accounted for a minimal amount of variance (l 1.8%). Third, perceived organizational climate is somewhat unstable and difficult to define (Franks & Wilson, 1977). This factor is therefore disregarded.

132 BARL1NG AND WAINSTEIN TABLE 1 VARIMAX ROTATED FACTOR STRUCTURE ~l Factors 1 2 3 Communality 1. Method efficacy.97.05.01.98 2. Manager evaluation.97.20.11.95 3. Method quality.96.18.05.97 4. Managerial support.94.09,20.94 5. Interest.89.05,03.90 6. Emotionality.88.02.10.87 7. Idealism.78.52.02.91 8. Communication.75.46.27.86 9. Motivation.60.34.07.59 10. Interaction facilitation.14.89.07.80 11. Goal emphasis.13.84.02,78 12. Flexibility.43.62.12.63 13. Tedium.44.60.32.79 14. Human resources primacy.33.12.82.72 15. Decision making.33.08.70.59 16. Work facilitation.50.33.03.52 Eigenvalue 7.65 3.06 1.43 Variance 63.00 25.20 11.80 a Item loadings of.53 and above define the meaning of a factor. was also required to monitor her workers to assess whether a decrease in absenteeism and the number of items rejected by the quality control inspector had been achieved. After identifying potential problem areas and setting goals for her workers for the following 3-month period, she was again socially reinforced for her performance. Procedure. On the basis of the median score (69.44; SD = 13,96) on the revised version of Musgrove's (1974) scale, subjects were divided into two groups, one favorably disposed toward and the other relatively antagonistic regarding the use of behavior modification in work organizations. Thirty-two subjects whose scores placed them at the extreme of these two groups were then contacted and asked to volunteer for an experiment that would involve their viewing and rating a training videotape used extensively in a large South African organization. The two groups formed differed significantly regarding their attitudes to organizational behavior modification [X 75.19 vs 57.95; t(31) = 6.82, p <.001, two tailed]. In all situations, subjects viewed the same videotape; only the description of the videotape's contents given the subjects was manipulated. Half of the subjects in each attitude group (n = 8) were randomly

ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION 133 assigned to an organizational behavior modification description, the rest receiving a humanistic description of the identical videotape." Two different paragraphs were used to introduce the experimental manipulations and were based on the descriptions of Woolfolk et al. (1977). The organizational behavior modification descriptions were introduced as follows: The purpose of this videotape is to introduce to you a significant development in industrial psychology, viz., the use of "organizational behavior modification," which is sometimes called O B Mod. This technique is based largely on the work of B. F. Skinner. Following success in controlling the behavior of laboratory animals, Skinner and other behavioristic psychologists concluded that to be optimally effective, managers, trainers, foremen and the like should behave in the industrial situation in a similar fashion to that of the experimental psychologist in the laboratory. Thus, it would be the responsibility of the manager, trainer, foreman to determine the final ("target") behaviors that are required of his/her subordinates. Following this, their behavior should be systematically shaped by presenting the correct reinforcements following the appropriate behavior. The humanistic condition was introduced as such: The purpose of this videotape is to introduce to you a significant development in industrial psychology, viz., the use of a humanistic approach which is sometimes called Need Hierarchy Theory. This technique is based largely on the work of A. H. Maslow. Recognizing the importance of both feelings and cognitive activities in the overall pattern of the individual's personality functioning, Maslow and other humanistic psychologists concluded that to be optimally effective, managers, trainers, foremen and the like should function in the industrial situation in a fashion that enhances self-esteem and fosters self-awareness in the climate of a warm, accepting, interpersonal relationship. Thus, it would be the responsibility of the manager, trainer or foreman to attempt to develop such a growth-potentiating relationship with his/her fellow workers to foster the attainment of the objectives. Within this context, individuals would develop to the point where the worker would be able to fulfill organizational goals. In both cases, the actual descriptions of the videotape's contents given the subjects closely followed the jargon used in the introductory paragraph. In the organizational behavior modification condition, procedures and techniques were described using "reinforcement" terminology; the humanistic condition was described in terms of "growth." Each testing session began with the experimenter reading the appropriate description of the videotape together with groups of between 8 and 12 subjects, all having been assigned to the same description. Although the experimenter was necessarily aware of which descriptions were being administered, he was "blind" to which subjects were either in favor of or 2 Transcripts of the actual videotape, copies of the different instructions given the various groups, as well as the revised version of Musgrove's (1974) questionnaire are available from the senior author upon request.

