Assessing Sympathy in Voir Dire: Exploring Jurors Intention-Behavior Gap

Similar documents
Improving Witness (and Attorney) Performance on Direct Examination

Juror Confirmation Bias: Powerful. Perilous. Preventable.

Selecting the Best Form of Jury Research for Your Case & Budget

The Essentials of Jury De-Selection

e Magnus Advantage Imagine... Imagine a case where you know the impact of human dynamics, in addition to the law and the issues...

Eyewitness Evidence. Dawn McQuiston School of Social and Behavioral Sciences Arizona State University

Getting to Yes. Getting to Yes: Negotiating Agreement Without Giving In - April 30, 1992

Keep the Reptile Away:

Some Possible Legal and Social Implications of Advances in Neuroscience. Henry T. Greely

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report

Research on jury instructions An experimental test of the novel NJ instruction

The Head and the Heart: Juror Perceptions of Sports Concussion Litigation

PERSUASIVE DIRECT EXAMINATION. Jerry Cox & Wil Zevely 2010

The Power of Feedback

ADDITIONAL CASEWORK STRATEGIES

Preparing Witnesses for Direct Examination Master Class: Working with Witnesses ABA 2018 Professional Success Summit By Kalpana Srinivasan

Aptitudes and Attitudes on Toxic Tort Cases

UNDERSTANDING CAPACITY & DECISION-MAKING VIDEO TRANSCRIPT

Appendix 11 GUIDELINES FOR TRIALS INVOLVING DEAF JURORS WHO SERVE WITH THE ASSISTANCE OF AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE INTERPRETERS

5 Quick Tips for Improving Your Emotional Intelligence. and Increasing Your Success in All Areas of Your Life

THE INTEGRITY PROFILING SYSTEM

- Conduct effective follow up visits when missing children return home ensuring intelligence is shared with appropriate partners.

EYEWITNESS IDENTIFICATION. Mary F. Moriarty SPD Annual Conference 2015

PRACTICAL TIPS FOR JURY TRIALS IN PATENT CASES. Scott K. Reed and Ralph A. Dengler FITZPATRICK, CELLA, HARPER & SCINTO

BACKGROUND + GENERAL COMMENTS

BASIC VOLUME. Elements of Drug Dependence Treatment

Lieutenant Jonathyn W Priest

Confidence in Sampling: Why Every Lawyer Needs to Know the Number 384. By John G. McCabe, M.A. and Justin C. Mary

Black Butterfly: A Statement on Counseling Minority Youth. Kimberly McLeod. Texas Southern University

Combating Plaintiff Reptilian Tactics in Complex Litigation: Discovery, Voir Dire, Direct and Cross-Exam

New York Law Journal. Friday, May 9, Trial Advocacy, Cross-Examination of Medical Doctors: Recurrent Themes

WEB FORM K COUNSELOR ACTIVITY SELF-EFFICACY SCALES

INTRODUCTION. HCA Health Coaching Guide for Health Practitioners 1. Introduction

Deanna Sullivan. Perception is Reality Working to Enhance the Image

FAQ: Heuristics, Biases, and Alternatives

Character Word of the Month

54 Emotional Intelligence Competencies

Managing Your Emotions

Problem Situation Form for Parents

Mastering Emotions. 1. Physiology

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation

Finding common ground with people who have diabetes

Biol/Chem 4900/4912. Forensic Internship Lecture 2

MAJ TRIAL SKILLS SEMINAR: PLAINTIFF S CLOSING ARGUMENT

Recording Transcript Wendy Down Shift #9 Practice Time August 2018

Emotional Intelligence

MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS

VOLUME B. Elements of Psychological Treatment

RISK COMMUNICATION FLASH CARDS. Quiz your knowledge and learn the basics.

Concluding Remarks. Mental illness is ubiquitous. Mental illnesses exist at the extremes of normal behavior

PERFECTIONISM Inwardly-Focused Outwardly-Focused

Lee's Martial Arts. The Five Principles. Principle #1: Preventive Defense. Principle #2: Awareness

Trial Preparation for Vocational Experts: A Defense Perspective

WASHINGTON SERVICE CORPS SERVES Institute

Assessing Readiness To Change

We admitted that we were powerless over alcohol that our lives had become unmanageable. Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) (2001, p. 59)

5. is the process of moving from the specific to the general. a. Deduction

ACT II An ACT Skills Building Workshop

How to empower your child against underage drinking

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Behaviorism: An essential survival tool for practitioners in autism

11. A Thumbnail Sketch of the Myers-Briggs Type Inventory (MBTI) Page 1 of 24

PM-SB Study MI Webinar Series Engaging Using Motivational Interviewing (MI): A Practical Approach. Franze de la Calle Antoinette Schoenthaler

