Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

Similar documents
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age

Classification of Inhibitory Function

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

Neuropsychological Test Development and Normative Data on Hispanics

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults

DOES IMPAIRED EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING DIFFERENTIALLY IMPACT VERBAL MEMORY MEASURES IN OLDER ADULTS WITH SUSPECTED DEMENTIA?

Use a diagnostic neuropsychology HOW TO DO IT PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

Carmen Inoa Vazquez, Ph.D., ABPP Clinical Professor NYU School of Medicine Lead Litigation Conference Philadelphia May 19, 2009 Presentation

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

A Neuropsychological Approach to Intelligence

EVIDENCE OF CONSTRUCT SIMILARITY IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS AND FLUID REASONING ABILITIES

What does the neuropsychological Category Test measure?

Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. February 27, 2006

The Context and Purpose of Executive Function Assessments: Beyond the Tripartite Model

Development of Metacognitive and Emotional Executive Functions in Children

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Hill, Elisabeth L Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), pp ISSN [Article]

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

Working Memory in Schizophrenia: Transient "Online" Storage Versus Executive Functioning

Neuropsychology of the 20th and 21st Century. How North America, Spain, and Russia have led us astray in the development of neuropsychology

Use of the Booklet Category Test to assess abstract concept formation in schizophrenic disorders

Empire BlueCross BlueShield Professional Commercial Reimbursement Policy

Expanded Scoring Criteria for the Design Fluency Test: Reliability and Validity in Neuropsychological and College Samples

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 30 (2011) `WCPCG-2011

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

CALYPSOS BRIEF FINAL REPORT ON THE INTELLECTUAL OUTPUTS OF THE PROJECT

A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans

Executive functioning in obsessive compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia

Reliability Evidence Validity Evidence. Criterion: Other (e.g.,

The Role of Attention in Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

The reliability of production strategy scores for the Ruff Figural Fluency Test

Cognitive recovery after severe head injury 2. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale during post-traumatic amnesia

Deficits in executive functions and motor coordination in children with frontal lobe epilepsy

Cognitive Impairment Following Frontal Lobe Damage and Its Relevance to Human Amnesia

Serial 7s and Alphabet Backwards as Brief Measures of Information Processing Speed

Beyond the Psychologist s Report. Nancy Foster, PhD Institute for Brain-Behavior Integration

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices*

Assessment of Memory

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Critical evaluation of ``Assessment: neuropsychological testing of adults'' $

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

Neurocognition and Schizophrenia

Exploration of social rule violation in patients with focal prefrontal neurosurgical lesions

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

Memory Retraining with Adult Male Alcoholics

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

Neuropsychological Evaluation in Clinical Practice: Overview and Approach

(2010) 14 (1) ISSN

The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery

Effects of Combined Frontal and Temporal Lesions

11/14/2017 SUPPORT FOR A MULTI STORE MODEL TEMPORARY MEMORY: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY INFORMATION PROCESSING MODEL & THE MODAL MODEL OF MEMORY

Neuropsychological Testing (NPT)

3-86 Psychological Tests and Evaluation Procedures ^ General Ability Measures

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Evidence for early impairment of verbal intelligence

Chapter 5 The Research Methods of Biopsychology

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

FRONTAL LOBE FUNCTIONS IN ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER FROM CHILDREN TO YOUNG ADULTS DISSERTATION

Executive function and behaviour

regardless of their IQ level and it can be attributed to the symptoms of autism spectrum disorders.

Neuropsychology in Spina Bifida. Dr Ellen Northcott Clinical Neuropsychologist Kids Rehab, CHW

Neurocognitive Deficit in Schizophrenia: A Quantitative Review of the Evidence

Memory and executive function impairments after frontal or posterior cortex lesions

Factors related to neuropsychological deficits in ADHD children

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 17. Peeking Inside The Head. Part 1. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert

It has become increasingly evident

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

SUPPORTING COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT IN HEART CHILDREN

The Five-Point Test: Reliability, Validity and Normative Data for Children and Adults

Method. NeuRA Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder April 2016

@ Copyright by Dawnette Benedict, Mount Saint Vincent University. Frontal Lobe Development. and Social Perspective-Taking in Children

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task

Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations{

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

THE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF POST-POLIO FATIGUE. Richard L. Bruno, Thomas Galski, John DeLuca.

