Published 21 May 2009 Cite this as: BMJ Case Reports 2009 [doi: /bcr ] Copyright 2009 by the BMJ Publishing Group Ltd.

Similar documents
Mirror neurons. Romana Umrianova

The Change of Mu Rhythm during Action Observation in People with Stroke. Tae-won Yun, PT, MSc, Moon-Kyu Lee, PT, PhD

Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients

To point a finger: Attentional and motor consequences of observing pointing movements

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Mirror Neurons in Primates, Humans, and Implications for Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Insights into the functional specificity of the human corpus callosum

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Deficits in Recall Following Partial and Complete Commissurotomy

neurons: how kids learn

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D.

Understanding Actions of Others: The Electrodynamics of the Left and Right Hemispheres. A High-Density EEG Neuroimaging Study

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Spatio-Temporal Dynamics of Human Intention Understanding in Temporo-Parietal Cortex: A Combined EEG/fMRI Repetition Suppression Paradigm

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

The origins of localization

When Do We Stop Calling Them Mirror Neurons?

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University

Supporting Information

Manuscript. Do not cite. 1. Mirror neurons or emulator neurons? Gergely Csibra Birkbeck, University of London

Mental rotation of anthropoid hands: a chronometric study

Mirror neurons in the infant brain are formed by the interactions between self and other. M. Iacoboni SELF AND OTHER. J. A. Pineda COGS171 UCSD

Psychology of Language

Unbroken mirrors: challenging a theory of Autism

Story Time Language Program Summary Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University

Running head: EMPATHY, MIRROR NEURONS, and HEART RATE

The old adage I feel your pain may be closer to reality

Reading Minds. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science, UCSD

Observational Learning Based on Models of Overlapping Pathways

Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system

Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system?

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

Higher Cortical Function

fmri Activities in the Emotional Cerebellum: A Preference for Negative Stimuli and Goal-Directed Behavior

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Cells That Read Minds

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests

NEUROPLASTICITY. Implications for rehabilitation. Genevieve Kennedy

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy

Tactual and auditory vigilance in split-brain man

A Scientific Model of Consciousness that Explains Spirituality and Enlightened States

Activation patterns during action observation are modulated by context in mirror. system areas.

Greg Hickok: The myth of mirror neurons

INTRODUCTION TO MIRROR NEURONS MARY ET BOYLE, PH.D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Motor Theories of Cognition

PsychoBrain. 31 st January Dr Christos Pliatsikas. Lecturer in Psycholinguistics in Bi-/Multilinguals University of Reading

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Interaction in isolation: 50 years of insights from split-brain research

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

synapse neurotransmitters Extension of a neuron, ending in branching terminal fibers, through which messages pass to other neurons, muscles, or glands

Hemispheric interaction: when and why is yours better than mine?

Studying structure-function relationships in the human brain. Lesley Fellows

No Language-Specific Activation during Linguistic Processing of Observed Actions

Genetic Conclusions. Layers of the ASD Onion. The notion of finding a simple genetic cause to help define ASD will not likely occur.

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

9.00 BLINDSIGHT & NEGLECT. Professor John Gabrieli

A CONVERSATION ABOUT NEURODEVELOPMENT: LOST IN TRANSLATION

Big brains may hold clues to origins of autism

Ben Cipollini & Garrison Cottrell

Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account

Modulation of cortical motor outputs by the symbolic meaning of visual stimuli

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

Support for lateralization of the Whorf effect beyond the realm of color discrimination

Myers Psychology for AP*

A Three Agent Model of Consciousness Explains Spirituality and Multiple Nondual Enlightened States Frank Heile, Ph.D.

Love, Emotions, and the Highly Sensitive Brain

Right hemisphere. Left hemisphere. Demonstration: Contralateral processing. Motor functions on left side of body Perceives left side of space

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

Pathways Toward Translational Research Programs for ASD. Helen Tager-Flusberg, Ph.D. Boston University NJ Governor s Council Conference April 9, 2014

The Basic Course on the Greenspan Floortime Approach. Mastery Test Instructions & Questions

Report. Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System. Caroline Catmur, 1, * Vincent Walsh, 1,2 and Cecilia Heyes 1 1

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Kinematic cues in perceptual weight judgement and their origins in box lifting

Autism Spectrum Disorders

Learning Objectives.

