Sex-specific relationship between digit ratio (2D:4D) and romantic jealousy. The Netherlands

Similar documents
Research Proposal 2D:4D and Perceived Aggression: a Human Subjects Study Maya Frost-Belansky. Introduction

Sexual Dimorphism and Relationship between Chest, Hip and Waist Circumference with 2D, 4D and 2D:4D in Nigerians

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Received January 19, 2004; revision received March 23, 2004; accepted April 8, 2004

Sex differences in multiple dimensions of jealousy

PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE

It is advisable to refer to the publisher s version if you intend to cite from the work.

Running head: MATE RETENTION STRATEGIES p. 1. Solving the Problem of Partner Infidelity:

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

RELATIVE DIGIT LENGTHS PREDICT MEN S BEHAVIOR AND ATTRACTIVENESS DURING SOCIAL INTERACTIONS WITH WOMEN

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective

Running Head: INSULTS AND MATE RETENTION

Psychology 305A: Lecture 7. Wrap up Biological Approach Begin Cognitive Perspective

Upset in Response to a Sibling s Partner s Infidelities. Richard L. Michalski. Hollins University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

It s Not All about Her: Men s Mate Value and Mate Retention. Emily J. Miner. Florida Atlantic University. Valerie G. Starratt

Class Update Today: Evolutionary approach Part 2 Friday: Exam 4

Intersexual Competition

The disengagement of attentive resources from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional. infidelity

Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity

Jealousy: Unconscious processes

Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford

Morbid Jealousy from an Evolutionary Psychological Perspective JUDITH A. EASTON. University of Texas at Austin LUCAS D. SCHIPPER TODD K.

Second to fourth digit ratio in relation to age, BMI and life history in a population of young adults: a set of unexpected results

[In press, Personality and Individual Differences, February 2008] Not all Men are Sexually Coercive:

Gender and Sexuality. Views of Gender. Gender and Sex. Evolutionary Social Cognitive. Gender social dimension of being female or male

Sex Differences in Jealousy: A Study from Norway

Distress about mating rivals

Do Women Pretend Orgasm to Retain a Mate?

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing?

The Circles of Sexuality

Selective Breeding for a Behavioral Trait Changes Digit Ratio

Predicting violence against women from men s mate-retention behaviors

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

Emotional and Behavioral Reactions to Emotional and Physical Infidelity: An Evolutionary Perspective

Suschinsky, Kelly D. University of Lethbridge Research Repository Evolutionary Psychology

The Role of Stimulus Specificity on Infidelity Reactions: Seeing is Disturbing

Although prenatal exposure to testosterone probably occurs, there are only limited data demonstrating the transfer of testos-

A night on the town: when the importance of mate acquisition overrides intrasexual competition

12 The biology of love

Mate Value of Romantic Partners Predicts Men s Partner-Directed Verbal Insults. Emily J. Miner and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

lse.bol.ucla.edu/publications/pillsworthhaselton_06.pdf

Research Report Beyond Touching: Evolutionary Theory and Computer-Mediated Infidelity

Which Infidelity Type Makes You More Jealous? Decision Strategies in a Forced-choice Between Sexual and Emotional Infidelity

Short-Term Sexual Strategies

Are your feelings just another evolutionary adaptation? A discussion of the field of evolutionary psychology

Women s Fertility Status Alters Other Women s Jealousy and Mate Guarding

Evolutionary Psychology (Psych 459) Midterm Exam Winter 2010

Visit ABLE on the Web at:

Cognitive Biases and Emotional Wisdom in the Evolution of Conflict Between the Sexes

Behavior Genetics and Evolutionary Psychology. Module 5

Decision strategies in continuous ratings of jealousy feelings elicited by sexual and emotional infidelity

Sensation seeking and men's face preferences

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

The Influence of Women s Self-Esteem on Mating Decision Making

Looking up versus looking down: attractiveness-based organizational biases are moderated by social comparison direction

Directional asymmetry (right left differences) in digit ratio (2D:4D) predict indirect aggression in women