Research Article Postoperative Neuropsychological Outcome in Patients with Mesial Temporal Lobe Epilepsy in Argentina

(Received 30 March 1990)

Lightning Review of Neuropsychological Assessments

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures

- Supporting Information. In order to better delineate the activity related to target processing, we analyzed

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

Barry A. Tanner and M. Cristina Ramírez

Frontal Lobe Disinhibition in Attention Deficit Disorder

Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder

2. Which pioneer in intelligence testing first introduced performance scales in addition to verbal scales? David Wechsler

W E N D Y H A R T I N G E R

Cognitive performance in UK sample of. Huntington's disease. presymptomatic people carrying the gene for. develop HD averaged 15 points less (1 SD)

NEUROCOGNITIVE, OUTCOMES IN PKU: IT S TIME TO RAISE THE BAR

Transcription:

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 1, pp. 31 36, 2000 Copyright 1999 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(98)00159-0 Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures Alfredo Ardila Miami Institute of Psychology David Pineda Neuropsychology Program, Department of Psychology, San Buenaventura University and Behavioral Neurology Program, School of Medicine, University of Antioquia Mónica Rosselli Florida Atlantic University In this study, some executive function measures (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST], verbal fluency, and Trial Making Test [TMT], Form A and Form B) were correlated with Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) scores. Fifty 13- to 16-year-old normal children were selected. It was found that verbal fluency tests correlated about 0.30 with Verbal Intelligence Quotient (IQ) and Full Scale IQ. In the WCST only Perseverative Errors negatively correlated with Verbal IQ and Full Scale IQ. Two correlations were found to be significant with regard to the TMT: TMT Form B Errors negatively correlated with WISC-R Vocabulary subtest; and TMT Form A Time negatively correlated with Performance IQ. These results support the assumption that traditional intelligence tests are not appropriately evaluating executive functions. 1999 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Keywords: intelligence, executive functions, cognitive testing, frontal lobe Prefrontal lobes activity has been associated with self-regulation, control of cognition (metacognition), temporal organization of behavior, monitoring of behavior, selective inhibition of responses to immediate stimuli, planning behavior, and control of attention (Brown, 1985; Damasio & Anderson, 1993; Fuster, 1989; Hécaen, 1964; Luria, 1966, 1969, 1973, 1980; Perecman, 1987; Pribram, 1973; Stuss & Benson, 1983, 1986, 1987). The name executive function has been proposed to refer to the multi-operational system mediated by prefrontal areas of the brain and their reciprocal cortical and subcortical connecting pathways (Stuss & Benson, 1986). Executive dysfunction may be summarized in two cardinal defects: controlling behavior and organizing cognition. Address correspondence to: Alfredo Ardila, 12230 NW 8th Street, Miami, FL 33182. 31