The Cause of Autism: Its Footprint Tells

"False tagging mechanism False Tagging Theory All idea initially believed Doubt occur when prefrontal cortex tags it as false Provides doubt and

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

The Cognitive Approach

TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES IN SOUTH AFRICAN SCHOOLBOY RUGBY PLAYERS

FUNCTIONAL MRI IN EPILEPSY December 6 th 2013

central sulcus parietal lobe frontal lobe occipital lobe Sylvian fissure temporal lobe

Intent Inference and Action Prediction using a Computational Cognitive Model

Mirroring People: The Science Of Empathy And How We Connect With Others PDF

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

Report. View-Based Encoding of Actions in Mirror Neurons of Area F5 in Macaque Premotor Cortex

BRAIN DEVELOPMENT: HELPING ALL CHILDREN DO AND BE THEIR BEST. February 9, :30 8:00 p.m. (Delicious Dinner First!)

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Chapter 11 summary definitief ineke brands.indd :57:59

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

Breaking New Ground: Understanding and Preventing Sexual Abuse 2015 ATSA Conference Friday October 16 3:30 PM - 5:00 PM F-38

Transcription:

Published 21 May 2009 Cite this as: BMJ Case Reports 2009 [doi:10.1136/bcr.07.2008.0593] Copyright 2009 by the BMJ Publishing Group Ltd. Right hemisphere dominance for understanding the intentions of others: evidence from a split-brain patient Stephanie Ortigue 1,2,3, Danielle King 3, Michael Gazzaniga 3, Michael Miller 2,3 and Scott Grafton 1,2,3 1 Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies, Psychology, 3803, Sage Center for the Study of the Mind, UCSB, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA 2 UCSB Brain Imaging Center, Department of Psychology, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA 3 Sage Center for the Study of the Mind, UCSB, Department of Psychology, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA Correspondence to: Stephanie Ortigue, ortigue@psych.ucsb.edu SUMMARY Understanding the actions performed by other people is a key aspect of social interaction, including in clinical settings where patients are learning from therapists and caregivers. While lesions of the left cerebral hemisphere induce praxic disorders, the hemispheric specialisation of intention understanding remains unclear. Do patients with a right hemispheric lesion understand the intentions of other people properly? The present study investigates how a split-brain patient understands the means (what) and intentions (why) of the actions of other people. Results show a significant left hemispheric dominance for understanding what is done, and a significant right hemispheric dominance for understanding why an action is carried out. This discovery might have important clinical implications in neurological patients, especially when those with right hemisphere lesions are faced with important decisions related to the interpretation of other s intentions. BACKGROUND Understanding the actions and intentions of others is a key aspect of social interaction, notably in clinical settings where patients are trying to learn from therapists and caregivers. However, one cannot access other s intentions directly; information is only available from observing their actions and surrounding contextual environment.1 The classical view on how individuals understand other s intentions suggests we use automatic cognitive reasoning to evaluate other s actions and compare them with similar representations stored from past self-related experiences.1 6 Along these lines, individuals can know what others are doing (eg, grasping a bottle of water) and also why (eg, to drink).1,2,7 While lesions of the left cerebral hemisphere induce praxic disorders, the hemispheric specialisation and neural substrates of intention understanding is unclear.

Localisation might be within the human mirror neuron system (MNS), a bilateral parietal and inferior frontal network activated during action execution, observation and imitation.1,4,5,7 Many brain imaging studies of action understanding suggest that understanding actions in terms of objects or the details of a movement are mostly lateralised to the left MNS.1,4 This activation is mostly related to the what of motor acts.4,8,10 In contrast, a few neuroimaging studies suggest a right MNS involvement for understanding other s intentions (the why of an action).1,2,4,7,9,11 If this rightlateralised specialisation is correct, one would expect hemispheric differences in neurological patients. To date, however, no case has been reported to reinforce this leftright hemispheric dissociation between the what and why of actions. Split-brain patients offer the unique opportunity to examine the specialisation of intention understanding within each hemisphere independently from each other.12 CASE PRESENTATION Here, we describe a 55-year-old right-handed woman who underwent a complete resection of her corpus callosum for intractable epilepsy in 1979.13,14 A follow-up MRI in 1984 revealed spared fibres in the rostrum and splenium. The spared rostral fibres comprised ~1.8% of the total cross-sectional area of the corpus callosum and the spared splenial fibres comprised ~1% of the area.14 The spared fibres did not allow much interhemispheric communication in the higher-order cognitive domain.13,14 Post-operative neurological examination revealed no focal deficits.13,14 Wechsler IQ scores were in the normal range.13 Although the patient lacked any obvious impairment of intention understanding in daily life, a deficit could be elicited when she performed a specific neuropsychological task in a randomised repeated design. INVESTIGATIONS The patient s ability to understand other s intentions was investigated using a means inference task (MIT) and an intention inference task (IIT). During these two tasks, the same stimuli were used; only the instruction was different. In both tasks, the patient observed one 500 ms frame from video clips displaying a scene including two daily objects (eg, a glass and a bottle of water, a hammer and a nail, or a lighter and a candle) followed by a second 1000 ms frame showing a hand grasping one of these two objects (eg, the bottle of water, the hammer or the lighter) and then a third 1000 ms frame displaying either a correct (eg, to pour water into the glass, to hammer the nail, or to light up the wick of the candle) or incorrect (to pour water outside the glass, to hammer near the nail, or to light up the bottom of the candle) outcome. Then, the patient was either asked to guess as rapidly and as accurately as possible if the means (what) of the agent s actions were correct (MIT) or if the intentions (why) of the agent were correct (IIT). More precisely, for MIT, the instruction was as follows: "During this task, you will have to press the YES or NO button to indicate if the grasp (useful hand object interaction) of each agent s action is correct or not". For IIT, the instruction was as follows: "During this task, you will have to press the YES or NO button to indicate if the outcome of each agent s action is correct or not". Each trial began with a fixation cross that was presented for 150 ms. A 5000 ms maximum inter-trial interval separated the onset of each movie

