Sarah C. Wymer, Jonathan Tarbox, Gracie A. Beavers & Christopher A. Tullis

Similar documents
COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

National University of Ireland, Maynooth

PERSPECTIVE TAKING AND NONLITERAL LANGUAGE OUTLINE DERIVED RELATIONAL RESPONDING

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

The Effects of Multiple-Tact and Receptive-Discrimination Training on the Acquisition of Intraverbal Behavior

Using a Behavioral Skills Approach to Train a Mother to Teach Basic Learning and Self-Help Skills to Her Daughter Diagnosed with Rett Syndrome

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

HELPING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM FIND THEIR VOICE FINAL EXAM

The Core Elements of Applied Verbal Behaviour AVB. Dr. Chafica Mansour Gharbieh Education & ABA/VB Specialist

The Role of Joint Control in Teaching Complex Behavior to Children with Autism

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

Teaching Two Children Diagnosed with Autism to Tolerate Physical Contact

An Investigation of Stimulus Pairing and Listener Training to Establish Emergent Intraverbals in Children with Autism

A COMPARISON OF SIMULTANEOUS VERSUS SEQUENTIAL PRESENTATION OF NOVEL FOODS IN THE TREATMENT OF FOOD SELECTIVITY

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

A Suggested Methodology for the Application of Parent Training in Teaching Verbal Behavior to Children with Autism

Emergence of Equivalence Relations: Comparing Sorting and Match-to-Sample Procedures. Lindsay J. Grimm. The New England Center for Children

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism

Teaching Staff Who Work With Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders to Evaluate the Treatment Integrity of Discrete-Trials Teaching Sessions

Cultivating Motivation During Natural Environment Teaching and Group Instruction for Diverse Learners. Pam Salerno Aja Weston Michael Houck

Behavioral Development Bulletin. Mary E. McDonald 1, Sharon A. Reeve 2, and Erin J. Sparacio 3. Volume 19 Number 1 March 2014

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

A Comparison of a Matrix Programming and Standard Discrete Trial Training Format to Teach Two-Component Tacts

A Comparison of Two Interventions to Treat Food Selectivity. A Thesis Presented. Shannon C. Garvey

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010

STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESSFUL INSTRUCTION

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

Generalization across Modalities of Expressive and Receptive Language. Acquisition through the Use of Multiple Exemplar Training and Errorless

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Teaching Children with ASD Intraverbal Responses About the Past

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

Comparison of Two Approaches for Identifying Reinforcers in Teaching Figure Coloring to Students with Down Syndrome

A COMPARISON OF PROCEDURES TO ESTABLISH EMERGENT INTRAVERBALS IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS. Mary Vallinger

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

Assessing Relational Networks: An Evaluation of Derived Relational Responding With Children With ASD and Typically Developing Children

Acquisition and Generalization of Tacts across Stimulus Modes in Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder

OBSERVATIONAL EFFECTS ON PREFERENCE SELECTION FOR FOUR CHILDREN ON THE AUTISM SPECTRUM: A REPLICATION

6/11/2017. Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism. stereotypy. Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D.

Evaluation of a Self-Instructional Package for Teaching Tutors to Conduct Discrete Trials Teaching with Children with Autism.

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Acquisition of Picture Exchange-Based vs. Signed Mands and Implications to Teach Functional Communication Skills to Children with Autism

TRANSFER OF MAND TOPOGRAPHIES TO TACT RELATIONS AND VICE VERSA IN TWO VOCAL-VERBAL CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Jill E. Castellani, B.A.

Glossary.

Implementing Discrete Trial Teaching

Evaluating Picture Exchange and the ipad as a Speech Generating Device to Teach Communication to Young Children with Autism

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

Using Pivotal Response Training with Peers in Special Education to Facilitate Play in Two Children with Autism

topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION JUNE 2012 ISSUE 12

Effects of Discrimination Abilities on Functional Analysis Outcomes. Abstract. Authors. Ashley Greenwald, Holly Seniuk, W.