Leadership Beyond Reason

PLAN FOR TODAY. What is Emotional Intelligence/EQ? Why it Matters An Overview of the EQ Model Lots of ideas for improving your EQ

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Step 2 Challenging negative thoughts "Weeding"

Ingredients of Difficult Conversations

THE MENTAL PREP PLAYBOOK BEING MENTALLY PREPARED TO PLAY YOUR BEST GAME

Motivational Interviewing for Family Planning Providers. Motivational Interviewing. Disclosure

The Conference That Counts! March, 2018

TRIAL IS DIFFERENT Trial Practice Do s and Don ts. Joseph J. Popolizio Holly A. McGee April 20, 2018 Arizona Paralegal Association

Soul of leadership workshop. Patricia E. Molina, MD, PhD What I learned

Assessments in Private Practice

Ayoung mechanic has been

Self-confidence can increase or decrease according to the context (situation, time, people) we are in.

Meta Skills: MEDDSS/MEDS Effectively Wise Mind IPE DEAR MAN GIVE FAST Nonjudgmental Radical Acceptance. MEDDSS / MEDS Mastery Exercise Diet

ADD/ADHD: REAL or IMAGINED?

Subliminal Programming

Client Care Counseling Critique Assignment Osteoporosis

SENTENCING ADVOCACY WORKSHOP. Developing Theories and Themes. Ira Mickeberg, Public Defender Training and Consultant, Saratoga Springs, NY

Emotional-Social Intelligence Index

54 Emotional Intelligence Competencies

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Why Coaching Clients Give Up

We admitted that we were powerless over alcohol that our lives had become unmanageable.

Psychotherapists and Counsellors Professional Liaison Group (PLG) 15 December 2010

Use of Supporting Evidence With the IME Physician at Trial

BEING A LEADER and LEADERSHIP

The Mental Capacity Act 2006 and the management of challenging behaviours: Applications to the Northern Ireland Capacity Bill

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance

Motivational Interviewing

Participant Manual DRE 7-Day Session 28 Case Preparation and Testimony

REPORT ON EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE: GENERAL

Difficult Conversations

CPRP PRACTICE DOMAIN I: Interpersonal Competencies. Module 4

What Constitutes a Good Contribution to the Literature (Body of Knowledge)?

Transcription:

Assessing Sympathy in Voir Dire: Exploring Jurors Intention-Behavior Gap Bill Kanasky, Ph.D.

CERVANTES (1547-1616) SAID LONG AGO that It s a long step from saying to doing. He was describing what is referred to today as the intention-behavior gap: knowing what to do, but struggling to actually do it. In today s courtrooms, the intention-behavior gap is most apparent during the jury selection and jury deliberation phases of trial. During jury selection, the overwhelming majority of jurors say that they will put sympathy aside during the trial, then proceed to award high money damages to the plaintiff during deliberations. In post-trial interviews, these jurors commonly admit that sympathy drove their decision-making, despite their earlier assurance that they would put sympathy aside. In reality, jurors who express strong intentions to follow the law often fail to act on them during deliberations because the emotional aspects of the case are overpowering. This scenario is every defense attorney s nightmare, when even the most thorough voir dire efforts are not enough to prevent sympathy from trumping the law. The incredible social pressure placed on jurors during these questions almost always results in a socially desirable response Defense attorneys typically use two types of questions during the voir dire process to ascertain whether jurors will be able to put their sympathy aside when making decisions about the case. First, jurors are usually individually asked if they are willing to make decisions about the case while concurrently eliminating sympathy from the equation. Most jurors 2 instinctively respond yes with the judge, attorneys, parties and other jurors staring at them. However, some defense attorneys realize that such basic questions regarding sympathy are insufficient, and therefore ask jurors to go a step further. Specifically, some defense attorneys ask the juror to agree to a verbal contract to promise to not let sympathy impact their decisions during deliberations. Again, with the entire courtroom watching, the vast majority of jurors instinctually agree to the verbal contract. Asking jurors if they can put sympathy aside or asking them to keep a promise to do so are deeply flawed voir dire strategies. First, the incredible social pressure placed on jurors during these questions almost always results in a socially desirable response rather than a truthful response. Social desirability bias is very powerful and refers to the strong tendency of people to present themselves in the best possible light on sensitive topics. 1 Secondly, such voir dire questions actually result in measurement of the wrong psychological variable. These questions ultimately measure jurors intention to follow the law, rather than measuring their likelihood of following the law. Intentions are notoriously weak predictors of human behavior outside of purely volitional decisions that lack emotion. Years of psychology research has shown that the correlation between intentions and behavior is modest at best. Metaanalyses have revealed that intentions only account for approximately 30% of the variance in social behavior. 2,3 These findings suggest that defense attorneys need to go well beyond assessment of a juror s intentions to determine whether or not a juror is capable of following the law with regard to sympathy. Since sympathy is such a powerful factor in jury decision making, defense attorneys need a