32 A. Ardila, D. Pineda, and M. Rosselli For a long time it has been noted that frontal damage does not result in evident deficits in psychometric intelligence tests (Hebb, 1939; Hebb & Penfield, 1940). This is true even in cases of bilateral frontal lobectomy. It was somehow surprising to find that intelligence quotient (IQ) in patients with frontal lobe damage could be normal (Hebb, 1945). These initial observations carried out during the 1940s have been further welldocumented in neuropsychology (e.g., Brazzelli, Colombo, Della Sala, & Spinnler, 1994; Damasio & Anderson, 1993). Milner (1983), for instance, reported a mean loss of only 7.2 IQ points following dorsolateral frontal lobectomies, with mean postoperative IQ scores remaining in the average range. More recently, Damasio and Anderson (1993) analyzed 10 patients with ventrolateral and dorsolateral frontal lesions that were caused by either vascular events or surgical resection for treatment of tumors. The most notable feature of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981) testing in these patients was the consistent preservation of the cognitive abilities required to perform the various intellectual tasks following frontal lobe damage. This observation mean that psychometric intelligence tests are not sensitive to frontal lobe deficits ( executive dysfunctions ). Very few studies have analyzed the association between executive function measures and psychometric intelligence test scores. Welsh, Pennington, and Groisser (1991) observed in children that most of the executive function tasks (Visual Search [Teuber, Battersby, & Bender, 1955]; Verbal Fluency [McCarthy, 1972]; Motor Planning [Golden, 1981]; Tower of Honoi [Simon, 1975]; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST; Heaton, 1981]; and Matching Familial Figures Test [Kagan, 1964]) were uncorrelated with IQ. Visual Search, Verbal Fluency, WCST, and Tower of Honoi did not correlate with any IQ measure (Verbal, Quantitative, and Nonverbal) from the Iowa Test of Basic Abilities. Using a 300-subject college-student sample, Ardila, Galeano, and Rosselli (1998) observed that Verbal Fluency tests presented a low but significant correlation (0.20 0.25) with some WAIS verbal subtests, particularly Digits, Arithmetic, and Information. However, WCST scores did not correlate at all with the Verbal, Performance, or Full Scale IQ. The independence of executive functions from psychometric intelligence may be conjectured. The purpose of this research was to analyze the association between executive function measures and psychometric intelligence test scores in adolescents. This study represents a secondary analysis of the data collected in the norming study of some neuropsychological tests carried out in Colombia, using a Spanish-speaking population. METHOD Participants Fifty 13- to 16-year-old male children (M 14.38; SD 1.02) were taken. Children were selected from the public schools of Medellin city (Colombia, population about 2 million). All children were monolingual native Spanish speakers. Their mean school level was 8.66 years (SD 1.22; range 7 10). They voluntarily agreed to participate with the purpose of obtaining norms for some psychological and neuropsychological tests. All of the subjects were in a low socioeconomic status. Testing was performed by graduate neuropsychology students from San Buenaventura University, under the supervision of a professor. Evaluation was performed in three sessions, each one lasting about 40 minutes. Tests were administered in the following sequence: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, Spanish version (WISC-R;

Intelligence and Executive Functions 33 Wechsler, 1993); Verbal Fluency; Trial Making Test (TMT; Reitan & Wolfson, 1985); and WCST. No subject had been on medication during the week prior to the testing, had a history of behavioral or attentional problems, or had evidence of psychiatric or neurological disorders. Instruments The following tests were individually administered to the experimental and control subjects: (a) WISC-R, Spanish version (Wechsler, 1993). The Spanish version was translated and adapted by De La Cruz, Lopez, and Cordero-Pardo (Wechsler, 1993). This version of the WISC has been previously standardized and normalized in Spain. Four verbal subtests (Information, Similarities, Arithmetic, and Vocabulary) and four performance subtests (Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, and Digit- Symbol) were used. (b) WCST (Heaton, 1981). Areas scored were categories achieved, perseverative errors, nonperseverative errors, and failure to maintain set. This test has been previously normatized in Spanish-speaking children (Rosselli & Ardila, 1993) and has been found to be sensitive to frontal lobe pathology (Lezak, 1995). (c) Verbal fluency (phonologic /f/, /a/, and /s/; and semantic animals and fruits). Verbal fluency was measured by the number of words produced in a particular category within a 1-minute time limit. The verbal fluency test has been described in the clinical neuropsychology literature as an executive function test (Ardila, Rosselli, & Puente, 1994; Lezak, 1995). (d) TMT, Forms A and B (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985). Set shifting ability is supposed to be evaluated with this test. TABLE 1 Means (M) and Standard Deviations (SD) Found in the Different Tests Test M SD WISC-R Information 10.82 3.63 Similarities 10.44 3.93 Arithmetic 10.10 2.09 Vocabulary 10.38 3.00 Picture completion 9.32 2.46 Picture arrangement 10.08 3.03 Block design 8.84 2.23 Digit-symbol 10.29 3.20 Verbal IQ 102.84 16.18 Performance IQ 92.86 10.82 Full Scale IQ 97.76 13.54 Verbal Fluency Phonologic 26.22 8.91 Semantic 30.22 5.12 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Categories 4.68 1.53 Perseverative errors 21.42 13.17 Nonperseverative errors 17.90 8.07 Fail maintain set 1.30 1.35 Trail Making Test Form A: Time 70.92 35.29 Errors 0.32 0.74 Form B: Time 52.18 28.92 Errors 0.26 1.33 Note. WISC-R Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised.

