They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry) From Out-Group Homogeneity to Out-Group Heterogeneity

Similar documents
They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry): From Out-group Homogeneity to Out-group Heterogeneity

Running head: RESOURCE SCARCITY & RACE 1. Does this Recession Make Me Look Black? The Effect of Resource Scarcity on the

Goal-Driven Cognition and Functional Behavior: The Fundamental-Motives Framework

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 March 1.

When Memory is Better for Out-group Faces: On Negative Emotions and Gender Roles

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 May 1.

Imagine being invited to a social gathering consisting. Following in the Wake of Anger: When Not Discriminating Is Discriminating

Adaptive allocation of attention: effects of sex and sociosexuality on visual attention to attractive opposite-sex faces

Black 1 White 5 Black

Class, Race, and the Face: Social Context Modulates the Cross-Race Effect in Face Recognition

Testing the Domain-Specificity of the. Disease-Avoidance and Self-Protection Systems. Uriah Steven Anderson

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

SHORT REPORT Facial features influence the categorization of female sexual orientation

Does Racial Bias in the Identification of Threatening Stimuli Generalize to Older Black Men?

Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces

The Effect of Facial Attractiveness on Recognition Memory

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

The Cross-Category Effect

Cross-Racial Identification

Sexually Selective Cognition: Beauty Captures the Mind of the Beholder

JOSHUA M. ACKERMAN CURRICULUM VITAE

An Ambiguous-Race Illusion in Children s Face Memory Kristin Shutts and Katherine D. Kinzler

good reputation, and less chance to be chosen as potential partners. Fourth, not everyone values a good reputation to the same extent.

Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution Theodosius Dobzhansky Descent with modification Darwin

The disengagement of attentive resources from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional. infidelity

Healing Otherness: Neuroscience, Bias, and Messaging

I know your face but not where I saw you: Context memory is impaired for other-race faces

Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias. Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW

Categorical Cognition: A Psychological Model of Categories and Identification in Decision Making: Extended Abstract

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5

Self-pacing study of faces of different races: metacognitive control over study does not eliminate the cross-race recognition effect

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1. Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

Evidence for a contact-based explanation of the own-age bias in face recognition

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 1.

Creating Fair Lineups for Suspects With Distinctive Features Theodora Zarkadi, Kimberley A. Wade, and Neil Stewart

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Thompson, Valerie A, Ackerman, Rakefet, Sidi, Yael, Ball, Linden, Pennycook, Gordon and Prowse Turner, Jamie A

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE

Discovering That the Shoe Fits

Recognising faces across continents: The effect of within-race variations on the own-race bias in face recognition

Building An Even Better Conceptual Foundation

CHAPTER 15. Social Psychology. Lecture Overview. Introductory Definition PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY. Social Cognition.

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams

A Contextual Approach to Stereotype Content Model: Stereotype Contents in Context

PSYC 210 Social Psychology

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Framing Discrimination: Effects of Inclusion Versus Exclusion Mind-Sets on Stereotypic Judgments

Fukuoka University of Education

The effect of lineup member similarity on recognition accuracy in simultaneous and sequential lineups.

CULTURE AND FACE RECOGNITION BIAS 1. Will you remember me? Cultural differences in own-group face recognition biases

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence

Infection Breeds Reticence: The Effects of Disease Salience on Self-Perceptions of Personality and Behavioral Avoidance Tendencies

Unconscious Knowledge Assessment

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

TRAINING POTENTIAL WITNESSES TO PRODUCE HIGHER QUALITY FACE COMPOSITES

Social Psychology. Social Thinking Social Influence Social Relations.

ADAPTATION TO RACIAL CONTENT OF EMERGENT RACE FACES: GRADIENT SHIFT OR PEAK SHIFT?

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

GRADE LEVEL AND SUBJECT: ADVANCED PLACEMENT PSYCHOLOGY (11 TH AND 12 TH )

Cognition. A memory advantage for untrustworthy faces. Nicholas O. Rule a,, Michael L. Slepian b, Nalini Ambady c. abstract

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure

Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Biology 321 Lab 1 Measuring behaviour Sept , 2011

The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Perceptions of Mental Illness

Functional Perspectives on Emotion, Behavior, and Cognition

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Sex-Specificity in the Reward Value of Facial Attractiveness. Amanda C Hahn 1, Lisa M DeBruine 1, Benedict C Jones 1. Corresponding author

Supplementary Material. other ethnic backgrounds. All but six of the yoked pairs were matched on ethnicity. Results

