Color name as a function of wavelength and instruction '

Similar documents
The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DETERMINANTS OF VISUAL BACKWARD MASKING 1

Information transmission in spectral color naming!

Psychophysical estimates of opponent-process response functions*

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

o^ &&cvi AL Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 20, Southern Universities Press 1965

The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect

LEA Color Vision Testing

BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

= add definition here. Definition Slide

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

The effect of the size of a stimulus on the perception of self-luminous colours

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

2. METHODS. 2.1 Apparatus

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972)

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

VISUAL PERCEPTION & COGNITIVE PROCESSES

The Effect of Training Context on Fixations Made During Visual Discriminations

Practice Test Questions

Perceptual separability and spatial models'

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

Supplementary Materials

GENERALIZATION GRADIENTS AS INDICANTS OF LEARNING AND RETENTION OF A RECOGNITION TASK 1

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Pigeons and the Magical Number Seven

Absolute judgments ofcolors using natural color names*

Surround articulation. I. Brightness judgments

CS 182 Sections Leon Barrett

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES?

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2003

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Stimulus dimensionality and temporal repetition*

Chromatic and luminance signals in visual memory

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

Prof. Greg Francis 7/7/08

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Unit 4: Sensation and Perception

SENSES: VISION. Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica

The path of visual attention

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Development of a new loudness model in consideration of audio-visual interaction

Taste PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 36. Taste

PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

C. L. Hardin, Are Scientific Objects Coloured?

account of the haptic perception of curvature.

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Using the ISO characterized reference print conditions to test an hypothesis about common color appearance

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be-

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Modifiers and Retransmitters (Secondary Light Sources)

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

Surround articulation. II. Lightness judgments

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

An empirical explanation of color contrast

Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces

The Perceptual Experience

Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Conditional spectrum-based ground motion selection. Part II: Intensity-based assessments and evaluation of alternative target spectra

Effect of Screen Contrast Ratio and Luminance Level on Visual Lobe Shape

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Transcription:

Color name as a function of wavelength and instruction ' ALEEZA CEF EAE, WESTINHOUSE DEFENSE AND SPACE CENTE, AEOSPACE DiVISION, ALTIMOE, MD. MICHAEL H. SIEEL, IPON COLLEE, IPON, WISCONSIN A series of experiments were performed to determine the effects of instructions upon color naming data. Although color name ~s basically a function of stimulus wavelength, even slight changes in the response categories available for the 5 led to substantial changes in the pattern of Ss' response alloc ation. ecently, several researchers have shown interest in color naming as a technique. oynton, Schafer, and Neun (1964) demonstrated that such a technique can be applied to the study of central fixation. Later reports employed color naming to study the ezold riicke effect (oynton & ordon, 1965) and the influence of exposure duration and luminance on naming (Luria, 1967). It has for some time been clear that there is a relation between stimulus wavelength and color name (eare, 1963). It is not clear how this relation is influenced by the instructions which are furnished to Ss, All of the recent studies employing color naming as an experimental technique have assumed that the particular set of instructions which they employed was satisfactory. If, however, instructions exert a significant influence on the data obtained, comparing sets of data obtained in response to differing instructions becomes a questionable practice. It is here assumed that naming instructions do indeed have an effect upon data, and that this effect depends on at least two components. The first component is the number of names allowed Ss, and the second, the nature of the name they are permitted. The present study, then, attempts to assess the role played by instructions given to Ss when the task is to name colors. It is hoped that the results will serve as a guideline for the future employment of the color naming technique. Apparatus A ausch and Lomb 500 mm monochromator provided the stimulus. A sector shutter permitteddiscrete 0.2 sec stimulus exposures. The stimulus was circular and subtended 3 deg at the observer's eye. Wavelength settings from 0 through 0 mp were employed. The stimulus could be set to either of two different luminance levels,.1 or 1.0 ft-l, while the area surrounding the stimulus was white and was maintained at a luminance of.1 ft-l, Calibrations were made both before and after the experiment upon the wavelength setting of the monochromator, the luminance levels of the stimulus and surround, and the duration of the exposure. No changes from the pre-experimental values were detected. Subjects Subjects were 24 young men from the volunteer pool available to the Edgewood Arsenal laboratories. All reported English as their native tongue, had normal color vision, and were emmetropic or wore corrective lenses during the experiment. efore the first experimental session, each S received a training session. Observation was made with the right eye only. Procedure The spectrum was sampled in 10 mp intervals from 0 mp, through 6 mu, etween 5 and 590 mu only 5 rnu steps were used so that the yellow region would be thoroughly sampled. Twenty-eight wavelengths were presented in random order. An experimental session consisted of three runs through the spectrum. Each S served for 10 sessions, five at each of the two luminance levels. efore each experimental session, Ss were told what color names would be available. Ss were then required to respond with an acceptable color name following a ready signal. Dividing the group of Ss into six subgroups allowed for a different SUbgroup to serve in each of the six experimental conditions defined below. Experimental Conditions (A) Free choice: No color names were prescribed. Ss were asked to name each color they saw as accurately as they could. () 1. Four color names: ed, Yellow, reen, or lue were permitted. 2. Four color names: ed, Orange, reen, or lue were to be used. (C) Four names with compounds: Ss were allowed the use of the four color names of Condition -1 as well as the following combinations: ed-lue, lue-reen, Yellow-reen, and Yellow-ed. Instructions regarding name precedence in compound naming were purposely omitted. (D) Six names: Only the color names Purple, lue, reen, Yellow, Orange, and ed were permitted. (E) Six names with compounds: In addition to the six color names of Condition D, Ss could use the following compounds: lue-purple, lue-reen, Yellow-reen, Yellow-Orange, and Orange-ed. As Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, VoL 2 (11) Copyright 1967, Psychonomic Press, oleta, Calif. 521