34 A. Ardila, D. Pineda, and M. Rosselli TABLE 2 Correlation Matrix Between WISC-R Scores and Executive Function Measures Verbal Fluency Perseverative Errors Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Non-perseverative Errors Fail maint TMT A TMT B Phonol Semant Categ Time Errors Time Errors Information 0.34* 0.51*** 0.04 0.28* 0.20 0.20 0.14 0.09 0.00 0.07 Similarities 0.32* 0.44*** 0.03 0.27* 0.07 0.11 0.14 0.22 0.04 0.23 Arithmetic 0.02 0.01 0.19 0.36** 0.23 0.08 0.02 0.17 0.05 0.14 Vocabulary 0.40** 0.38*** 0.24 0.12 0.23 0.22 0.02 0.13 0.00 0.34* Picture completion 0.10 0.12 0.21 0.19 0.05 0.17 0.06 0.30* 0.13 0.03 Picture arrangement 0.11 0.04 0.18 0.02 0.17 0.19 0.25 0.03 0.14 0.39 Block design 0.22 0.20 0.11 0.37** 0.02 0.18 0.14 0.06 0.24 0.05 Digit-symbol 0.13 0.26 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.14 0.19 0.08 0.17 0.03 Verbal IQ 0.32* 0.39*** 0.14 0.37** 0.20 0.08 0.11 0.20 0.00 0.25 Performance IQ 0.18 0.18 0.12 0.08 0.11 0.01 0.31* 0.07 0.20 0.16 Full Scale IQ0.30* 0.24* 0.04 0.30* 0.09 0.07 0.04 0.18 0.08 0.26 TMT Trail Making Test. *p.05. **p.01. *** p.001. 34

Intelligence and Executive Functions 35 RESULTS Table 1 presents the general results in the different tests. Mean Full Scale IQ was 97.76. Verbal IQ was higher than performance IQ. This difference, however, was lower than 1 standard deviation. Table 2 presents the correlations between WISC scores and executive function measures. Verbal fluency tests correlated with Verbal IQ and Full Scale IQ. As a matter of fact, Verbal fluency tests significantly correlated with all the WISC-R verbal subtests, excepting Arithmetic. On the WCST, only Persevertive Errors negatively correlated with Verbal IQ and Full Scale IQ. WCST Perseverative Errors also correlated with four the WISC-R subtests (Information, Similarities, Arithmetic, and Block Design). Only two correlations were found to be significant with regard to the TMT: TMT Form B Errors negatively correlated with Vocabulary subtest; and TMT A Time negatively correlated with Performance IQ. No other significant correlation was observed. CONCLUSIONS Few significant correlations were found between WISC-R scores and executive function measures. Verbal IQ and Full Scale IQ significantly correlated with verbal fluency tests. Verbal IQ and Full Scale IQ also correlated with WCST perseverative errors. These correlations, although significant, were rather low. This finding may emphasize that the WCST is indeed measuring an ability (concept formation, executive function) not traditionally included in psychological intelligence test batteries. Performance IQ did not correlate with any executive function test score, except TMT Form A: Time. This was also a low correlation. Full Scale IQ correlated only with the Verbal Fluency tests, and WCST-Perseverative errors. The rest of the correlations were nonsignificant. These results support the assumption that traditional intelligence tests do not appropriately evaluate executive functions. It must be concluded that either executive functions should not be included as elements of intelligent behavior, or that psychometric intelligence tests are insufficient in testing for intelligence. These tests are not sensitive to the most important elements of intelligence : to act purposefully (i.e., controlling and planning behavior), and to think rationally (i.e., organizing and directing cognition), according to Wechsler s (1944) own definition of intelligence. The conclusion is evident: Psychometric intelligence tests do not appropriately appraise intelligence. Or, at least, they are not appraising abilities that, from a neuropsychological perspective (and also, from the point of view of Wechsler s intelligence testing), should be understood as the most important elements in cognition. REFERENCES Ardila, A., Galeano, L. M., & Rosselli, M. (1998). Toward a model of neuropsychological activity Neuropsychology Review, 8, 171 190. Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A. (1994). Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish speaker. New York: Plenum Press. Brazzelli, M., Colombo, N., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired abilities after bilateral frontal damage. Cortex, 30, 27 51. Brown, J. E. (1985). Frontal lobe syndromes. In J. A. M. Fredericks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical neurology: Vol. 45. Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 32 41). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