presentation. The tasks were run using JAVA on a MacBook Pro laptop computer with a 17-inch colour monitor located 70 cm from the patient. There were eight blocks in total. Thirty video clips were presented per block. The patient thus watched a total of 240 video clips (30x8). The order of the tasks was randomised and pre-determined according to an ABBA design. Responses were collected from two response keys that were randomly attributed across blocks. Prior to participation, the patient provided written informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of California, Santa Barbara. Analysis of accuracy did not reveal any significant differences (p>0.05). During MIT, similar accuracy rates were observed with the right hand/left hemisphere (mean (SD): 81% (11.55%)) and the left hand/right hemisphere (mean (SD): 73% (10.99%); p>0.05). During IIT, similar accuracy rates were observed with the right hand/left hemisphere (mean (SD): 83% (8.52%)) and the left hand/right hemisphere (mean (SD): 86% (5.31%); p>0.05). No difference was observed as a function of the type of means or the agent s intentions (correct/incorrect; p>0.05). Interestingly, analysis of the patient s reaction times revealed the following significant effects. During MIT, faster reaction times were observed with the right hand/left hemisphere (mean (SD): 1522 (161) ms) in comparison with the left hand/right hemisphere (mean (SD): 1963 (287) ms; F(1,17)=6.28; p=0.023). This left hemispheric advantage for the what was independent of the type of means (correct/incorrect). Faster reaction times were observed for right hand/left hemisphere for detecting both correct (mean (SD): 1300 (172) ms) and incorrect (mean (SD): 1623 (150) ms) action s means in comparison with the left hand/right hemisphere (mean (SD): 1748 (268) ms for correct; mean (SD): 2178 (306) ms for incorrect; fig 1). Conversely, during IIT, slow reaction times (mean (SD): 2500 (390) ms) were collected from the right hand/left hemisphere, while faster reaction times were collected from the left hand/right hemisphere (mean (SD): 1750 (154) ms; F(1,22)=12.5; p=0.0019). This right hemispheric advantage for the why was independent of the type of the agent s intentions (correct/incorrect). Faster reaction times (paired t test: p<0.05) were observed for left hand/right hemisphere when detecting both correct (mean (SD): 1695 (163) ms) and incorrect (mean (SD): 1805 (144) ms) intentions in comparison with the right hand/left hemisphere (mean (SD): 2386 (398) ms for correct; mean (SD): 2614 (381) ms for incorrect). Thus it is suggested that the reaction-time measurement is a good indicator of the level of patients decoding of the actions and intentions of other people. Notably, the right hemisphere shows a greater sensitivity for understanding the why of an observed action, while the left hemisphere is more sensitive to understanding the what of an observed action.

Figure 1 Figure 1 (A) Graphic representation of the experimental design with stimuli. (B) Reaction times obtained during the means inference task (MIT) for both types of action s means (C, correct; I, incorrect) and for both hemispheres (LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere). (C) Reaction times obtained during the intention inference task (IIT) for both types of agent s intentions (C, correct; I, incorrect) and for both hemispheres (LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere). OUTCOME AND FOLLOW-UP By demonstrating the role of the right hemisphere in understanding the intentions of other people s actions and the left hemisphere in evaluating how actions are performed, the present case report suggests there are hemispheric differences of action understanding. This might have important clinical implications in how rehabilitation therapy is designed for neurological patients with right or left brain lesions. The systematic screening for intention or means understanding using our task in patients with either right or left brain damage might be helpful to detect those who struggle with understanding the purpose of the actions performed on them by medical staff or therapists. The prediction would be that patients with right hemisphere lesions would benefit from instructions or explanations focused on action means and would have difficulty inferring why a person is performing a particular act. In contrast, patients with left hemisphere lesions would