Table 1. Number of Times Each Name was Used for All Conditions Luminance Level: 0.1 Ft-L 1.0 Ft-L Exp. Condition: A -1-2 C 0 E A -1-2 C 0 E Name Purple 218 236 9 113 148 171 Purp Ie-lue 8 10 lue-ed 194 126 lue 305 537 526 308 309 325 371 509 538 346 349 384 lue-reen 3 224 57 3 163 59 reen 654 613 695 392 550 503 574 5 657 337 490 1 Yellow-reen 8 137 93 14 167 136 Yellow 4 4 42 178 95 98 238 91 225 105 Yellow-ed 221 191 Yellow-Orange 2 24 4 64 Orange 234 217 185 165 224 286 168 157 Orange-ed 1 45 5 52 ed 192 326 242 162 222 156 199 313 199 199 2 141 old 14 in Condition C, no instructions about name precedence in compound naming were given. esults Table 1 presents the total number of times each name occurred in each experimental condition for both luminance levels. Table 2 presents the median wavelength for each name distribution. Table 3 provides statistical comparisons between some of the medians from Table 2, for the high luminance con-. dition. The statistical evaluation follows the Median Test (Siegel, 1956). Figure 1 shows the results for Condition A for both Iuminances, in which choice of name was unrestricted. Although a great many different color names were employed at both luminance levels, only five distinct modes appear: Purple, lue, reen, Orange, and ed. All peak at 100% except Orange at the low luminance. Curves for Yellow never approached 100% and are shown to be the most profoundly influenced by differences in luminance. Peak frequency for Yellow dropped from over % at high luminance to approximately 30% at low luminance. There was no shift in wavelength for the named colors as a function of the two different luminance levels. The free response condition yielded a total of eight categories, represented by separate curves at the low luminance and seven at the high luminance. These represent names which occurred either with a frequency greater than four or over a minimum wavelength range of three adjacent wavelength points. Names which occurred with a frequency below either of these criteria included Turquoise, Aquamarine, Chartreuse, eige, and Lemon. Figures 2 and 3 show Conditions -1 and -2, respectively. Condition -1 divided the spectrum into lue, reen, Yellow, and ed. Yellow was replaced by Orange in Condition -2. Figures 2 and 3 show all distributions peaking at 100% except Yellow in -1. A in Condition A, the peak percent for Yellow was greater at the brighter of the two luminance levels; however, there was no shift in the wavelength location of any of the four curves either in Condition -1 or -2 as a function of the change in luminance Table 2. Median of Each Name Distribution for All Conditions (mil) Luminance Level: 0.1 Ft-L 1.0 Ft-L Exp. Condition: A -1-2 C 0 E A l -2 C 0 E Name Purple 418.5 419.8 417.5 410 423 415.5 Purple-lue 442.5 436.6 lue-ed 419.4 421.4 lue 463.9 444.9 446.8 458.2 465 463.1 449.8 442.5 446.5 459.8 463.5 462.2 lue-reen 490 507.5 494.7 490 508.7 497.8 reen 552.7 5.7 548.7 554.1 528.3 541.7 529.6 541.5 546.2 5.9 536.7 538.1 Yellow-reen 576 575.7 571.1 562.5 573 568.1 Yellow 582.8 584.3 588.8 578.6 583.8 573.6 583.9 582.9 576.7 584.2 Yellow-ed 2.5 599.9 Yellow-Orange 585 593.7 577.5 583.3 Orange 598.2 595.1 597.4 3 587.7 594.7 595.1 4.6 Orange-ed 610 617.7 610 623.7 ed 631.5 612.8 629.2 635.7 631.5 635.3 623.4 624 633.4 633.5 6 636.9 old 578.8 522 Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, Vol. 2 (11)