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Literature Henrich, Joseph, and Natalie Henrich Why Humans Cooperate A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Andrew W. Delton. College of Business Phone: (805) Stony Brook University Fax: (631)

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in

Attitudes influence implicit racial face categorization in a perceptual task

The world is a scary place: Individual differences in belief in a dangerous world predict specific intergroup prejudices

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

Accuracy and Confidence in Person Identification: The Relationship is Strong when Witnessing Conditions Vary Widely

The Effects of Facial Similarity on the Express of Displaced Aggression

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR)

Author's personal copy

Transcription:

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry) From Out-Group Homogeneity to Out-Group Heterogeneity Joshua M. Ackerman, 1 Jenessa R. Shapiro, 1 Steven L. Neuberg, 1 Douglas T. Kenrick, 1 D. Vaughn Becker, 1 Vladas Griskevicius, 1 Jon K. Maner, 2 and Mark Schaller 3 1 Arizona State University, 2 Florida State University, and 3 University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada ABSTRACT People often find it more difficult to distinguish ethnic out-group members compared with ethnic in-group members. A functional approach to social cognition suggests that this bias may be eliminated when out-group members display threatening facial expressions. In the present study, 192 White participants viewed Black and White faces displaying either neutral or angry expressions and later attempted to identify previously seen faces. Recognition accuracy for neutral faces showed the outgroup homogeneity bias, but this bias was entirely eliminated for angry Black faces. Indeed, when participants cognitive processing capacity was constrained, recognition accuracy was greater for angry Black faces than for angry White faces, demonstrating an out-group heterogeneity bias. People readily confuse individuals from other races and ethnic groups with one another the they all look the same to me phenomenon. This pattern may reflect a more general cognitive bias toward perceiving the membership of other groups as less variable than the membership of one s own group the outgroup homogeneity bias (Anthony, Copper, & Mullen, 1992; Ostrom & Sedikides, 1992). This bias is generally interpreted as resulting from constraints on perceptual processing capacity and perceivers tendency to allocate limited perceptual resources in a functional way (e.g., Rodin, 1987; Sporer, 2001). Here, we elaborate on this functional approach to the out-group homogeneity bias, reporting a study in which we tested novel Address correspondence to Joshua Ackerman, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104, e-mail: joshua.ackerman@asu.edu, or to Steven Neuberg, e-mail: steven. neuberg@asu.edu, or to Douglas Kenrick, e-mail: douglas.kenrick@ asu.edu. predictions about circumstances in which this bias may be eliminated or even reversed. FUNCTIONAL APPROACH TO PERCEPTION OF IN-GROUP AND OUT-GROUP MEMBERS Cognitive resources are famously limited: People cannot attend to, encode, and remember all information available in their social environments (e.g., Todd, Hertwig, & Hoffrage, 2005). Thus, they selectively allocate cognitive resources to processing stimuli likely to have functional implications (Kenrick, Sadalla, & Keefe, 1998). For instance, snakes capture and hold attention more readily than do benign objects (Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001). In social situations, one attends more closely to some people than to others, favoring individuals whose physical appearance suggests greater benefits to one s own reproductive fitness or threats to one s well-being (Maner et al., 2003; Oda, 1997). Whether explained in psychological terms (e.g., needs and goals), economic terms (e.g., costs and benefits), or evolutionary terms (e.g., reproductive fitness), perceptual and cognitive resources are selectively allocated toward individuals who appear to have the most profound functional implications for perceivers. Historically, the interpersonal interactions that mattered most to individuals outcomes (e.g., mate selection, reciprocal exchange, and negotiation of status hierarchies) occurred within coalitional groups. Today, these interpersonal interactions continue to occur primarily within cultural and ethnic groups (Fiske, 1992). Contacts with out-group members tend not only to be less frequent, but also to involve group-level rather than individual-level interactions. Thus, we suggest that whereas the costs of allocating cognitive resources to processing in-group members are often offset by the benefits, the functional benefits 836 Copyright r 2006 Association for Psychological Science Volume 17 Number 10