TEACHING CHILDREN WITH AUTISM TO TELL SOCIALLY APPROPRIATE LIES RYAN BERGSTROM AND ADEL C. NAJDOWSKI MARISELA ALVARADO JONATHAN TARBOX

Derived Textual Control in Recreational Activity Schedules with Children with Autism

The Creative Porpoise Revisited

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

35 th Annual Conference of the Berkshire Association For Behavior Analysis

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

Utilizing Group Teaching Interaction and Generalization Procedure to Improve Social Skills of Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder

An Evaluation of Mand and Tact Assessment Procedures

The Efficacy of a Video Self-Modeling Intervention on Peer Social Initiation Skills of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)

Establishing Operations and the Mand

Empowering Families and Children with Autism through STEPS: Screening, Teaching, Evaluating, and Parenting for Success!

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

PECS and Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Andy Bondy, Ph.D. Lori Frost, MS, CCC-SLP

---EBP Brief Packet---

Comparing the Teaching Interaction Procedure to Social Stories: A Replication Study

Evaluating the Emergence of Reverse Intraverbals in Children with Autism

Reinforcing Effects of Social Stimuli on Responding After. Implementation of A Pairing Procedure. A Thesis Presented. Amanda J.

Teaching Language to Young Children. Functional Analysis and Intervention

BEHAVIOR REDUCTION PLAN

WESTERN MICHIGAN UNIVERSITY

Teaching Communication at Home

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S.

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Increasing Independent Task Engagement in an Individual. with Autism. A thesis presented. Jessica Paredes

Transcription:

Teaching Children with Autism to Follow Rules Specifying a Behavior and Consequence Sarah C. Wymer, Jonathan Tarbox, Gracie A. Beavers & Christopher A. Tullis The Analysis of Verbal Behavior ISSN 0889-9401 DOI 10.1007/s40616-016-0059-1 1 23

Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Association for Behavior Analysis International. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be selfarchived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self-archive your article, please use the accepted manuscript version for posting on your own website. You may further deposit the accepted manuscript version in any repository, provided it is only made publicly available 12 months after official publication or later and provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be accompanied by the following text: "The final publication is available at link.springer.com. 1 23

DOI 10.1007/s40616-016-0059-1 BRIEF REPORT Teaching Children with Autism to Follow Rules Specifying a Behavior and Consequence Sarah C. Wymer 1,2 & Jonathan Tarbox 3 & Gracie A. Beavers 1 & Christopher A. Tullis 1 # Association for Behavior Analysis International 2016 Abstract Rule-governed behavior (RGB) results from contact with a verbal description of a contingency as opposed to prior contact with that contingency. Despite its importance, research on the establishment of RGB with learners who do not display the skill is limited. Tarbox, Zuckerman, Bishop, Olive, and O Hora (The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 125-139, 2011) used multiple-exemplar training (MET) to teach children with autism spectrum disorder to follow rules specifying an antecedent and a behavior. We conducted a systematic replication of the Tarbox et al. study with three boys diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder and extended those methods to rules specifying a behavior and either a preferred or nonpreferred consequence (e.g., BIf you clap, then you get candy^). In baseline, participants typically followed a given instruction regardless of whether the consequence was preferred or nonpreferred. Following MET, all participants responded accurately to novel rules, indicating that MET may be an effective method to establish basic RGB repertoires. Keywords Autism spectrum disorder. Multiple-exemplar training. Rule following. Rule-governed behavior Rule-governed behavior (RGB) is behavior that is evoked by a verbal stimulus specifying contingencies as opposed to direct contact with those contingencies (Skinner, 1969). RGB is an essential skill and offers enormous advantages to those * Sarah C. Wymer swymer1@student.gsu.edu 1 2 3 Georgia State University, 30 Pryor Street, Atlanta, GA 30302, USA Marcus Autism Center, 1920 Briarcliff Road NE, Atlanta, GA 30324, USA FirstSteps for Kids, 3424 Motor Avenue, Los Angeles, CA 90034, USA