Table 3. Comparisons of Differences in Name-Distributions Medians Across Two Conditions. High Luminance Level Only Compared Significant at Name Cond itions' X2 Confid. Level ed -2x -1.5.005 ed E xc 5.008.025 ed 0 xa 17.1859.005 ed C xd 6.156.005 reen -2x -1 2.84.05 reen C xe 0.797 reen C xd 7.9369.005 reen 0 xa 1.74 lue E xc 1.725 lue 0 xc 3.6.05 lue A xd 43.818.005 lue-reen C xe 7.310.005 Yel lew-reen C x E 11.595.005 Yellow E xc 9.969.005 Yellow C xd 14.4659.005 Yellow 0 xa 19.7174.005 Orange 0 xa 135.2166.005 Yellow-ed in C x Orange in 0 2.976.05 Purple 0 xa 117.8.005 That condition having the median with the longer wavelength is the first listed in each pair of compared conditions. level. The wavelength range for Orange was always wider than that for Yellow. Figure 4 superimposes the curves of Figs. 2 and 3 to allow comparisons of distributions when one name is substituted for another in an otherwise identical set of categories. Curves for Orange are displaced toward the long wavelength end of the spectrum as compared with curves for Yellow. The striking finding which emerges from the substitution of Orange for Yellow is that both the range and the location of the other curves in the name set are influenced. When Orange replaces Yellow, the reen curve shows a wider wavelength range and a shift toward the long wavelength end of the spectrum; the ed curve's rise is also shifted toward the long wavelength end of the spectrum. A similar, though less pronounced shift occurs in the distributions for lue. This wavelength displacement is statistically significant at the.005 level for ed and at the.05 level for reen, and occurs independently of luminance level. esults from Condition C appear in Fig. 5. This condition specified the same four category names as Condition -1, I.e., lue, reen, Yellow, and ed, together with combinations of any two of these names. Of the eight name curves obtained at each luminance level, only those for lue and ed peak at 100%. Frequency was consistently greater for ed-lue, lue, reen, Yellow-ed, and ed than for lue reen, Yellow-reen, and Yellow. As in the other conditions, a greater number of Yellow responses occurred at the high luminance level than at the low Peaks of reen, Yellow-reen, and ed curves showed displacements toward the short wavelength end of the spectrum at the higher luminance level. Figure 6 presents data for Condition D which prescribed the name categories Purple, lue, reen, Yellow, Orange, and ed. oth the Yellow and Orange curves were relatively narrow and neither distribution reached 100%. At both luminance levels, there were fewer Orange responses in this condition than there were Yellow-ed responses in Condition C. Luminance difference did not have an appreciable effect on the data in this condition. 100 p 8 0 0.1,. 4. 100 z. "- Fig. 1. Color name distributions with unrestricted choice of name (Condition A). Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, Vol. 2 (11) 523

100 O 01 '"«100 z ;-- O 1.0 0 500 550 WAVELENTH(m~) 0 650 Fig. 2. Color name distributions for lue, reen, Yellow, and ed (Condition -1). Figure 7 presents results from Condition E, which prescribed the six names of Condition D, t.e., Purple, lue, reen, Yellow, Orange, and ed, as well as appropriate compounds of these names. Eleven name distributions were obtained at each luminance level, of which only the curves for Purple and lue reached 100%. The other predominating name distributions for this condition were reen, Yellow, Orange, and ed. Addition of the compound names reduced the width of the Yellow, Orange, and ed distributions in comparison to results for Condition D, and Yellow and Yellow-reen reached a higher percentage at the higher of the two luminances. reen was displaced toward the short wavelength end of the spectrum at the higher of the two luminance levels. Discussion Although the two luminance levels employed in these experiments were not radically different, the ezold-rticke shift in hue, as inferred from name 100 O 01 Fig. 3. Color name distributions for lue, reen, Orange, and ed (Condition -2). WAVELENTH(m~} 550 0 650 524 Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, Vol. 2 (11)