Functionality, Parsimony, Discovery, Avoiding Hamartia: How Evolutionary Perspectives are Changing Psychology

warwick.ac.uk/lib-publications

To Poach or Not to Poach? Men are more Willing to Short-term Poach Mated Women who are more Attractive than their Mates

Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2002, 16 (2), BRIEF REPORT. Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner s infidelity

FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH JEALOUSY OVER REAL AND IMAGINED INFIDELITY: AN EXAMINATION OF THE SOCIAL-COGNITIVE AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY PERSPECTIVES

Judith A. Easton CURRICULUM VITA October 2011

Psych 305A: Lecture 19. Evolutionary Approach Wrap Up. The Cognitive Approach Part I Learning and Behaviorism

The impact of visual exposure to a physically attractive other on self-presentation

Advanced Code of Influence. Book 10

Chapter 15: Mixed design ANOVA

UNCORRECTED PROOF ARTICLE IN PRESS

Nature vs. Nurture Debate

Mate retention, semen displacement, and sperm competition in humans

Sex! Woo Hoo! Finally something interesting!

Gender. 7 Gender, Gender Identity, and Transgender

mating; jealousy; conflict; mate guarding

The Mere Presence of Opposite-Sex Others on Judgments of Sexual and Romantic Desirability: Opposite Effects for Men and Women

Nature and Nurture. What makes us who we are?

Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application

Love and Sex Lecture 24

CHAPTER 11: GENDER AND SEXUALITY

The Relationship between Objective Sperm Competition Risk and Men s Copulatory Interest is. Moderated by Partner s Time Spent with Other Men

2D:4D Digit Ratio: Indicator of Sports and Gaming Participation in Males. Nick Lehan & Kayla Smith. Introduction

Women s mating preferences. What does a woman really want?

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men

I Can Dance: Further Investigations of the Effect of Dancing Ability on Mate Value

Friendship As A Relationship Infiltration Tactic During Human Mate Poaching: An Experimental Investigation

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR)

Facial symmetry and the Ôbig-fiveÕ personality factors

BIRTH CONTROL, KISSING, AND ASSESSMENT OF MATE QULAITY 1. Running head: BIRTH CONTROL, KISSING, AND ASSESSMENT OF MATE

Motivation and Emotion deals with the drives and incentives behind everyday thoughts and actions.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

When facial attractiveness is only skin deep

LOW DIGITAL FINGER RATIO: AN INDICATOR OF MENTAL TOUGHNESS AND PRENATAL PROGRAMMING OF SPORTING SUCCESS

Human mate poaching can be defined as a special

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY. Effective Communication and Dealing with Athletes with Low Self-Esteem

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Female Mate Choice is Influenced by Male Sport Participation

Evolutionary Psychology

Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, Michigan 3 Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, Michigan

Genes: Our Biological Blueprint

Sexual and Emotional Infidelity: Evolved Gender Differences in Jealousy Prove Robust and Replicable

Transcription:

* Title page with author details 1 Running Head: 2D:4D AND JEALOUSY Sex-specific relationship between digit ratio (2D:4D) and romantic jealousy Justin H. Park a,*, Martijn B. Wieling a, Abraham P. Buunk a,b, Karlijn Massar a a Department of Psychology, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, The Netherlands b Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, The Netherlands *Correspondence: Justin H. Park Department of Psychology University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1 9712 TS Groningen The Netherlands Phone: +31 50-363-7151 Fax: +31 50-363-4581 E-mail: j.h.park@rug.nl

* Manuscript without author identities Sex-specific relationship 1 Abstract The ratio of index finger length to ring finger length (2D:4D) is an index of prenatal androgen exposure. In a study with 71 female and 52 male undergraduate students, we assessed the relationship between 2D:4D and jealousy with respect to various dimensions of rival characteristics. Following the presentation of a jealousy-evoking scenario, participants rated the extent to which they would feel jealous if the rival possessed various characteristics (some which have been found to be more jealousy evoking for men, others which have been found to be more jealousy evoking for women). Men with higher, more feminine 2D:4D reacted more jealously toward socially dominant rivals. Women with lower, more masculine 2D:4D reacted more jealously toward physically attractive rivals. These results show that the level of prenatal testosterone affects which rival characteristics elicit the highest level of jealousy, and differently for men and women. Keywords: androgens; 2D:4D; digit ratio; jealousy; sex differences; sex hormones; testosterone