who engage in it. It allows individuals to respond effectively in situations without a history of contacting the contingencies. A large portion of human society relies on the use of rules, as rules allow individuals to benefit from the experiences of other people instead of only learning through direct contact with contingencies (Skinner, 1969, 1974). In short, rules allow humans to move forward cumulatively by building upon the past experiences of other humans. Without RGB, humanity would, quite literally, need to Breinvent the wheel^ with each new generation. Despite the importance of RGB, only one published study on establishing RGB in learners who do not already have an established repertoire exists. Tarbox et al. (2011) taught children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) to respond to simple rules specifying an antecedent and behavior (e.g., BIf this is a dog, then clap your hands^) through multiple-exemplar training (MET). Participants were directly trained on a number of rules, and after each set of rules was mastered, generalization probes were conducted. Although all participants eventually demonstrated accurate responding across a large variety of untrained rules, some participants required extensive training and modifications to both the format of the rules and the generalization probes. Tarbox and colleagues hypothesized that some participants may have lacked prerequisite skills to readily master novel rules. Tarbox et al. (2011) provided an initial investigation on teaching RGB but only investigated rules describing antecedents and behaviors. While such rules are important, rules that describe consequences are equally, if not more, important in everyday life. For example, parents and teachers may often ask children to engage (or not engage) in particular behaviors and will state the relevant consequence that the child will then contact or not contact. In such cases, if the rule describes a nonpreferred or aversive consequence, the child with an intact rule-following repertoire would not follow the instruction. If the consequence described is preferred, however, the child should engage in the behavior. For example, if a child is told, BGo line up if you want to play kickball,^ whether the child should line up or not depends on whether the consequence (e.g., playing kickball) is momentarily preferred or not preferred. Therefore, research is needed to determine whether the multiple-exemplar procedure evaluated by Tarbox and colleagues is effective for establishing RGB when rules that describe behaviors and consequences are provided. The primary purpose of the current study was to extend the Tarbox et al. study by teaching rules describing behaviors and consequences. We also extended the Tarbox et al. study by including stricter inclusion criterion for prerequisite skills and including treatment fidelity data collection and analysis. Method Participants, Setting, and Materials Three boys diagnosed with ASD participated. Jack was 5 years old and Ben and Bailey were 4-year-old identical twins. All three children attended an intensive language intervention program, and clinicians working with the children reported that the participants were unable to follow if/then rules. All participants were given the Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program (VB-MAPP; Sundberg, 2008) upon

admission to the program, and each demonstrated most of the skills in the 18 30-month range, with emerging skills in the 30 48-month range. The inclusion criteria included demonstration of emergence of untrained responses following tact and/or listener training during the participants regular clinical programming. That is, if the child was taught to tact a picture, the child also identified as a listener (i.e., receptively) the picture without additional training, and vice versa (also referred to as generalized symmetry or naming; Greer, Stolfi, & Pistoljevic, 2007). This criterion was a modification from the Tarbox et al. (2011) study and was used because generalized derived symmetry has been proposed as a prerequisite and component skill of other more complex relational skills, such as rulefollowing (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). Experimental Design A nonconcurrent, multiple-baseline design across participants (Kazdin, 2011) was used to evaluate the effects of the independent variable. Following mastery of a set of rules in training, generalization probes were conducted. Response Measurement, Interobserver Agreement, and Treatment Fidelity The primary dependent measure was the percentage of correct independent responses, defined as engaging in the specified behavior when the consequence was preferred or engaging in any behavior except for the one specified when the consequence was nonpreferred. A second, independent observer collected data to assess interobserver agreement (IOA) and treatment fidelity during all phases of the study across all participants. To calculate IOA, the number of trials with an agreement between observers was divided by the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplied by 100 %. To calculate treatment fidelity, each step of each phase was listed, and the observer used the list of steps to record and calculate the total percentage of correct implementation. For baseline sessions, IOA and treatment fidelity data were collected for 33.33, 40.00, and 42.85 % of sessions for Jack, Ben, and Bailey, respectively, with an average agreement of 100 % and an average percentage of correct implementation of 99.2 %. For treatment sessions, data were collected for 33.33, 50.00, and 33.33 % of sessions for Jack, Ben, and Bailey, respectively, with an average agreement of 100 % and an average percentage of correct implementation of 99.6 %. For generalization probes, data were collected for 50.00, 50.00, and 33.33 % of sessions for Jack, Ben, and Bailey, respectively, with an average agreement of 100 % and an average percentage of correct implementation of 98.8 %. Procedures Pre-assessments All rules across all phases were presented in the format of BIf you instruction, thenconsequence^ (e.g., If you clap, then you get the ball). Prior to baseline, each listener instruction and the identification of each consequence were probed in isolation, and the participant was required to respond correctly three consecutive times in order to include the instruction or consequence. Items to be used as consequences were identified through therapist and parent report in addition to vocal requests from participants. Table 1 displays a list of all instructions and consequences used during the study.

