How Much Should We Trust the World Values Survey Trust Question?

Similar documents
NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES UNDERSTANDING TRUST. Paola Sapienza Anna Toldra Luigi Zingales. Working Paper

Distinguishing Trust from Risk: An Anatomy of the Investment Game

UNDERSTANDING TRUST*

Trust and Cooperation: An experimental study*

Cooperation, Trust, and Economic Development: An Experimental Study in China

Cultures of Kindness: A Meta-Analysis of Trust Game Experiments

Directed Reciprocity in an Exogenous Social Network: An Experiment

Decomposing trust and trustworthiness

UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI DI SIENA QUADERNI DEL DIPARTIMENTO DI ECONOMIA POLITICA E STATISTICA. Luigi Luini Annamaria Nese Patrizia Sbriglia

Generalized trust and prosocial behavior

Measuring Trust: A Reinvestigation. Billur Aksoy a, *, Haley Harwell a,b, Ada Kovaliukaite a and Catherine Eckel a

Microfoundations of Social Capital

Decomposing Trust and Trustworthiness

Today s lecture. A thought experiment. Topic 3: Social preferences and fairness. Overview readings: Fehr and Fischbacher (2002) Sobel (2005)

Economics 970 Empirical Project

War and Relatedness Enrico Spolaore and Romain Wacziarg September 2015

TRUST AND EXPECTED TRUSTWORTHINESS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE FROM ZIMBABWEAN VILLAGES*

Trust and Reciprocity among Urban Slum Dwellers:

The Dynamic Interplay of Inequality and Trust - An Experimental Study

John F. Kennedy School of Government Harvard University Faculty Research Working Papers Series

UNIVERSITY OF SIENA DEPARTMENT OF ECONOMICS. Alessandro Innocenti Maria Grazia Pazienza. Altruism and Gender in the Trust Game 5/2006 WORKING PAPER

The Role of Gender and Race in Cooperation and Trust: Experimental Examination of Identity Based Strategic Interaction

«Is there a relation between. trust and trustworthiness?» Tamás KOVÁCS

How to identify trust and reciprocity

MEASURING TRUST* EDWARD L. GLAESER DAVID I. LAIBSON JOSÉ A. SCHEINKMAN CHRISTINE L. SOUTTER

The Dynamic Interplay of Inequality and Trust - An Experimental Study

Cooperation in Social Dilemmas, Trust and Reciprocity 1,2

Does strategic kindness crowd out prosocial behavior?

Is There Selection Bias in Laboratory Experiments?

Social Preferences of Young Adults in Japan: The Roles of Age and Gender

Do people behave in experiments as in the field? evidence from donations

Group Composition and Cooperation

Behavioral Game Theory

Can Trust Be Induced in High Schools?

Cross-racial envy and underinvestment in South African partnerships

THE DETERMINANTS OF TRUSTING AND RECIPROCAL BEHAVIOUR: EVIDENCE FROM AN INTERCULTURAL EXPERIMENT

Evaluating Trust and Trustworthiness in Social Groups and Networks

The causal effect of market participation on trust: An experimental investigation using randomized control

Representative Trust and Reciprocity: Prevalence and Determinants

The Effect of Stakes in Distribution Experiments. Jeffrey Carpenter Eric Verhoogen Stephen Burks. December 2003

Whom to trust? Choice of partner in a trust game.

WHAT DETERMINES TRUST? HUMAN CAPITAL VS. SOCIAL INSTITUTIONS: EVIDENCE FROM MANILA AND MOSCOW

Sequential Decision and Strategy Vector Methods in Ultimatum Bargaining: Evidence on the Strength of Other- Regarding Behavior

An investment game with third-party intervention

JENA ECONOMIC RESEARCH PAPERS

The Behavioural Consequences of Unfair Punishment

Topic 3: Social preferences and fairness

Whom to trust? Choice of partner in a trust game.

Trust and reciprocity: Extensions and robustness of triadic design. *

Research Dissemination and Impact: Evidence from Web Site Downloads

Conditional behavior affects the level of evolved cooperation in public good games

Tilburg University. On Representative Social Capital Bellemare, C.; Kroger, E.S. Publication date: Link to publication

DEPARTMENT OF ECONOMICS AND FINANCE COLLEGE OF BUSINESS AND ECONOMICS UNIVERSITY OF CANTERBURY CHRISTCHURCH, NEW ZEALAND

Online Appendix to: Online Gambling Behavior: The Impacts of. Cumulative Outcomes, Recent Outcomes, and Prior Use

Do People Behave in Experiments as in the Field? Evidence from Donations

TRUST AND RECIPROCITY AMONG INTERNATIONAL GROUPS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE FROM AUSTRIA AND JAPAN

A Note On the Design of Experiments Involving Public Goods

Correlation Neglect in Belief Formation

Religion, trust and reciprocity: evidence from six Latin American cities

An Experimental Investigation of Self-Serving Biases in an Auditing Trust Game: The Effect of Group Affiliation: Discussion

Repeated Interactions vs. Social Ties: Quantifying the Economic Value of Trust, Forgiveness, and Reputation Using a Field Experiment 1.