benefit from instructions related to what the therapist or caregiver is trying to do but would have difficulty understanding specific movements, gestures or actions. Along these lines, we propose future clinical studies should systematically investigate intention understanding skills in neurological patients with a focal brain damage and/or inter-hemispheric disorder in order to better understand the patients ability to understand the actions and intentions of other people during (and also after) hospitalisation. DISCUSSION By demonstrating the first clinical evidence of a dissociate neural network between the understanding of the what and why of other s actions and intentions, our case report opens a new avenue to the understanding of the social active brain in clinical settings. Up until now, the lack of assessment of this mechanism in clinical settings led to the absence of neurological reports regarding the hemispheric representation of the what and why of actions and intentions. Thus, the present double dissociation between left and right brain highlights the dynamics of hemispheric specialisation and integration in the context of goal-directed behaviors.15 This reinforces the idea that the functional participation of both hemispheres in understanding goal-directed behaviour is not fixed but based on versatile dynamics between the hemispheres.15,16 Clinically, our results may have important implications in neurological patients, especially when those with right hemisphere lesions are faced with important decisions related to the interpretation of other s intentions. LEARNING POINTS * The way people understand what others are doing and why they are acting calls for different neural networks in the human brain. * The left hemisphere is dominant for understanding the what of others actions, while the right hemisphere is dominant for understanding the why of others actions. * A focal cerebral lesion limited to the right or left hemisphere might have important consequences for the patient s social life, and also for their ability to fully understand medical staff or therapists during (and/or after) their hospitalisation. * For clinicians, it may be important to evaluate the severity of action understanding deficits in patients with a focal right or left hemispheric lesions. * The rapid detection of action understanding deficits is a challenge for medical personal and may be important for tailoring dialogues and therapy to a patient level of understanding and for establishing if a patient has the ability to understand medical acts and their possible outcomes. The authors thank Professor Giacomo Rizzolatti for helpful discussions. We also thank Catherine Chang for her drawing, and Nisa Patel for technical assistance. This study was supported by NIH #R01NS031443-10A2 to MBM and MG, and grant DAAD19-03-D-0004 to STG. Competing interests: none. Patient consent: Patient/guardian consent was obtained for publication.

REFERENCES 1. Rizzolatti, G, & Cragheiro, L. The mirror neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 2004; 27: 169 92.[CrossRef][Medline] 2. Grafton, ST, & Hamilton, A. Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. Hum Mov Sci 2007; 26: 590 616.[CrossRef][Medline] 3. Keysers, C, & Gazzola, V. Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 2007; 11: 194 6.[CrossRef][Medline] 4. Iacoboni, M, & Dapretto, M. The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006; 7: 942 51.[CrossRef][Medline] 5. Rizzolatti, R, & Sinigaglia, C. Mirror neurons and motor intentionality. Funct Neurol 2007; 22: 205 10.[Medline] 6. Cattaneo, L, Fabbri-Destro, M, Boria, S, et al. Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104: 17825 30.[Abstract/Free Full Text] 7. Iacoboni, M, Molnar-Szakacs, I, Gallese, V, et al. Grasping the intentions of others with one s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol 2005; 3: e79.[crossref][medline] 8. Aziz-Zadeh, L, Koski, L, Zaidel, E, et al. Lateralization of the human mirror neuron system. J Neurosci 2006; 26: 2964 70.[Abstract/Free Full Text] 9. De Lange, FP, Spronk, M, Willems, RM, et al. Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Curr Biol 2008; 18: 454 7.[CrossRef][Medline] 10. Aziz-Zadeh, L, Maeda, F, Zaidel, E, et al. Lateralization in motor facilitation during action observation: a TMS study. Exp Brain Res 2002; 144: 127 31.[CrossRef][Medline] 11. Aziz-Zadeh, L, Iacoboni, M, Zaidel, E, et al. Left hemisphere motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 19: 2609 12.[CrossRef][Medline] 12. Gazzaniga, M. Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong. Nat Rev 2005; 6: 653 9.[CrossRef] 13. Gazzaniga, M, Nass, R, Reeves, A, et al. Neurologic perspectives on right hemisphere language following surgical section of the corpus callosum. Sem Neurol 1984; 4: 126 35.[CrossRef] 14. Funnell, MG, Corballis, PM, & Gazzaniga, MS. Insights into the functional specificity of the human corpus callosum. Brain 2000; 123: 920 6.[Abstract/Free Full Text] 15. Serrie, DJ, Ivry, R, & Swinnen, SP. Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control. Nat Rev 2006; 7: 160 7. 16. Ivry, R, & Robertson, L. The two sides of perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1997.