more sophisticated procedure to assess jurors in jury selection. Self-Efficacy To accurately assess a juror s likelihood of abiding by the law regarding sympathy, defense attorneys should assess a juror s confidence in their ability to complete the actual task rather than merely their intention to do so. Self-efficacy is the belief in one s abilities to execute a course of action in a given situation. 4 More specifically, self-efficacy measures individual perceptions of how easy or difficult performing a task is likely to be and how much confidence they have in their ability to perform that task. People who have a stubborn belief in their capabilities will persevere in their efforts despite difficulties and obstacles that may surface. People with high levels of self-efficacy are not easily dissuaded by adversity. As such, the stronger the sense of personal efficacy, the greater the perseverance and the higher the likelihood that the chosen task will be performed successfully. 5 Therefore, it is imperative that defense attorneys learn how to accurately assess jurors self-efficacy ratings related to their ability to perform the specific task of putting sympathy aside in their decisionmaking on liability and/or damages questions Jurors instinctively respond yes with the judge, attorneys, parties and other jurors staring at them Measuring Juror Self-Efficacy Quantitative Data. In the standard methodology for measuring self-efficacy beliefs, people rate the strength of their belief in their ability to execute the required activity on a 10-point interval scale, ranging from 0 ( Cannot do ); through 10 ( Highly certain can do ). 5 The interval scale has equal units of measurement, thus making it possible to interpret not only the order of scale scores but also the distance between them. It is possible to add or subtract the scale values without affecting the form of the scale, but one cannot multiply or divide the values. For example, it can be said that two jurors with scale positions 1 and 2 are as far apart as two jurors with scale positions 4 and 5. However, it does not mean that a juror with score 10 feels twice as strongly as one with score 5. Importantly, this approach deflates social desirability bias as jurors are given a flexible response scale rather than being forced into an inflexible, all in type of response in which the influence of social desirability is very powerful. Qualitative Data. While quantitative data is very useful, the ability to acquire meaningful qualitative (i.e., descriptive) data from jurors is essential for cause challenges. Measuring self-efficacy on a scale as a continuous variable allows defense counsel to follow up with critical why questions that will extract the reasoning behind jurors beliefs in their ability to put sympathy aside. The goal of jury selection should not be persuading jurors to put sympathy aside, as that is an impractical task. Rather, the defense attorney s overall goal is to build 3

a case for a successful cause challenge against a highly sympathetic juror. While having a juror openly admit that they are unable to put sympathy aside is an obvious strong case for a cause challenge, the odds of such an occurrence is very low due to social desirability bias. By allowing jurors to rate their self-efficacy regarding sympathy and then provide specific reasons to justify such a rating gives defense counsel multiple ways to develop a persuasive cause challenge. Question Construction. Self-efficacy is concerned with perceived capability rather than intention. Therefore, voir dire questions related to sympathy should be phrased in terms of can do rather than will do. Can provides a judgment of capability, while will is a statement of intention. 5 Additionally, behavior is better predicted by people s beliefs in their capabilities to do what is specifically needed to succeed than by their beliefs in their ability to accomplish a general task. For example, asking jurors can you put sympathy aside in this matter is a general question that lacks specificity. However, asking Ms. Jones, this is obviously a very sad case; I need to be sure that you can put any sadness for Mr. Smith aside when you decide whether or not my client is liable in this case; please rate your ability to do just that on a 0-10 scale, with 0 meaning completely incapable and 10 meaning entirely capable defines a much more specific task that will result in more accurate data collection. Sole reliance on a juror s self-efficacy rating of a general task is not as effective as a rating relating to the specific actions that will need to be taken. Case Example: Birth Injury Case Defense Counsel Introduction to Sympathy Topic. As you know this is a very sad case involving a child that was injured during the birth process and an emotional response from you is completely normal and expected. Please understand: feeling sympathy for the plaintiffs in this case is NOT wrong or bad in any way, and if you do feel sympathy, you are not going to be in trouble or disappoint the court in any way - it is understandable. But, feeling sympathy may mean that this may not be the appropriate case for you to serve as a juror - and that a different case, with different circumstances, may be a better fit for you. Defense Counsel to Juror #1. Juror #1, like I just mentioned, this case involves an injured child. I need your honest thoughts about your confidence to not only reduce sympathy, but eliminate sympathy from the equation as you listen to both sides and then deliberate on this case. Please rate your ability to eliminate sympathy on a 0 to 10 scale, with zero meaning that you absolutely could not, and 10 meaning you absolutely can. Again, I am just looking for your honest feedback here; there is no right or wrong rating. Scoring and Defense Counsel Follow-up. <5 = I understand; and why do you feel that way? Record the response and move for an immediate cause challenge. 4