Trust and Reciprocity: Implications of Game Triads and Social Contexts*

Online Appendix A. A1 Ability

Trust as a Proxy for the Ability to Produce Local Public Goods: Testing Different Measures

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES CAN WE INFER SOCIAL PREFERENCES FROM THE LAB? EVIDENCE FROM THE TRUST GAME. Nicole M. Baran Paola Sapienza Luigi Zingales

An investment game with third-party intervention

A cash effect in ultimatum game experiments

Positive reciprocity and intentions in trust games

Implications of Game Triads for Observations of Trust and Reciprocity. By James C. Cox. University of Arizona

Author's personal copy

Leadership with Individual Rewards and Punishments

Playing the Trust Game with Other People s Money *

Reconsidering Social Capital: A Latent Class Approach

GENDER SELECTION DISCRIMINATION: EVIDENCE FROM A TRUST GAME* Robert Slonim Case Western Reserve University. September 7 th, 2004

Directed Trust and Reciprocity in a Real-Life Social Network: An Experimental Investigation

Masculinity and lying

EXPERIMENTAL ECONOMICS INTRODUCTION. Ernesto Reuben

Beauty, Gender and Stereotypes: Evidence from Laboratory Experiments

Overbidding and Heterogeneous Behavior in Contest Experiments: A Comment on the Endowment Effect Subhasish M. Chowdhury* Peter G.

The Voluntary Provision of a Public Good with Binding Multi-Round Commitments

Emanuela Carbonara. 31 January University of Bologna - Department of Economics

Identifying Social Effects in Economic Field Experiments

Playing with the Good Guys: A Public Good Game with Endogenous Group Formation

Gift-Exchange in the Field

Competition for Trophies Triggers Male Generosity

Trust and Testosterone: A Contribution to Neurosociology

What s in a Name? Using IRI Scanner Data to Evaluate Retail Food Labeling for Shell Eggs. Janet G. Peckham

Fairness behavior in an economic crisis environment: a Romanian experimental perspective

Trust, Gender and Social Capital: Experimental Evidence from Three Western European Countries. Matteo Migheli* Abstract

Behavioral Game Theory

I. Introduction. Armin Falk IZA and University of Bonn April Falk: Behavioral Labor Economics: Psychology of Incentives 1/18

Status and distrust: The relevance of inequality and betrayal aversion

What Behaviors Do Behavior Programs Change

Area Conferences 2012

Do People Behave in Experiments as in the Field?

Chapter 14 TRUST, RECIPROCITY, AND OTHER-REGARDING PREFERENCES: GROUPS VS. INDIVIDUALS AND MALES VS. FEMALES 1. INTRODUCTION

Supplementary Information Appendix for: Prenatal Sex Hormones (2D:4D Digit Ratio) and Social Integration

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES HOW WAS THE WEEKEND? HOW THE SOCIAL CONTEXT UNDERLIES WEEKEND EFFECTS IN HAPPINESS AND OTHER EMOTIONS FOR US WORKERS

Theoretical Explanations of Treatment Effects in Voluntary Contributions Experiments

Transcription:

How Much Should We Trust the World Values Survey Trust Question? Noel D. Johnson * Department of Economics George Mason University Alexandra Mislin Kogod School of Business, American University Abstract We use a unique data set of trust game replications in order to validate the commonly used trust question from the World Values Survey. We find that trust as measured by the World Values Survey is positively correlated with experimentally measured trust and unrelated to experimentally measured trustworthiness. This Version: September 19, 2011 Keywords: Trust, Trust Game, World Values Survey, Cross-Cultural Experiments JEL Codes: C91, Z10, C83 * Corresponding author. Assistant Professor, Economics Department, George Mason University. Mercatus Center at George Mason University. 3351 North Fairfax Dr., 4 th Floor, Arlington, VA 22201. njohnsol@gmu.edu.