General Procedures Prior to each session and across all phases, the therapist presented each of the preferred and nonpreferred stimuli to the child in order to identify preferred and nonpreferred items to use during that session. Items that were consumed Table 1 Listener instructions and preferred (P) and nonpreferred (NP) consequences for each participant Instruction Participant(s) Consequence Participant(s) Stand up Jack, Ben, Bailey Ball toy Jack (P), Bailey (P) Stick out tongue Jack, Bailey Batman toy Ben (P), Bailey (P) Stomp feet Jack, Ben Book Jack (P) Thumbs up Bailey Broccoli Jack (NP) Tickle the table Jack, Ben, Bailey Brush teeth Bailey (NP) Touch head Jack, Ben, Bailey Bubbles Ben (P) Touch tummy Jack, Ben Carrots Jack (NP) Touch arm Bailey Cars Ben (P), Bailey (P) Touch chair Bailey Celery Jack (NP) Touch ear Ben, Bailey Chips Jack (P) Touch feet Jack Chocolate Ben (P), Bailey (P) Touch floor Ben, Bailey Draw shapes Bailey (NP) Touch hair Ben Drum Bailey (P) Touch mouth Bailey Elmo guitar Jack (P), Bailey (P) Touch nose Jack, Ben, Bailey Envelope Bailey (NP) Touch shirt Bailey Fruit snacks Jack (P), Ben (P), Bailey (P) Touch shoes Jack, Ben Goldfish Jack (P), Ben (P), Bailey (P) Touch shoulders Jack, Bailey Green beans Jack (NP), Ben (NP), Bailey (NP) Touch teeth Jack Gummy bears Jack (P), Bailey (P) Wave Jack, Ben, Bailey ipad Ben (P) Wiggle fingers Bailey M&Ms Jack (NP), Ben (P), Bailey (P) Wiggle your arms Jack, Ben Oreos Jack (P) Stand up Jack, Ben, Bailey Paper Bailey (NP) Stick out tongue Jack, Bailey Paper towel Ben (NP) Bailey (NP) Stomp feet Jack, Ben Peas Jack (NP), Ben (NP), Bailey (NP) Thumbs up Bailey Pete the Cat Jack (P) Tickle the table Jack, Ben, Bailey Plate Ben (NP), Bailey (NP) Touch head Jack, Ben, Bailey Play doh Ben (P), Bailey (P) Touch tummy Jack, Ben Reese s Jack (NP) Touch arm Bailey Sort silverware Ben (NP), Bailey (NP) Touch chair Bailey Spin toy Bailey (P) Spoon Ben (NP) Tomato Jack (NP) Trace letters Ben (NP) Trace numbers Bailey (NP) Trash Ben (NP), Bailey (NP) Vegetable juice Jack (NP), Ben (NP), Bailey (NP)