Sex-specific relationship 2 1. Introduction The second-to-fourth-digit ratio (2D:4D) is lower among men than women, which is presumed to be the result of differences in prenatal testosterone exposure (Manning, 2002). Strong evidence for the link between higher embryonic exposure to androgens and lower 2D:4D comes from a recent study of dizygotic twins showing that 2D:4D is more masculinized among females with a male twin than among females with a female twin (van Anders, Vernon, & Wilbur, 2006). There is also experimental evidence that injecting testosterone into eggs leads to changes in digit ratio in the ring-necked pheasant (Romano, Rubolini, Martinelli, Alquati, & Saino, 2005). Within each sex, 2D:4D has been found to be associated with a variety of physical and psychological characteristics. For example, men with lower 2D:4D are more aggressive, more athletic, less feminine (on the Bem Sex Role Inventory), and more musically talented (Bailey & Hurd, 2005; Manning & Taylor, 2001; Rammsayer & Troche, 2007; Sluming & Manning, 2000). Women with lower 2D:4D have higher waist-to-hip ratio, are more masculine (on the Bem Sex Role Inventory), and are more athletic (Csathó et al., 2003; Manning et al., 2000; Pokrywka, Rachon, Suchecka-Rachon, & Bitel, 2005). Among both men and women, 2D:4D is correlated positively with verbal intelligence and agreeableness, and negatively with numerical intelligence and physical fitness (Hönekopp, Voracek, & Manning, 2006; Luxen & Buunk, 2005). The present research examined the relationship between 2D:4D and romantic jealousy. Jealousy is thought to be an evolved psychological response with distinct differences between men and women (Buss, 2000). Feelings of jealousy are evoked partly through a process of social comparison in which jealous individuals compare their own characteristics with those of the rival when the rival is believed to surpass them on these characteristics, the rival is likely to be perceived as a threat to the relationship and therefore evoke feelings of jealousy (Buunk, Dijkstra, & Massar, 2007; DeSteno & Salovey, 1996). Because men and women value different

Sex-specific relationship 3 characteristics in a mate, they may pay attention to different characteristics in a rival. Crosscultural studies on mate preferences have shown that women (more than men) prefer mates with high social status and dominance, which is presumably an evolved preference for men who can provide them and their children with resources. In contrast, men (more than women) prefer mates who are high in physical attractiveness, which may serve as an important cue to a woman s health and fertility (Buss, 1994). Paralleling sex differences in mate preferences, males respond more jealously to a rival with status- and dominance-related characteristics, whereas females respond more jealously to a rival who is physically attractive (e.g., Buss, Shackelford, Choe, Buunk, & Dijkstra, 2000; Dijkstra & Buunk, 2002). Because women desire status- and dominance-related (typically masculine ) features in a mate, these features observed in a rival are likely to evoke a particularly high level of jealousy among less masculine men. Likewise, because men desire physically attractive (typically feminine ) features in a mate, these features observed in a rival are likely to evoke a particularly high level of jealousy among less feminine women. Assessment of 2D:4D offers an unobtrusive means of examining the relationship between masculinity/femininity and jealousy, as individuals are not likely to be aware of their own digit ratio. The present study tested the following hypotheses: (1) The higher (i.e., less masculine) their 2D:4D, the more jealously men will respond to a rival with masculine features such as those related with social status, physical dominance, and social dominance; and (2) the lower (i.e., less feminine) their 2D:4D, the more jealously women will respond to a rival with feminine features such as those related with physical attractiveness. 2. Method 2.1. Participants