1. Introduction Attempts to compare trust across societies often rely on survey responses contained in the World Values Survey (WVS), which was administered in five waves between 1981 and 2008 to over 250,000 respondents across 80 countries (e.g. Knack & Keefer, 1997; Zak & Knack, 2001). 1 The level of trust in a country is typically assessed using responses to the single question, Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted, or that you can t be too careful in dealing with people? The indicator of trust is the percentage of individuals in the society who respond most people can be trusted. In this paper we ask if the WVS Trust question is correlated with experimentally measured trust or trustworthiness across countries. We find it is strongly correlated with experimental trust, but not with trustworthiness. While the single item WVS Trust measure may provide insights into attitudes about trust across the markets surveyed, it is not clear whether it is an internally valid measure or predictor of general trust behavior. Surveys of people s attitudes have been weak predictors of people s actual behaviors (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977). Furthermore, the WVS measure of trust leaves the interpretation of who most people are to the survey respondents, who may interpret this differently under different circumstances in different societies (Glaeser et al., 2000). For example, some may be thinking of the people within their community, while others may be considering the institutional environment of the country and whether government officials can generally be trusted. 2 In contrast to attitudinal measures, experimental measures of trust between anonymous individuals and with monetary incentives offer a concrete exchange setting which can capture generalized trust behavior more effectively (Camerer, 2003). Behavioral measures of trust are captured in a carefully designed and controlled experimental setting which presents an exchange with real money at stake, and forces subjects to carefully consider expectations of the other person s behavior and their willingness to be vulnerable. In previous studies, however, these trust experiments have correlated poorly with surveys of trust attitudes (Glaeser et. al., 2000; Lazzarini et. al., 2005). For example, Glaeser and colleagues conducted a study examining trust measures and antecedents of trust, which compared individual responses to survey measures of trust to their actual behavior in an experimental trust game following the 1 http://www.wvsevsdb.com/wvs/wvsdata.jsp. Examples of papers which rely on the WVS Trust Question include: La Porta et. al., 1997; Fisman & Kahanna, 1999; Guiso et. al., 2006; Glaeser et. al., 2000; Lazzarini et al., 2005; Ashraf et. al., 2006; Nunn & Wantchekon, 2009. 2 Knack & Keefer (1997) offered evidence for the validity of this WVS trust measure by measuring and finding a correlation (0.67) with a sample of European countries and the percentage of wallets returned in the Reader s Digest experiments of lost wallets (See The Economist, June 22, 1996). Returning a lost wallet, however, is more a reflection of trustworthiness than trust, and this supports other research which has found that survey measures of trust correlate slightly with trustworthiness rather than willingness to trust (Lazzarini et al., 2004; Glaeser et al. 2000).

survey. They found no significant correlation between the WVS trust measure and behavior in the trust game, and concluded that the experimental measure of trust more effectively captures individual willingness to trust. 3 Lazzarini and colleagues (Lazzarini et al., 2005) extended Glaser s research by comparing the WVS results for level of trust in Brazil with actual behaviors in the BDM trust game experiments conducted in the same country. Individuals in the trust experiments were more willing to trust than the WVS findings suggested, and again there was no significant correlation between willingness to trust in the experiments and the answer to the question whether most people can be trusted. 2. Data and Model We use a newly constructed data set containing observations of trust and trustworthiness behavior from replications of the Berg, Dickhaut, and McCabe Investment Game collected across 35 countries from more than 23,000 subjects in order to test whether the WVS Trust Question is correlated with experimental trust. 4 In the original BDM Game there is a sequential exchange between a Sender and Receiver. After being anonymously paired, both players are endowed with $10. In the first stage, the Sender may pass any amount of her $10 to the Receiver. The experimenter triples this amount and gives it to the Receiver, who then may return any amount desired back to the Sender. The amount passed by the Sender as proportion of her endowment is our measure of Experimental Trust. The proportion returned by the Receiver measures Experimental Trustworthiness (Camerer, 2004, p. 85). Each observation in our data corresponds to the average trust and trustworthiness across players for a single replication of the BDM game. 5 We have 152 observations for trust and 128 for trustworthiness. The data set also includes an extensive set of controls for different experimental protocols used in each replications. 6 The data on the World Values Survey Trust Question come primarily from the most recent two collection waves corresponding to the years 1999-2004 and 2005-2008. 7 These two waves of WVS trust data are highly correlated with a Pearson s correlation coefficient of 0.91 (p-value < 3 Glaeser s study also indicated that survey questions asking about specific instances of past trust behavior rather than attitudes provided an effective measure of trust behavior. 4 The data were collected by the Authors and are described in detail in Johnson & Mislin (2011). The data are also reproduced in their entirety in the appendix to Johnson and Mislin (2011). 5 Since the variables Trust and Trustworthiness are bounded by zero and one, we choose to perform the logit transformation on each of them so as to avoid inappropriately specifying the OLS model. Thus, the dependent variables range from negative to positive infinity. 6 All variables, including the experimental controls, are described in Appendix A [Note to Editor: Appendix A not necessarily for publication. All variables are also described extensively in Johnson & Mislin, 2011]. 7 Given that BDM was published in 1995, most replications of their game didn t begin until 1999. For the Wave Five data (2005-2008), we simply use all observations available. For Wave Four (1999-2004) we use earlier waves (back to 1995) in order to maximize the overlap between countries covered by the experimental trust data and the WVS. For the Wave Four data we always use the most recent observation on a country.