134 BARLING AND WAINSTEIN against the use of organizational behavior modification, thereby ensuring some experimental control for possible experimenter bias effects. Following this, subjects received three questionnaires in counterbalanced order. All subjects received the same instructions regarding their completions, viz., they were to respond as if they were the line supervisor in the particular organization. The 32 subjects participating in this section of the study had all completed course work on behavior modification and humanistic approaches to organizational behavior. The videotape session was consequently administered by the second author who had not been involved in any of this teaching and therefore could not be identified with any particular approach, which may have biased the results. RESULTS Separate 2 x 2 (Label x Attitude) analyses of variance were performed on the two dependent variables. Significant main effects were obtained for method IF (1,28) = 6.77, p <.015] and leadership [F (1,28) = 6.06, p <.02] characteristics. However, these significant attitude main effects toward organizational behavior modification are statistical artifacts of the median split and therefore will be disregarded. A significant main effect for labels was obtained on method characteristics IF (1,28) = 5.18, p <.03]. Analysis of the mean scores (see Table 2) indicated that subjects receiving the humanistic description rated this factor significantly more positively. The method characteristics interaction, however, was not significant. A significant main effects for labels IF (1,28) = 10.25, p <.003] was also obtained on leadership characteristics. Again, leadership characteristics were rated more positively by those subjects receiving the humanistic description. A significant Label x Attitude interaction [F (1,28) = 4.38, p <.05] was also obtained when rating this dependent variable. DISCUSSION For a labeling bias to be evident against behavior modification, significant main effects for labels should be obtained. Within the context of TABLE 2 MEAN SCORES FOR THE FOUR GROUPS ON THE TWO DEPENDENT VARIABLES Dependent variables Humanistic label Organizational behavior modification label High Low High Low Method characteristics 153.37(4.80) a 168.12(3.02) 159.12(5.56) 137.25(10.23) Leadership characteristics 35.62(152) 39.37(1.45) 37.12(1.98) 27.87(2.86) a Standard error of the mean presented in parentheses.

ORGANIZATIONAL BI~HAVIOR MODIFICATION 135 work organizations, labeling biases regarding either or both of the dependent variables would assume considerable import since they have been shown to affect behavior in such situations. In addition, they appear to represent separate entities as evidenced by their factorial distinctiveness. The specific characteristics of the method would probably be of major importance in deciding which particular technique would be utilized. Leadership characteristics have been shown to have some influence on a variety of behaviors in organizational settings, for example, job performance, satisfaction, and group processes (cf. McCormick & Tiffin, 1974; Steers & Porter, 1975). Significant main effects for labels appeared on both the method and leadership characteristics. This would suggest that a labeling bias exists against organizational behavior modification since these two variables were rated more positively when described in humanistic terms. However, a significant Label Attitude interaction emerged for the leadership characteristics. In addition, although the more pronounced Label Attitude interaction occurred for method characteristics, it was not significant due to the wide dispersion and consequently the overlap of scores (see Table 2). Examination of the direction of these two interactions minimizes the importance of the labeling main effects. In both cases, the significant main effects for labels is a function of prior attitudes to organizational behavior modification. Individuals who tended to favor organizational behavior modification and subsequently received a behavior modification description rated the method and leadership characteristics more positively; those exposed to a humanistic label of the videotape rated these two variables negatively. On the other hand, both method and leadership characteristics were rated more favorably by individuals who were against the use of behavior modification techniques in work organizations and exposed to a humanistic description, but negatively if they had received a behavior modification of the videotape's contents. The present results extend the findings of previous research. Woolfolk et al. (1977) maintain that a significant labeling bias exists against behavior modification in educational settings since the identical videotape in their study was rated more favorably when described in humanistic rather than behavior modification terminology. Yet their study could not account for the possible effects of prior attitudes toward behavior modification. Based on the results of the present study, however, it is suggested that their significant results may have been a function of prior attitudes toward behavior modification. Despite this possible limitation in their study, the suggestion of Woolfolk et al. (1977) that the manner in which behavioral programs are described and presented is important in determining their subsequent acceptance remains salient. That the application of behavior modification techniques in work settings is still received with some suspicion (Franks & Wilson, 1977) may be a function of two factors, rather than the label per se. First, the esoteric, mechanistic jargon employed in describing such programs and, second, the tendency to equate behavior modification with