6-7 = Why are you a 6/7, and not a 9/10? These jurors have reservations and uncertainty about their ability to follow the judge s instructions regarding sympathy, but they fear showing it. Keep asking why questions to illuminate their concerns, then move for a cause challenge when you have enough information from the juror. In other words, keep digging to develop a solid case for a cause challenge. 8-9 = Why are you not a 10? These jurors tend to be level-headed and realistic about their ability to put sympathy aside. Defense counsel should still ask the follow up question as to why they are not a 10, in an effort to identify a belief or attitude that may be grounds for a cause challenge. However, many of these jurors benignly respond with I can t say 10 because I am not perfect; no one is perfect; I promise to do my best which is a genuine, realistic response. 10 = OK, why a 10? What makes you so absolutely sure you could remove sympathy from your decision making in this matter? These jurors are dangerous as they insist they can be perfect in a situation which perfection is clearly impossible. When questioned, these jurors tend to state that following the law and judge s instructions is the right thing to do and that they could never do otherwise, giving defense counsel a false sense of security. Jurors who respond with a 10 are often narcissists who want to be on the jury, and during deliberations will enforce their idealistic, perfectionist attitudes upon other jurors to get their way. These 10s should be strong candidates for priority preemptory challenges. Timing of self-efficacy questions is critical Timing. Timing of self-efficacy questions is critical, as jurors must first be exposed to key plaintiff case issues (i.e., sympathy) prior to being asked if they can follow the law. During judge-conducted voir dire, many judges mistakenly ask questions about sympathy far too early in the process, resulting in jurors providing premature assessments of their abilities to follow the law. Self-efficacy questions regarding sympathy need to be placed at the very end of the voir dire script, as the sympathetic, emotional information about the case needs time to weigh on jurors minds and hearts before they provide any self-assessment data. This premise begs the question of what plaintiff evidence defense counsel should try to expose to jurors during the jury selection process. To get accurate self-efficacy assessment, it is vital that defense counsel insist that the plaintiff is present during jury selection, or at the very least insist that a clip of the plaintiff s day in the life video be played to jurors instead. Sympathy is a complex emotion that takes time to surface, and therefore defense counsel should wait until the very end of voir dire to explore it. 5

Conclusion The impact of juror sympathy on verdict and damage awards should not be underestimated. This is particularly true when the defense has a strong causation defense based on complicated scientific relationships. Even when the science is compelling, sympathetic jurors are infamous for making the leap from negligence to causation purely out of compassion for the plaintiff. Fortunately, solving the intention-behavior gap problem is indeed possible by using more scientifically-based voir dire questions designed to tap juror self-efficacy rather than mere intentions. Defense attorneys who are serious about preventing juror sympathy from derailing their case will need to significantly adjust their voir dire strategy in order to adequately assess juror self-efficacy. Otherwise, juries will continue to unanimously indicate that they will follow judge s instructions on sympathy, yet will subsequently go on to award an undeserving plaintiff millions in the end. About the Author Bill Kanasky Jr., Ph.D. is the Vice President of Litigation Psychology at Courtroom Sciences, Inc., a full-service, national litigation consulting firm. He is recognized as a national expert, author and speaker in the areas of witness preparation and jury psychology. Dr. Kanasky specializes in a full range of jury research services, including the design and implementation of mock trials and focus groups, venue attitude research, and post trial interviewing. Dr. Kanasky s success with training witnesses for deposition and trial testimony is remarkable. His systematic witness training methodology is efficient and effective, as it is designed to meet each witness s unique needs, while concurrently teaching core principles of persuasive communication. Clients benefit from Dr. Kanasky s ability to transform poor or average witnesses into extraordinary communicators. He can be reached at 407.883.2325 or bkanasky@courtroomsciences.com. 6

References 1. Fisher, R. J. (1993). Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 303-315. 2. Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-499. 3. Hagger, M., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior: Chatzisarantis, Hagger / MINDFULNESS AND BEHAVIOR 675; Predictive validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 3-32. 4. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. 5. Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing selfefficacy scales. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Selfefficacy beliefs of adolescents (Vol. 5, pp. 307-337). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. 7

Copyright 2014 Litigation Psychology, LLC. All Rights Reserved.