36 A. Ardila, D. Pineda, and M. Rosselli Damasio, A. R., & Anderson, S. W. (1993). The frontal lobes. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (3rd ed., pp. 409 460). New York: Oxford University Press. Fuster, J. M. (1989). The prefrontal cortex (2nd ed.). New York: Raven Press. Golden, C. J. (1981). The Luria-Nebraska Children s Battery: Theory and formulation. In: G.W. Hynd & J. E. Obrzut (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment and the school-aged child (pp. 277 302). New York: Grune & Stratton. Heaton, R. (1981). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Hebb, D. (1939). Intelligence in man after large removals of cerebral tissue: Report of four left frontal lobe cases. Journal of General Psychology, 21, 73 87. Hebb, D. O. (1945). Man s frontal lobe: A critical review. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 54, 421 438. Hebb, D., & Penfield, W. (1940). Human behavior after extensive bilateral removal from the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 44, 421 438. Hécaen, H. (1964). Mental symptoms associated with tumors of the frontal lobe. In J. M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 335 352). New York: McGraw-Hill. Kagan, J., Rosman, B. L., Day, L., Albert, J., & Phillips, W. (1964). Information processing in the child: Significance of analytic and reflective attitudes. Psychological Monographs, 78(1), Whole No. 578. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Luria, A. R. (1966). Human brain and psychological processes. New York: Harper & Row. Luria, A. R. (1969). Frontal lobe syndromes. In P. J. Vinken & G. W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 2, pp. 725 757). Amsterdam: North Holland. Luria, A. R. (1973). The frontal lobes and the regulation of behavior. In K. H. Pribram & A. R. Luria (Eds.), Psychophysiology of the frontal lobes (pp. 3 26). New York: Academic Press. Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed.). New York: Basic. McCarthy, D. (1972). Manual for the McCarthy Scales for Children s Abilities. New York: Psychological Corp. Milner, B. (1982). Some cognitive effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298, 211 226. Perecman, E. (1987). Consciousness and the meta-functions of the frontal lobes. In E. Perecman (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 1 10). New York: IRBN Press. Pribram, K. H. (1973). The primate frontal cortex-executive of the brain. In K. H. Pribram & A. R. Luria (Eds.), Psychophysiology of the frontal lobes (pp. 293 314). New York: Academic Press. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery. Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press. Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1993). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in five to 12- year-old children. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 145 154. Simon, H. A. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 268 288. Stuss, K. H., & Benson, D. F. (1983). Frontal lobe lesions and behavior. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology (pp. 429 454). New York: Academic Press. Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press. Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1987). The frontal lobes and control of cognition and memory. In E. Perecman (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 141 158). New York: IRBN Press. Teuber, H. L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1955). Changes in visual searching performance following cerebral lesions. American Journal of Physiology, 159, 592 600. Wechsler, D. (1944). The measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R manual. New York: Psychological Corp. Wechsler, D. (1993). Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Nin~ os revisada. Madrid: TEA Editores. Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131 149.