136 BARLING AND WAINSTEIN any attempt to control behavior may predispose such negative attitudes. To achieve increased acceptance, the terminology used in describing behavior modification might profitably be changed to reflect a more humanistic terminology (cf. Bandura, 1975; Reppucci & Saunders, 1974; Woolfolk et al., 1977). REFERENCE NOTE 1. Mauer, K. F. The assessment of human resources utilization. Doctoral dissertation, Rand Afrikaans University, Johannesburg, 1976. REFERENCES Bandura, A. The ethics and social purpose of behavior modification. In C. M. Franks & G. T. Wilson (Eds.), Annual review of behavior therapy: Theory and practice (Vol. 3). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1975. Dubno, P., Hillburn, D., Robinson, G., Sandier, D., Trani, J., & Weingarten, E. An attitude toward behavior modification scale. Behavior Therapy, 1978, 9, 99-108. Franks, C. M., & Wilson, G. T. (Eds.). Annual review of behavior therapy: Theory and practice (Vol. 4). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1976. Franks, C. M., & Wilson, G. T. (Eds.). Annual review of behavior therapy: Theory and practice (Vol. 5). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1977. Fry, F. L. Operant conditioning in organizational settings: Of mice or men?personnel, 1974, 51, 17-24. Hammer, M. The application of behavioral conditioning procedures to the problems of quality control: Comment. Academy of Management Journal, 1971, 14, 529-532. Lazarus, A. A. Has behavior therapy outlived its usefulness?american Psychologist, 1977, 32, 550-554. Luthans, F., & Kreitner, R. Organizational behavior modification. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman, 1975. McCormick, E. J., & Tiffin, J. Industrial psychology (6th ed.). London: George Allen & Unwin, 1974. Musgrove, W. A scale to measure attitudes toward behavior modification. Psychology in the Schools, 1974, 11, 392-396. Parker, W. S. Black-white differences in leader behavior related to subordinates' reactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61, 140-147. Reppucci, N. D., & Saunders, J. T. Social psychology of behavior modification: Problems encountered in natural settings. American Psychologist, 1974, 29, 649-660. Saad, L. J., & Barling, J. I. Relating pay incentives to work performance: Effects of fixed ratio (group and individual contingent) versus fixed interval reinforcement in industry. Psychologia Africana, 1977, 17, 135-142. Schneier, C. B. Behavior modification in management: A review and critique. Academy of Management Journal, 1974, 17, 528-548. Steers, C. B., & Porter, L. W. Motivation and work behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1975. Taylor, J. C., & Bowers, D. G. Survey of organizations. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, 1972. Woolfolk, A. E., Woolfolk, R. L., & Wilson, G. T. A rose by any other name... : Labeling bias and attitudes toward behavior modification. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1977, 45, 184-191. RECEIVED: March 3, 1978; REV1SED: May 17, 1978 FINAL ACCEPTANCE: July 7, 1978