J.M. Ackerman et al. of allocating cognitive resources to individual out-group members are typically outweighed by the costs. BUT WHAT IF THEY ARE ANGRY? Social cognitive processes are typically sensitive to functional aspects of dyadic and group ecology (Haselton, Nettle, & Andrews, 2005; Neuberg, Kenrick, Maner, & Schaller, 2004; Schaller, Park, & Faulkner, 2003). Although individuals outcomes may depend more on interactions with in-group members than on interactions with out-group members, there are circumstances in which out-group members may have greater functional importance. One circumstance in which the functional significance of outgroup members increases is when those out-group members are angry. Research has shown that angry facial expressions capture and hold attention (e.g., Fox et al., 2000; Öhman et al., 2001). Two further considerations suggest that perception of anger may lead perceivers to allocate resources toward processing the individuating features of angry target persons. First, anger is an interpersonal emotion; it implies threatening intent held by a specific individual (the person expressing anger) toward another specific individual (the person perceiving the expression). Second, angry expressions, like all emotional expressions, are fleeting; the angry expression may disappear even when the threatening intention persists. Therefore, just as it is functional to attend to people with angry facial expressions (so one can detect and avoid those who intend one harm), it is also functional to encode individuating features of those individuals (so one can detect and avoid them later even when the overt anger cue is gone). This reasoning suggests that angry individuals from both in-groups and out-groups are likely to command an enhanced proportion of available cognitive resources, and such allocation of resources might lead to the attenuation of the out-group homogeneity bias. An additional functional consideration suggests that perceivers may devote even more processing resources to angry outgroup members than to angry in-group members: Angry outgroup members may be judged to pose an even greater potential threat than angry in-group members. Various factors mitigate the actual threat posed by angry in-group members. For example, people more readily empathize with in-group members than with out-group members (Stürmer, Snyder, & Omoto, 2005), and empathy inhibits aggression (Loudin, Loukas, & Robinson, 2003). Moreover, high interdependency makes it more costly to harm in-group members than to harm out-group members. Interactions between groups, in contrast, are more frequently competitive (Wildschut, Pinter, Vevea, Insko, & Schopler, 2003). Thus, anger expressed by an out-group member may be perceived as more threatening than anger expressed by an ingroup member. If so, angry out-group members may receive enhanced scrutiny, and subsequently be better differentiated from one another in memory than are angry in-group members. That is, in the case of perceived anger, there may be an out-group heterogeneity bias. EXPLORATORY EVIDENCE AND THE PRESENT RESEARCH Though designed to test conceptually unrelated hypotheses, two studies from our research program allowed for preliminary exploration of the effects of angry facial expressions on the outgroup homogeneity bias. In one study, 168 White participants sequentially viewed single male faces varying in ethnicity (White, Black) and emotional expression (neutral, angry). Participants were later asked to distinguish these faces from previously unseen foils. A second study presented 206 participants with similar stimuli under more cognitively demanding circumstances: Two faces were presented simultaneously and for a shorter duration. In both of these studies, a significant interaction of ethnicity and facial expression emerged, F(1, 205) 5 48.38, p <.001, p rep 5 1.0, Z p 2 ¼ :19, and F(1, 167) 5 4.93, p <.05, p rep 5.91, Z p 2 ¼ :03, respectively. Results for neutral faces were consistent with the usual out-group homogeneity bias lower recognition accuracy for neutral Black faces than for neutral White faces. For angry faces, however, the results diverged. In the first study, the usual homogeneity bias was eliminated: Angry Black faces were recognized just as accurately as angry White faces. In the second study, angry Black faces were recognized more accurately than angry White faces; that is, participants exhibited an out-group heterogeneity bias. Results of these studies provided initial evidence that the outgroup homogeneity bias might be eliminated, and perhaps even reversed, for angry facial expressions. However, neither study was specifically designed to test these possibilities. We therefore designed an experiment to test the hypotheses directly and to investigate one factor that may have led to the reversal of the out-group homogeneity bias in the second study cognitive constraint. Previous research suggests that functional biases are often revealed more strongly when cognitive resources are limited (e.g., Maner et al., 2003; Meissner & Brigham, 2001). Applying this logic, a finding that processing constraints are associated with a reversal (and not merely the mitigation) of the out-group homogeneity bias would be consistent with the notion that angry out-group faces are functionally important enough to demand even more immediate processing than in-group faces. METHOD Participants, Design, and Stimuli One hundred ninety-two White undergraduate students (117 male) participated in exchange for course credit. The experiment was a 2 (target race: Black, White) 2 (target expression: neutral, angry) 2 (distractor presence: present, absent) 3 (presentation duration: 500 ms, 1,000 ms, 4,000 ms) mixed design. Target race and target expression were manipu- Volume 17 Number 10 837