Sex-specific relationship 4 Seventy-one female (mean age = 21.62, SD = 2.13) and 52 male (mean age = 21.98, SD = 2.28) heterosexual students at the University of Groningen participated in exchange for 5. 2.2. Procedure Participants were presented with a jealousy-evoking scenario that has been used in a series of previous studies (e.g., Buunk & Dijkstra, 2001; Dijkstra & Buunk, 1998, 2002; see Appendix) and were asked to indicate their level of jealousy if the rival possessed one of several characteristics. For each characteristic, participants provided their response on a 5-point scale (1 = not jealous at all, 5 = very jealous). Following Dijkstra and Buunk (2002), mean jealousy scores were calculated for five dimensions of rival characteristics: social dominance (17 items; Cronbach s e.g., is more charismatic, is more self-confidence, has more ascendance, is a better talker, is more popular), physical attractiveness e.g., is more slender, has a better figure, has a more attractive body), seductive behaviors (7 items; e.g., dresses more revealingly, behaves more provocatively, is more of a seducer), physical dominance (8 items; is more muscular, is more athletic, has a heavier build), and social status (4 items; ter job, has more money, has a better education). A Hewlett Packard 4670 VP see-through scanner (resolution 300 DPI) was used to acquire digital images of both hands. The digits were then measured with the measurement tool in Adobe Photoshop CS to the nearest 0.1 mm. 2D:4D was calculated by dividing the length of the second digit by the length of the fourth digit. To assess the reliability of the measurements, 24 pairs of left and right hands were measured twice. Finger measurements were highly reliable across the two measurements (all rs[22] >.99, all ps <.01). 3. Results

Sex-specific relationship 5 Right and left 2D:4D were highly correlated, (r[121] =.72, p <.01). Consistent with previous reports, the mean male 2D:4D was lower than the mean female 2D:4D (t[121] = 2.49, p =.014, d =.46; M male =.96, SD male =.028, M female =.98, SD female =.028). Table 1 reports mean levels of jealousy in response to the five dimensions of rival characteristics, as well as tests of sex differences. Compared with women, men tended to be more jealous of physically dominant rivals; compared with men, women tended to be more jealous of physically attractive rivals, results which are fairly consistent with previous reports (e.g., Dijkstra & Buunk, 2002). ------------------------------ Insert Table 1 here ------------------------------ Table 2 reports the correlations between 2D:4D and jealousy in response to each of the five rival characteristic dimensions. Men with higher 2D:4D were especially jealous of socially dominant rivals (left hand: r[50] =.28, p <.05; right hand: r[50] =.35, p <.05), and women with lower 2D:4D were especially jealous of physically attractive rivals (left hand: r[69] =.25, p <.05; right hand: r[69] = -.32, p <.01). Unexpectedly, women with lower right-hand 2D:4D were more jealous of physically dominant rivals (r[69] = -.24, p <.05). ------------------------------ Insert Table 2 here ------------------------------ It is somewhat surprising that 2D:4D did not predict jealousy in response to a physically dominant rival, especially since jealousy ratings toward socially dominant and physically dominant rivals were highly correlated (r[50] =.59, p <.01). 4. Discussion This study examined sex differences in the relationship between 2D:4D and jealousy in response to different rival characteristics. In partial agreement with the first hypothesis, men with

Sex-specific relationship 6 higher, more feminine 2D:4D were more jealous of socially dominant rivals; in line with the second hypothesis, women with lower, more masculine 2D:4D were more jealous of physically attractive rivals. These effects were observed for both hands. We also found that women with lower right-hand 2D:4D were more jealous of physically dominant rivals. Although we have no clear explanation for this latter finding, the trend toward negative correlations between 2D:4D and jealousy among women across all rival characteristics suggests that the effect might not be exclusive to physical attractiveness. Less easy to explain is the patterns of results among men. Rivals high in social status and physical dominance did not evoke more jealousy among men with higher 2D:4D. The finding that jealousy in response to the social status of the rival was not related to 2D:4D may be explained by the fact that high social status (e.g., more education, more money) might be achieved by men regardless of prenatal androgen exposure. It is more surprising that 2D:4D did not predict jealousy for physically dominant rivals, as prenatal testosterone exposure is expected to exert direct effects on body size and muscular development. One speculation is that although low 2D:4D is associated with physical fitness (e.g., Hönekopff, Manning & Müller, 2006), it is also associated with features such as musical ability, numerical intelligence, and cooperative behavior (e.g., Luxen & Buunk, 2005; Millet & DeWitte, 2006; Sluming & Manning, 2000) characteristics that may be more directly associated with social rather than physical dominance. Indeed, it has been noted that the adaptive consequences of male masculinity may manifest through socially mediated behaviors, rather than simply through physical characteristics (Gangestad & Thornhill, 2007). The results shown in Table 1 do show that men s jealousy was aroused most strongly by socially dominant rivals (and the least strongly by physically dominant rivals). Nevertheless, future research must examine more directly how 2D:4D is related to various types of dominance. Although there is evidence that men with lower 2D:4D are perceived to be