0.001). The kernel density plots of each Wave shown in Figure 1 highlight the close relationship between the two groups of surveys. Given their similarities, and in order to maximize the number of countries covered, we choose to combine the two WVS waves into a single variable called WVSTrust. The final data set contains 152 observations on experimental trust covering 30 countries and 128 on trustworthiness. 8 We estimate a series of specifications based on: y ij = α + β WVSTrust i + Γ X i + φ j + ε ij where y ij represents either the logit transformed measures of experimental trust or trustworthiness for observation i in geographic region j. 9 WVSTrust is the survey measure of trust behavior, X i is a vector of methodological control variables, φ j is a vector of geographic region dummies, and ε ij is an i.i.d. error term. 3. Findings Table 1, Panel A reports the estimate for β when experimental trust is the dependent variable. Panel B reports β when experimental trustworthiness is the dependent variable. Huber-White standard errors are reported in parentheses. In brackets we report the proportion of a standard deviation in the dependent variable that a one standard deviation increase in WVSTrust has conditional on the estimated coefficient. The results in Panel A indicate a very strong positive correlation between WVSTrust and experimental trust. Across all three specifications, the coefficient on WVSTrust is positive and statistically significant. According to the full specification reported in column (3) a one standard deviation increase in the percent of survey respondents who answer yes to the WVS Trust Question is associated with an increase in a country s experimentally measured trust of about 28% of a standard deviation. In Panel B we report the value of β using experimental trustworthiness as the dependent variable. In contrast to the results in Panel A, there is no evidence of a positive correlation 8 The Trust and Trustworthiness samples do not perfectly overlap since not all experimenters collected data on Receiver behavior. In the Trust sample there are 46 observations from North America, 64 from Europe, 23 from Asia, 10 from South America, and 9 from Africa. In the Trustworthy sample there are 41 observations from North America, 53 from Europe, 15 from Asia, 13 from South America, and 9 from Africa. 9 The geographic regions are defined as North America, Europe, Asia, South America, and Africa. See Johnson and Mislin (2011) for details of how these dummies are constructed.

between experimental trustworthiness and answers to the WVS Trust Question. The coefficient estimates are neither economically nor statistically significant at conventional levels. As robustness checks we estimate, but do not report, versions of specifications (1)-(6) in which we weight observations by the number of participants in the experimental studies. We also estimate (1)-(3) using the restricted experimental trustworthiness sample of 128 studies. Finally, we estimate all specifications using the raw, untransformed, versions of the dependent variables. In all cases the results in Table 1 do not change in any substantive way. 4. Conclusions This study assesses the validity of the WVS Trust Question using data on experimental trust across countries drawn from 152 replications of the Berg, Dickhaut, and McCabe Investment Game. We find strong support for the interpretation that the WVS Trust Question measures the same thing experimenters call trust in the lab. By contrast, we find no relationship between our measure of experimental trustworthiness and answers to the WVS Trust Question. Acknowledgements We would like to thank Nathan Nunn for suggesting the idea for this study.

References Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. 1977. Attitude--Behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84: 888-918. Ashraf, N., Bohnet, I., & Piankov, N. 2006. Decomposing Trust and Trustworthiness. Experimental Economics, 3(Sept.): 193-208. Camerer, C. 2003. Behavioral Game Theory. Princeton University Press and Russell Sage Foundation. Fisman, R., & Khanna, T. 1999. Is trust a historical residue? Information flows and trust levels. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 38: 79-92. Gächter, S., Herrmann, B., & Thöni, C. 2004. Trust, voluntary cooperation, and socio-economic background: survey and experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55: 505-531. Glaeser, E. L., Laibson, D. I., Scheinkman, J. A., & Soutter, C. L. 2000. Measuring trust. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3): 811-846. Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. 2006. Does Culture Affect Economic Outcomes? The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(2): 23-48. Johnson, N. and Mislin, A. 2011. Trust Games: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, Forthcoming. Available for download from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s0167487011000869 Knack, S., & Keefer, P. 1997. Does social capital have an economic payoff? A cross-country investigation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112: 1251-1288. La Porta, R., Lopez-de-Silanes, F., Shleifer, A., & Vishny, R.W. 1997. Trust in Large Organizations. The American Economic Review, 87(2): 333-338. Lazzarini, S. G, Madalozzo, R., Artes, R., & Siqueira, J.O. (2005). Measuring trust: An experiment in Brazil. Revista de Economia Aplicada, 9(2), 153-169 Nunn, N. and Wantchekon, L. 2009. The Slave Trade and the Origins of Mistrust in Africa. NBER Working Paper 14783. Zak, P. J., & Knack, S. 2001. Trust and Growth. The Economic Journal, 111(April): 295-321.

Tables and Figures Figure 1: Distribution of Two World Values Survey Waves Kernel Density 0 2 4 6 0.2.4.6.8 WVS Wave 1999 2004 WVS Wave 2005 2008 kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = 0.0342

Table 1: Are Experimental Trust and Trustworthiness Correlated with WVS Trust Question? Panel A: Dependent Variable: Experimental Trust (proportion sent) (1) (2) (3) WVSTrust 0.912** 1.084*** 1.267** (0.420) (0.437) (0.610) [0.200] [0.238] [0.279] Experimental Controls x x Region Dummies x Observations 152 152 152 R-squared 0.040 0.168 0.120 Panel B: Dependent Variable: Experimental Trustworthiness (proportion returned) (4) (5) (6) WVSTrust 0.011-0.155-0.440 (0.357) (0.359) (0.412) [0.003] [-0.040] [-0.113] Experimental Controls x x Region Dummies x Observations 128 128 128 R-squared 0.000 0.214 0.245 Notes: The measures of experimental trust and trustworthiness are logit transformed. Huber-White standard errors reported in parentheses. Numbers in brackets indicate the percentage change in a standard deviation of the dependent variable a one standard deviation change in WVSTrust induces. *, **, and *** indicate significance at the 10, 5, and 1 percent levels respectively. Experimental controls included and definitions for the region dummies are contained in Johnson and Mislin (2011).

Appendix A Appendix: Descriptive Statistics Panel A: Trust Sample Variable Name Obs Mean Std. Dev. Min Max Variables of Interest WVS8199 151 0.32 0.15 0.03 0.66 WVS0508 131 0.36 0.14 0.09 0.68 WVSTrust 152 0.32 0.15 0.06 0.67 Experimental Trust 152 0.03 0.55-1.24 2.04 Control Variables Sender Endowment 152 20.42 47.31 0.00 238.10 Receiver Endowed 152 0.56 0.50 0.00 1.00 Anonymous 152 0.86 0.35 0.00 1.00 Rate Return 152 0.91 0.28 0.00 1.00 Double Blind 152 0.11 0.31 0.00 1.00 Student 152 0.58 0.50 0.00 1.00 Strategy Method 152 0.40 0.49 0.00 1.00 Both Roles 152 0.22 0.42 0.00 1.00 Random Payment 152 0.34 0.48 0.00 1.00 Real Person 152 0.96 0.20 0.00 1.00 Panel B: Trustworthy Sample Variable Name Obs Mean Std. Dev. Min Max Variables of Interest WVS8199 129 0.31 0.15 0.03 0.66 WVS0508 111 0.36 0.14 0.09 0.68 WVSTrust 129 0.32 0.15 0.06 0.67 Experimental Trustworthiness 129-0.55 0.53-2.11 1.46 Control Variables Experimental Trust 128 0.04 0.55-1.24 2.04 Sender Endowment 129 22.22 49.80 0.18 238.10 Receiver Endowed 129 0.56 0.50 0.00 1.00 Anonymous 129 0.85 0.36 0.00 1.00 Rate Return 129 0.90 0.30 0.00 1.00 Double Blind 129 0.12 0.33 0.00 1.00 Student 129 0.55 0.50 0.00 1.00 Strategy Method 129 0.39 0.49 0.00 1.00 Both Roles 129 0.23 0.42 0.00 1.00 Random Payment 129 0.37 0.48 0.00 1.00