They All Look the Same to Me lated within participants, and presentation duration and distractor presence were manipulated between participants. Presentation stimuli included sixteen 5-in. 3.5-in. grayscale front-oriented male faces (Black and White faces that were either angry or neutral). For participants in the distractorpresent condition, 16 similarly sized gray-scale images of abstract art were randomly paired with the faces. Sixteen new faces (also Black and White faces that were either angry or neutral) were employed as foils in the recognition memory test. The foils and studied faces were counterbalanced across participants. Procedure Each participant was seated at a computer, and an electrode was attached to his or her arm (purportedly to assess galvanic skin response, but the electrode was actually just a ruse). Participants then viewed the stimulus faces presented randomly in slide-show format with a 3,000-ms delay between successive slides. Participants next watched a 5-min distractor film clip (consisting of landscapes) and then completed the recognition memory task (including previously presented faces and foils). For each photograph, participants responded on a 6-point scale ranging from definitely did not see to definitely did see. Recognition was assessed by transforming these confidence ratings into binary yes/no judgments and computing nonparametric signal detection measures of sensitivity (A 0 ) and response bias (B 00 d; for computational details, see Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999, and Donaldson, 1992). Sensitivity reflects recognition accuracy and reveals the extent to which participants are able to differentiate previously seen stimuli from newly presented foils. Response bias reflects a participant s general threshold for responding did see versus did not see to a stimulus category. These statistics were calculated for each combination of target race and target expression. RESULTS Table 1 summarizes results for sensitivity (A 0 ) and response bias (B 00 d), as well as the hit and false alarm rates from which these measures were derived. Participant s sex produced no significant effects on these measures (all Fs < 1); therefore, we collapsed across sex for all analyses reported. For statistical tests, we report both p rep values and standard p values, as indicators of replicability and significance, respectively (see Killeen, 2005). Sensitivity (Recognition Accuracy) Our focal hypotheses were tested via a 2 (target race) 2 (target expression) repeated measures analysis of variance on sensitivity (A 0 ). The predicted two-way interaction emerged, F(1, 191) 5 44.90, p <.001, p rep >.99, Z p 2 ¼ :19. Planned contrasts revealed better recognition accuracy for neutral White faces than for neutral Black faces, F(1, 191) 5 TABLE 1 Recognition Memory for Target Faces as a Function of Target Race and Target Expression Target face Hit rate False alarm rate A 0 B 00 d White, neutral.677 (.28).121 (.21).844 (.17).406 (.71) Black, neutral.793 (.24).421 (.28).742 (.23).371 (.69) White, angry.773 (.24).223 (.26).833 (.19).022 (.98) Black, angry.802 (.24).178 (.23).873 (.15).058 (.75) Note. A 0 5 sensitivity (recognition accuracy); B 00 d 5 response bias. A 0 typically ranges from.5 to 1 (.5 5 chance, 1 5 perfect accuracy). For ease of interpretation, we report B 00 d values on a scale from 1 (did not see) to1(did see). Standard deviations are in parentheses. 36.72, p <.001, p rep >.99, Z p 2 ¼ :16 the familiar out-group homogeneity bias. In particular, the false alarm rate was higher for the neutral Black faces than for the neutral White faces, F(1, 191) 5 192.50, p <.001, p rep >.99, Z p 2 ¼ :50, indicating that participants had difficulty differentiating the previously seen neutral Black faces from the neutral Black foils. In contrast, recognition accuracy was better for angry Black faces than for angry White faces, F(1, 191) 5 7.27, p <.01, p rep 5.957, Z p 2 ¼ :04 the predicted out-group heterogeneity bias. There were fewer false alarms to angry Black faces than to angry White faces, F(1, 191) 5 5.37, p 5.02, p rep 5.924, Z p 2 ¼ :03, meaning participants had relatively little difficulty differentiating between previously seen angry Black faces and angry Black foils. Indeed, participants were much better at differentiating among angry Black faces than differentiating among neutral Black faces, F(1, 191) 5 60.04, p <.001, p rep >.99, Z p 2 ¼ :24. Figure 1 presents the results for recognition accuracy in each of the six conditions defined by the manipulation of processing constraints (distractor presence/absence, presentation duration). A planned contrast comparing the Target Race Target Expression interaction in the most highly constrained condition (500-ms duration, distractor present) and the least constrained condition (4,000-ms presentation, distractor absent) indicated a significant change in the strength of the memory crossover, F(1, 186) 5 4.51, p <.05, p rep 5.88, Z p 2 ¼ :02. These results support the idea that the out-group heterogeneity bias for angry faces may emerge primarily when processing ability is limited. Response Bias Research on cross-race identification reveals a more general set of response biases in addition to biases in recognition accuracy (e.g., Slone, Brigham, & Meissner, 2000; Sporer, 2001). Perceivers tend to show a low threshold for indicating recognition of out-group faces (i.e., typically reporting did see to out-group faces regardless of whether they have previously been encountered), whereas they show a more stringent threshold for indicating recognition of in-group faces (i.e., typically reporting did not see to in-group faces regardless of whether they have previously been encountered). 838 Volume 17 Number 10