Sex-specific relationship 7 more dominant and masculine (Neave, Laing, Fink, & Manning, 2003), there is no unequivocal evidence that lower 2D:4D is associated with more socially dominant behaviors. Despite the limitations, our results suggest that the level of prenatal testosterone predicts in part which rival characteristics are especially likely to evoke jealousy among men and women. These findings corroborate previous research indicating that 2D:4D serves as a useful physical correlate of functional individual differences. The findings also indicate that jealousy is not an invariant response, but is flexibly engaged in a predictable manner, depending on one s sex and characteristics relevant to one s mate value. Future research should investigate which psychological variables such as sex role identity may mediate the effects of 2D:4D on jealousy and on other psychological phenomena within the realm of romantic and interpersonal relationships. It would also be useful to examine more complex effects involving 2D:4D. For instance, 2D:4D may moderate other sex differences in patterns of jealousy (e.g., the degree to which men and women are more upset by sexual or emotional infidelity of the partner). Or it may moderate the degree to which individuals engage in mate guarding tactics in specific social situations.

Sex-specific relationship 8 Acknowledgements This work was supported by a grant from the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. We thank Liga Klavina and Shelli Dubbs for their assistance.

Sex-specific relationship 9 References Bailey, A. A., & Hurd, P. L. (2005). Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women. Biological Psychology, 68, 215-222. Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York: Basic Books. Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as necessary as love and sex. New York: Free Press. Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Choe, J., Buunk, B. P., & Dijkstra, P. (2000). Distress about mating rivals. Personal Relationships, 7, 235-243. Buunk, B. P., & Dijkstra, P. (2001). Evidence from a homosexual sample for a sex-specific rivaloriented mechanism: Jealousy as a function of a rival s physical attractiveness and dominance. Personal Relationships, 8, 391-406. Buunk, A. P., Massar, K., & Dijkstra, P. (2007). A social cognitive evolutionary approach to jealousy: The automatic evaluation of one s romantic rivals. In J. P. Forgas, M. G. Haselton, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition (pp. 213-228). New York: Psychology Press Csathó, Á., Osváth, A., Bicsák, É., Karádi, K., Manning, J., & Kállai, J. (2003). Sex role identity related to the ratio of second to fourth digit length in women. Biological Psychology, 62, 147-156. DeSteno, D. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Jealousy and the characteristics of one s rival: A selfevaluation maintenance perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 920-932. Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (1998). Jealousy as a function of rival characteristics: An evolutionary perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1158-1166.

Sex-specific relationship 10 Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (2002). Sex differences in the jealousy-evoking effect of rival characteristics. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 829-852. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2007). The evolution of social inference processes: The importance of signaling theory. In J. P. Forgas, M. G. Haselton, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition (pp. 33-48). New York: Psychology Press. Hönekopp, J., Manning, J.T., & Müller, C. (2006). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and physical fitness in males and females: Evidence for effects of prenatal androgens on sexually selected traits. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 545-549. Hönekopp, J., Voracek, M., & Manning, J. T. (2006). 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and number of sex partners: Evidence for effects of prenatal testosterone in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 30-37. Luxen, M. F., & Buunk, B. P. (2005). Second-to-fourth digit ratio related to Verbal and Numerical Intelligence and the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 959-966. Manning, J. T. (2002). Digit ratio: A pointer to fertility, behavior and health. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Manning, J. T., Barley, L., Walton, J., Lewis-Jones, D. I., Trivers, R. L., Singh, D., et al. (2000). The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success. evidence for sexually antagonistic genes? Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 163-183. Manning, J. T., & Taylor, R. P. (2001). Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 61-69. Millet, K. & Dewitte, S. (2006). Second to fourth digit ratio and cooperative behavior. Biological