100..4 0.1,,, /", f \/ y 1\ '\ II '"~ <t Z ~ ~ \ I, " -N \,..." ~~ ~~ "'_~""""li:'lill 1-1 00 ZZ 00 uu 1.0 0 450 550 (mili Fig. 4. Comparison of the distributions obtained from Conditions -1 and -2 (Four name categories). applied, was observed. This shift in named hue with increased luminance was most apparent in the Yellow reen portion of the spectrum. In this area many stimuli were judged reen at low luminance, while at the higher luminance they were judged Yellow. The basic finding that a clear-cut relationship exists between color name and wavelength has been reaffirmed by the data from this experiment. At the same time the data demonstrate that this relationship is highly sensitive to instructional manipulation. The curves of Fig. I, from instructions which specified no categories, bear a striking similarity to those of Fig. 6, obtained in response to instructions specifying the names Purple, lue, reen, Yellow, Orange, and ed. In the absence of specific naming instructions, Ss will, nevertheless, assign the same color name to roughly the same wavelength region in a reliable and consistent manner, so that definite wavelength regions have come to correspond to definite color names. This correspondence may be more -Y Y- Fig. 5. Color name distributions for lue, reen, Yellow, ed, and compounds (Condition C). Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, Vol. 2 (11) 525

Y 0 Fig. 6. Color name distributions for Purple, lue, reen, Yellow, Orange, and ed (Condition D). refined for Sa who are highly trained in color observations, and may vary from our observed pattern when non-english speaking Sa are employed. When responding is limited by experimentally restricting the S's repertoire of color names, the resulting frequency distributions wul be similarly limited. When, on the other hand, a large number of names are made available, the peak frequencies are lower and extend over a smaller wavelength range than when fewer naming categories are available, An overall view of the data shows that the greater the prescribed response availability, the greater also the response dispersion. The basic wavelength-name relationship, however, acts as the limiting factor to this response dispersion. The remarkable influence which a change in the nature of the name exerts on the data was a major finding. esults plotted in Fig. 4 indicate that when only one response category differs, Ss wul respond differently not only to the changed category, but also 100 8 Y 0 8 ;;- Fig. 7. Color name distributions for Purple, lue, reen, Yellow, Orange, ed and compounds (Condition E). 526 Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, Vol. 2 (11)

'" et 100 0 4"" 500 550 WAVELENTH(mj.U 0 650 Fig. 8. Color name distributions for six name categories under two different experimental conditions and four luminance levels. to the ones which remain common to the two conditions. A S's response pattern, then, is determined by the combined influence of all the available categories. How the combination of both number and nature of name categories significantly affects response patterns can be studied in two ways: (1) by examining the curve for one name which occurs in two or more differing conditions, and (2) by examing the curves for two different names which are commonly used to describe the same hue. As an example for the first instance, the median wavelength for ed is significantly longer for Conditions E, D, and C than for Conditions C, A, and D, respectively (see Table 3). As an example for the second instance, the names Yellow-ed and Orange, which are generally thought of as equivalent, are shown to have significantly different median wavelengths in Conditions C and D, respectively. In Condition C both Yellow-ed and ed were available as categories, while in Condition D Orange and ed were permitted. Within each of the six experimental conditions used in this study, Ss responded highly consistently. This consistency has been observed by others employing color naming as the experimental technique (oynton & ordon, 1965, p. 86), but.ts retained only as long as the instruction components for the Ss are not changed. For this reason, a discussion of the uniqueness of a particular name (Sternheim & oynton, 1966) has no meaning except in the context of the total available name categories. Figure 8 provides the final illustration of this point by showing data from Condition D, together with data obtained by eare (1963). Exposure time, brightness levels, Ss, and wavelength exposure steps were different for the two experiments. The common features of the two experiments were the naming methodology, the fact that all luminance levels were photopic, and the similarity in number and nature of prescribed name categories. The similarity between the two sets of data confirms the conclusion that, within specified luminance levels, the same instruction set will reliably produce comparable naming distributions. Changes in response availability, although apparently minor, produce important changes in the S's response pattern. Although the choice of specific color categories must remain dependent upon the E's theoretical biases, it must be understood that this choice will have a profound influence upon the data. eferencii eare, A. C. Color-name as a function of wavelength. Amer. J. PsYcho!., 1963, 76, 248-256. oynton,. M., & ordon, J. ezold-nlcke shift measured by color-naming technique. J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1965, 55, 78-86. oynton,. M., Schafer, W., & Neun, M. E. Hue-wavelength relation measured by color-naming method for three retinal locations. Science, 1964, 145, 666-668. Luria, S. M. Color-name as a function of stimulus-intensity and duration. Amer. J. Psucnot., 1967,, 14-27. Siegel, S. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: Mcraw-Hill, 1956. Sternheim, C. E., & oynton,. M. Uniqueness of perceived hues investigated with a continuous judgemental technique. J. exp. Psychol., 1966, 72, 770-776. HOle 1. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the October 1966 meeting of the Optical Society of America. This research was conducted at Medical esearch Laboratories, esearch Laboratories, Edgewood Arsenal, Edgewood Arsenal, Maryland 21010. (Accepted for publication July 12, 1967.) Perception & Psychophysics, 1967, Vol. 2 (11) 527