and/or interacted with were included as preferred consequences for that session, whereas items that were not consumed or interacted with were included as nonpreferred consequences for that session. It was planned that, if a participant did not consume/ interact with any potential preferred items presented on a given day (or did consume/ interact with potential nonpreferred items presented), then no sessions would be conducted that day. This never occurred for nonpreferred items and occurred once each for Ben and Bailey for preferred items. Prior to starting a session, the therapist pointed to and vocally tacted each item and then stated BYou can have the (nonpreferred item name) or the (preferred item name)^ while pointing to the items. Across all phases, the order of rule presentation was randomized prior to the session. Baseline Each session consisted of six trials with three rules describing preferred consequences and three rules describing nonpreferred consequences. Each consequence was paired with one of six instructions. At the outset of each trial, a rule such as BIf you clap, then you get the broccoli^ was presented. If the child engaged in the specified behavior, the therapist provided the specified consequence. If the child engaged in another behavior or did not respond, the therapist did not provide the specified consequence and ended the trial. The therapist periodically interspersed trials of mastered tasks and reinforced compliance with those tasks using preferred items not used in the study. Training Each session consisted of eight trials. One preferred consequence and one nonpreferred consequence were used for each set of training rules and were paired with four instructions. For each trial, the therapist presented the rule and the opportunity to respond independently. If the child engaged in the specified behavior, the therapist provided the specified consequence. If the child did not engage in the specified behavior, the therapist did not provide the specified consequence. If a participant responded incorrectly, he was given specific feedback (e.g., BYou didn t want broccoli, so you shouldn t have clapped.^)in a neutral voice. If the child engaged in an incorrect response on the initial independent opportunity, the therapist then implemented an error correction procedure. The error correction procedure included physically prompting the specified behavior when the consequence was preferred and physically prompting an alternative behavior, such as putting the participant s hands on the table, when the consequence was nonpreferred. The rule was repeatedly presented with prompts faded after each presentation (see Figs. 1 and 2 for prompt fading steps). Mastery criteria consisted of correct responding on seven out of eight trials on the initial independent opportunity across two sessions. Generalization Probes Following mastery of a set of rules, generalization probes were conducted to determine whether participants could respond correctly to novel rules. Sessions consisted of six rule presentations, with no behaviors or consequences described in the rules used at any previous time in the study. Therapist responses were identical to those in baseline. If a participant responded correctly to fewer than five out of six trials, the participant returned to training with a new set of rules. If the participant responded correctly to five or six trials, another probe was conducted. If the participant responded correctly to at least five trials, their participation was completed. If the participant responded correctly to four or fewer trials, the participant returned to training with a new set of rules.

Rules with a Preferred Consequence Therapist presents the rule Therapist does not provide item and gives specific feedback Therapist provides preferred item and praise Therapist re-presents rule, physically guides child to follow instruction, and prompts to say, I do want Therapist provides preferred item and praise Therapist physically prompts child to respond correctly and then begins next trial Therapist re-presents rule, models instruction and, prompts child to say, I do want Therapist provides preferred item and praise Therapist re-presents rule with no prompts Therapist provides preferred item and praise l Therapist does not provide item, gives specific feedback, and begins next trial Fig. 1 The training procedures when the specified consequence was preferred Results Results for all three participants are displayed in Fig. 3. During baseline, all three participants responded correctly during approximately 50 % of sessions (i.e., chance level). Following the introduction of training, Jack and Ben met mastery criteria following three and four training sessions, respectively. Jack and Ben responded correctly to all trials (specifying novel rules) during two consecutive generalization probe sessions. Bailey met mastery criteria for the first set of rules following 15

Rules with a Nonpreferred Consequence Therapist presents the rule Therapist provides item and specific feedback Therapist does not provide item and gives descriptive praise Therapist re-presents rule, places child s hands on table or lap, and prompts child to say, I don't want Therapist praises; no item delivered Therapist physically prompts child to respond correctly and then begins the next trial Therapist re-presents rule, shadows hands to block, and prompts child to say, I don t want Therapist praises; no item delivered Therapist re-presents rule with no prompts Therapist praises; no item delivered Therapist provides item, gives specific feedback, and begins the next trial Fig. 2 The training procedures when the specified consequence was nonpreferred sessions of training. During generalization probes, Bailey responded correctly to only four of six trials, after which he returned to training with a new set of training rules. An additional three sessions were conducted until he mastered the second set of rules. Following mastery, generalization probes were repeated (with novel rules), and Bailey responded correctly to all trials across two consecutive sessions. Discussion Results from the current study suggest that MET was effective at establishing a generalized repertoire of following untrained rules that describe behaviors and consequences. Generalization of rule following to novel rules was observed after mastery of only one (Jack and Ben) or two (Ben) sets of rules, in contrast with participants in

Fig. 3 The percentage of correct responses on independent opportunities to respond to a rule across participants Tarbox et al. (2011), who required up to 11 sets of exemplars. It is possible that the relatively quicker acquisition compared to some of the participants in the Tarbox et al. study may be the result of participant selection procedures. Tarbox et al. noted that there were no formal inclusion criteria for their study participants. In the current study, all participants demonstrated generalized symmetry or naming, suggesting that generalized symmetry may be a prerequisite for the establishment of RGB.