J.M. Ackerman et al. DISCUSSION Fig. 1. Memory sensitivity (A 0 ) as a function of target race and target expression. Results for the distractor-present condition (on the left) and for the distractor-absent condition (on the right) are shown for exposure durations of 500 ms, 1,000 ms, and 4,000 ms (from top to bottom). The effects of processing constraints are highlighted by comparing the outlined panels, which present the results for the conditions with the greatest processing constraints (distractor present, 500-ms presentation) and the least processing constraints (distractor absent, 4,000-ms presentation). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. A 2 (target race) 2 (target expression) repeated measures analysis of variance on the response-bias measure revealed an interaction of target race with target expression, F(1, 191) 5 70.43, p <.001, p rep >.99, Z p 2 ¼ :27 (see Table 1). Planned contrasts revealed the expected pattern of response biases for neutral faces, F(1, 191) 5 134.66, p <.001, p rep >.99, Z p 2 ¼ :41: Participants exhibited a liberal threshold for recognizing neutral Black faces and a conservative threshold for recognizing neutral White faces. However, these response biases were entirely absent for responses to angry faces (as indicated by B 00 d scores that were not meaningfully different from zero; all ps >.35). These effects were not moderated by either of the two processing-constraint manipulations (all ps >.17). People often have unusual difficulties accurately identifying previously encountered members of ethnic out-groups, relative to members of their own ethnic group. In reviewing the evidence, Chance and Goldstein (1996) observed that the number of studies that have replicated [this effect] is impressive. Few psychological findings are so easy to duplicate (p. 171). Indeed, our results duplicated this effect exactly but only for target individuals with affectively neutral facial expressions. The out-group homogeneity bias disappeared entirely for angry faces. In fact, angry Black faces were identified more accurately than were angry White faces indicating an out-group heterogeneity bias. Recent research suggests other limits to the out-group homogeneity bias. Johnson and Fredrickson (2005) found a mitigation of this bias in memory for neutral faces when perceivers experienced positive emotion. The findings presented here differ in an important way: We have demonstrated a circumstance in which there is no such bias to remediate when the targets faces are angry. Our findings are consistent with a functional perspective on person perception. According to this perspective, cognitive resources are selectively allocated (often without conscious awareness or intent) to individuals who, on the basis of superficial characteristics or environmental setting, appear especially relevant to perceivers functional outcomes (e.g., Kurzban, Tooby, & Cosmides, 2001; Maner et al., 2003; Öhman et al., 2001; von Hippel, Sekaquaptewa, & Vargas, 1995). A growing body of research suggests that threat cues trigger elevated stereotypical beliefs about ethnic out-groups along trait dimensions associated with danger. For instance, danger-relevant (but not danger-irrelevant) stereotypes about Blacks and other out-groups are activated more strongly when perceivers are in the dark an environmental cue connoting vulnerability than when they are in a lighted space, and this effect is especially strong among perceivers who are chronically concerned about danger (Schaller, Park, & Mueller, 2003). Similarly, fear induces a strong tendency for White participants to erroneously see anger (but not fear or other emotions) in affectively neutral faces of Black men (Maner et al., 2005). It may seem paradoxical that threat cues can lead both to increased stereotyping of ethnic out-groups and to the elimination of the out-group homogeneity bias. However, a functional approach presumes that very different psychological mechanisms are responsible for categorical representations of groups, on the one hand, and individuating representations of individual group members, on the other (Smith & DeCoster, 2000). In a study consistent with this logic, Judd and Park (1988) reported that a competitive (but not a cooperative) intergroup context led simultaneously to both categorical judgments of similarity within an out-group ( they re all the same ) and greater recognition accuracy for individual members of that out-group. This pattern is consistent with the functional approach outlined here: Effective self-protection may be facilitated by the stereotypical Volume 17 Number 10 839