Sex-specific relationship 11 Psychology, 71, 111-115. Neave, N., Laing, S., Fink, B., & Manning, J. T. (2003). Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270, 2167-2172. Pokrywka, L., Rachon, D., Suchecka-Rachon, K., & Bitel, L. (2005). The second to fourth digit ratio in elite and non-elite female athletes. American Journal of Human Biology, 17, 796-800. Rammsayer, T. H., & Troche, S. J. (2007). Sexual dimorphism in second-to-fourth digit ratio and its relation to gender-role orientation in males and females. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 911-920. Romano, M., Rubolini, D., Martinelli, R., Alquati, A. B., & Saino, N. (2005). Experimental manipulation of yolk testosterone affects digit length ratios in the ring-necked pheasant (Phasianus colchicus). Hormones and Behavior, 48, 342-346. Sluming, V. A., & Manning, J. T. (2000). Second to fourth digit ratio in elite musicians: Evidence for musical ability as an honest signal of male fitness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 1-9. van Anders, S. M., Vernon, P. A., & Wilbur, C. J. (2006). Finger-length ratios show evidence of prenatal hormone-transfer between opposite sex twins. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 315-319.

Sex-specific relationship 12 Table 1 Jealousy in Response to Distinct Rival Characteristics Among Men and Women, and Tests of Sex Differences Dimension of Rival Characteristics Men (n = 52) Women (n = 71) t p d Social Dominance 2.97 (.87) 3.22 (.77) -1.64.10.30 Physical Attractiveness 1.93 (.56) 2.95 (1.06) -6.86.01 1.19 Seductive Behavior 1.87 (.73) 2.11 (.83) -1.68.10.31 Physical Dominance 1.80 (.76) 1.46 (.47) 2.85.01.54 Social Status 1.99 (.88) 1.96 (.75).19.85.03 Note. Values in parentheses are SDs.

Sex-specific relationship 13 Table 2 Correlations Between Left- and Right-Hand 2D:4D and Jealousy With Respect to Different Dimensions of Rival Characteristics Among Men and Women, and tests of Sex Differences in the Correlations (Fisher s z test) 2D:4D Sex Differences Men (n = 52) Women (n = 71) z z Left Right Left Right Left Right Social Dominance.28*.35* -.08 -.14 1.93 2.66* Physical Attractiveness.12.19 -.25* -.32* 1.98* 2.74* Seductive Behavior.05.13 -.06 -.11.59 1.28 Physical Dominance.08.19 -.15 -.24* 1.23 2.29* Social Status -.04 -.07 -.17 -.16.68.51 Note. p <.10, * p <.05 (two-tailed).

Sex-specific relationship 14 Appendix Scenario used to assess jealousy-evoking nature of rival characteristics You are at a party with your girlfriend/boyfriend and you are talking with some of your friends. You notice your girlfriend/boyfriend across the room talking to a man/woman you do not know. You can see from his/her face that he/she is very interested in your girlfriend/boyfriend. He/She is listening closely to what she/he is saying and you notice that he/she casually touches her/his hand. You notice that he/she is flirting with her/him. After a minute, your girlfriend/boyfriend also begins to act flirtatiously. You can tell from the way she/he is looking at him/her that she/he likes him/her a great deal. They seem completely absorbed in each other. Participants were then asked, When my partner and the man/woman flirt with each other, I would feel particularly jealous if that other man/woman is, which was followed by 1 of 56 characteristics (e.g., is more charismatic, has a better figure, has more money ). Each characteristic was rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not jealous at all, 5 = very jealous).