One noteworthy aspect of the study is that in baseline, participants contacted the contingencies described by the rules but did not begin to respond accurately to the rules until prompting procedures and praise were added during training. It was anticipated that some participants would acquire the skill during baseline. However, this never occurred despite repeated contact with the contingencies described in the rule, possibly because the participants had a long history of reinforcement for compliance with the specified instructions. Additionally, it may have been difficult to respond correctly due to the need to avoid engaging in a behavior stated by a therapist, perhaps requiring a self-control repertoire (Kanfer & Karoly, 1972). The prompting of alternative responses during treatment may have helped the participants respond effectively by providing an appropriate response to engage in instead of avoiding the named behavior. Although the results from the current investigation were positive across all three participants, four main limitations may provide avenues for future investigation. First, novel rules used during generalization probes were not probed prior to intervention, and therefore, no data on the level of responding to those rules are available. This method of generalization probes was used to assess for truly RGB (the participants had no exposure to the rules prior to the probe), to allow for reinforcement of instances of generalization (Stokes & Baer, 1977), and to prevent repeated presentations of a rule to which responding was never reinforced from serving as an S Δ (Tarbox et al., 2011). Future researchers may correct this limitation by probing rules used for generalization probes a limited number of times in order to have data on the level of responding prior to intervention while still minimizing exposure to the rules. Second, preferred and nonpreferred stimuli were not tested for reinforcing effectiveness prior to inclusion in the study, but brief preference assessments were conducted prior to each session (Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, & Page, 1985). It is worth noting that sessions only needed to be cancelled on two occasions because of a participant s lack of stimulus consumption. No session was ever halted due to consumption of nonpreferred items. However, to ensure that stimuli were (or were not) reinforcing, future researchers could confirm whether stimuli functioned as reinforcers prior to including them as consequences in rules. Third, although the rules taught are likely analogous to those presented by parents and teachers in everyday life, they remained arbitrary in nature, and an assessment of generalization of RGB to completely natural environments was not included. Although the inclusion of only arbitrary rules limits the extent to which broader practical implications can be drawn regarding effectiveness, the current procedure was selected in order to maintain a high degree of control over the procedure and to exclude the possible influence of participant reinforcement history. Future research could focus on more naturalistic rules and test for generalization outside of the clinical environment. Fourth, it is unclear whether Jack and Ben required exposure to multiple exemplars prior to acquiring the skill, or if fewer examples would have been sufficient. The initial training set for both participants included eight exemplars, and both participants acquired the generalized repertoire after MET. Future research could consider probing for generalization after a smaller number of rules are acquired. The current study successfully replicated the findings of Tarbox et al. (2011) that MET can be used to establish a basic repertoire of RGB in children with ASD and extended this finding to if/then rules specifying a behavior and consequence. The results also support Skinner s (1969) analysis of rule following, which suggests that

individuals engage in RGB due to a history of reinforcement for following rules. Future research should focus on more complex rules, rules encountered in the natural environment, and teaching the ability to derive rules. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed Consent their parents. Informed consent was obtained for all individual participants included in the study from References Greer, D. R., Stolfi, L., & Pistoljevic, N. (2007). Emergence of naming in preschoolers: A comparison of multiple and single exemplar instruction. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 8, 109 131. doi:10.1080/15021149.2007.11434278. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.). (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Springer Publishing. doi:10.1007/b108413 Kazdin, A. E.(2011). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1080/07317107.2012.654458. Kanfer, F. H., & Karoly, P. (1972). Self-control: A behavioristic excursion into the lion's den. Behavior Therapy, 3, 398 416. doi:10.1016/s0005-7894(72)80140-0. Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stimulus preference and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded individuals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 249 255. doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-249. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts. doi:10.1016/s0005-7894(70)80039-9. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Vintage Books. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349 367. doi:10.1901/jaba.1977.10-349. Sundberg,M.L.(2008).Verbal behavior milestones assessment and placement program: A language and social skills assessment program for children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Concord: AVB Press. doi:10.1002/9781118660584.ese2479. Tarbox, J., Zuckerman, C. K., Bishop, M. R., Olive, M. L., & O'Hora, D. P. (2011). Rule-governed behavior: Teaching a preliminary repertoire of rule-following to children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 125 139.