They All Look the Same to Me presumption that all out-group members are potentially dangerous, as well as by additional mechanisms that allow differentiation between out-group members, to identify those who actually pose the greatest threat. The out-group homogeneity bias is an important phenomenon in person perception and has real consequences. It is justifiably highlighted in textbook treatments of eyewitness identification, stereotyping, and social cognition more generally. Although powerful and real, this bias is not ubiquitous. As illustrated here, the same functional analysis that explains the out-group homogeneity bias also predicts realistic circumstances in which it disappears entirely. Acknowledgments This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH64734 to Douglas T. Kenrick, Steven L. Neuberg, and Mark Schaller. We thank Noah Goldstein, Uriah Anderson, Chad Mortensen, and Elaine Perea for their helpful feedback on previous versions of this article. REFERENCES Anthony, T., Copper, C., & Mullen, B. (1992). Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 296 301. Chance, J.E., & Goldstein, A.G. (1996). The other-race effect and eyewitness identification. In S.L. Sporer & R.S. Malpass (Eds.), Psychological issues in eyewitness identification (pp. 153 176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Donaldson, W. (1992). Measuring recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 275 278. Fiske, A.P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689 723. Fox, E., Lester, V., Russo, R., Bowles, R.J., Pichler, A., & Dutton, K. (2000). Facial expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition and Emotion, 14, 61 92. Haselton, M.G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P.W. (2005). The evolution of cognitive bias. In D.M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 724 746). New York: Wiley. Johnson, K.J., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2005). We all look the same to me : Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875 881. Judd, C.M., & Park, B. (1988). Out-group homogeneity: Judgments of variability at the individual and group levels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 778 788. Kenrick, D.T., Sadalla, E.K., & Keefe, R.C. (1998). Evolutionary cognitive psychology: The missing heart of modern cognitive science. In C. Crawford & D.L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 485 514). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Killeen, P.R. (2005). An alternative to null-hypothesis significance tests. Psychological Science, 16, 345 353. Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 98, 15387 15392. Loudin, J.L., Loukas, A., & Robinson, S. (2003). Relational aggression in college students: Examining the roles of social anxiety and empathy. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 430 439. Maner, J.K., Kenrick, D.T., Becker, D.V., Delton, A.W., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C.J., & Neuberg, S.L. (2003). Sexually selective cognition: Beauty captures the mind of the beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1107 1120. Maner, J.K., Kenrick, D.T., Becker, D.V., Robertson, T.E., Hofer, B., Neuberg, S.L., Delton, A.W., Butner, J., & Schaller, M. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 63 78. Meissner, C.A., & Brigham, J.C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3 35. Neuberg, S.L., Kenrick, D.T., Maner, J.K., & Schaller, M. (2004). From evolved motives to everyday mentation: Evolution, goals, and cognition. In J. Forgas & K. Williams (Eds.), Social motivation: Conscious and unconscious processes (pp. 133 152). New York: Cambridge University Press. Oda, R. (1997). Biased face recognition in the prisoner s dilemma games. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 309 315. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466 478. Ostrom, T.M., & Sedikides, C. (1992). Out-group homogeneity effects in natural and minimal groups. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 536 552. Rodin, M.J. (1987). Who is memorable to whom: A study of cognitive disregard. Social Cognition, 5, 144 165. Schaller, M., Park, J.H., & Faulkner, J. (2003). Prehistoric dangers and contemporary prejudices. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 105 137. Schaller, M., Park, J.H., & Mueller, A. (2003). Fear of the dark: Interactive effects of beliefs about danger and ambient darkness on ethnic stereotypes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 637 649. Slone, A.E., Brigham, J.C., & Meissner, C.A. (2000). Social and cognitive factors affecting the own-race bias in Caucasians. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 71 84. Smith, E.R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108 121. Sporer, S.L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 36 97. Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 137 149. Stürmer, S., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A.M. (2005). Prosocial emotions and helping: The moderating role of group membership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 532 546. Todd, P.M., Hertwig, R., & Hoffrage, U. (2005). Evolutionary cognitive psychology. In D.M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 776 802). New York: Wiley. von Hippel, W., Sekaquaptewa, D., & Vargas, P. (1995). On the role of encoding processes in stereotype maintenance. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 177 254. Wildschut, T., Pinter, B., Vevea, J.L., Insko, C.A., & Schopler, J. (2003). Beyond the group mind: A quantitative review of the interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 689 722. (RECEIVED 10/1/05; REVISION ACCEPTED 3/1/06; FINAL MATERIALS RECEIVED 3/13/06) 840 Volume 17 Number 10