EFFECT OF ILLUMINATION CONDITION ON RISK ASSESSMENT BEHAVIORS OF MICE

Similar documents
"One-trial tolerance" to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state?

Supplementary Methods

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Rat Pups Reduce Ultrasonic Vocalization After Exposure to an Adult Male Rat

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen.

Conditioning and residual emotionality effects of predator stimuli: some reflections on stress and emotion

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Department of Psychology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48104

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Ethoexperimental Studies of Behaviour in Wild and Laboratory Mice Risk Assessment, Emotional Reactivity and Animal Welfare

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 10 A Synthetic Perspective on Instrumental Learning

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

RESPONSE INITIATION AND DIRECTIONALITY AS FACTORS INFLUENCING AVOIDANCE PERFORMANCE

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Predator Odor as an Unconditioned Fear Stimulus in Rats: Elicitation of Freezing by Trimethylthiazoline, a Component of Fox Feces

Genetic Analysis of Anxiety Related Behaviors by Gene Chip and In situ Hybridization of the Hippocampus and Amygdala of C57BL/6J and AJ Mice Brains

Effects of baseline anxiety on response to kindling of the right medial amygdala

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER. Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification Home

Stress and Emotionality: a Multidimensional and Genetic Approach

PSYO/NESC 3161 Measuring Behaviour

Do Rats Consume Ethanol to Regulate a Negative Emotion Induced by a Successive Negative Contrast Procedure?

What can mother hens teach us about chicken welfare?

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

What to watch for when analyzing mouse behavior

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Social influence on avoidance of dangerous stimuli by rats

ARTICLE IN PRESS. TMT-induced autonomic and behavioral changes and the neural basis of its processing

Synergistic Anti-Depression Activity Evaluation of Different Composition of Volatile Oil of Eucalyptus and Neem Oil by Tail Suspension Model

30 40 cm)that were illuminated from above by fluorescent vulnerable to attack on that side or unable to

: /18

Research With Animals in Psychology

Age at Puberty and First Litter Size in Early and Late Paired Rats 2

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

Limbic system outline

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

OBJECTIVES. To examine the influence of conditioned fear on pain threshold

The effect of controllable and uncontrollable neonatal preshocks on adult escape/avoidance behavior in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus)

The Effects of MÛGÛKA (Catha edulis vahl) on the Behaviour of Rats

Awake-behaving recordings in mice during fear conditioning. Eric H. Chang, Ph.D. Goldsmith Postdoctoral Fellow

Social Modulation of Pain as Evidence for Empathy in Mice. Introduction

Supporting Online Material for

Behavioral and neurochemical changes following predatory stress in mice

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 9 Biological Influences on Learning

TARGET NOVELTY ELICITS AND DIRECTS SHOCK-ASSOCIATED AGGRESSION IN WILD RATSI

The effects of caffeine on operant behavior in the Mongolian gerbil

Potentiation of locomotor activity by d-amphetamine in infant Swiss-Webster mice

A Method for Assessing Attentional Bias in Anxious Rats. A Senior Honors Thesis

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD

Effects of Systemic Administration of 8-OH-DPAT on Agonistic Social Behaviors in Male Syrian Hamsters

HIROSHI IMADA, KOZO SUGIOKA2, YUJI OHKI, HAJIME NINOHIRA AND AKIHIKO YAMAZAKI Department of Psychology, Kwansei Gakuin University

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

EFFECT OF CONVULVULUS PLURICAULIS CHOISY. ON LEARNING BEHAVIOUR AND MEMORY ENHANCEMENT ACTIVITY IN RODENTS

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

CO DITIO ED LEARNING I PREGNA CY

Thigmotactic responses in an open-field

A Study of the Predator-Prey Relationship

Standard Operating Procedure

Study of Agomelatine for the Reduction of Marble Buying Behavior in Brain Disorder

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

The relationship between anxiety and depression in animal models: a study using the forced swimming test and elevated plus-maze

Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions

Conditioned taste aversion produced by the oral ingestion of ethanol in the rat

YOUR CHAT MODERATOR. Leslie Sinn, DVM,CPDT-KA Behavior Resident in Private Practice Training

Table 1. Studies included in the review in temporal order. The table show the main characteristics: species, type of stimuli, tasks, manipulation

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

Effects of prenatal exposure to a mild chronic variable stress on body weight, preweaning mortality and rat behavior

The vocal expression of feeding motivation and frustration in the domestic laying hen, Gallus gallus domesticus

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

Prevention of Development of Tolerance and Dependence to Opiate in Mice by BR-16A (Mentat) A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Some studies on the influence of chlordiazepoxide on the social interaction of golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus)

Transfer of visual and haptic maze learning in rats

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Imitation and Affordance Learning by Pigeons (Columba livia)

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

HISTAMINE INHIBITS EATING WITHOUT ALTERING POSTPRANDIAL SATIETY IN RATS KELLY TSCHANTZ

auditory flutter fusion threshold (A.F.F.T.), is the auditory equivalent of the visual critical

Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals: The Past, Present, and Future

An Exploration of Animal Behavior In Isopods

ENVIORNMENTAL ENRICHMENT AFFECTS THE BEHAVIOR OF ADOLESCENT RATS IN A SOCIAL PREFERENCE TASK. By: Kelly L. Patterson

Transcription:

The Psychological Record, 1997,47, 167-174 EFFECT OF ILLUMINATION CONDITION ON RISK ASSESSMENT BEHAVIORS OF MICE ERNEST D. KEMBLE and MICHAEL J. GOBLIRSCH University of Minnesota-Morris Levels of risk assessment evoked by either a synthetic predator odor or noncontact exposure to live rats were examined under white or red light conditions. Defensive responses to the predator odor were infrequent in white light and showed a modest, but reliable, increase under red light. Noncontact exposure to rats, in contrast, produced considerably higher levels of risk assessment than did odors in white light, and these levels were nearly doubled under red light. Judicious selection of eliciting and background stimuli may provide considerable control over the intensity of risk assessment. Such procedures may prove useful for the study of anxiety-like states. Risk assessment is a popular tool for the study of anxiety-like states (e.g., Blanchard & Blanchard, 1988, 1990) but the variables controlling the intensity of these behaviors are not well characterized. Presentation of various odors, for example, regularly evokes modest elevations in the levels of these defensive responses (e.g., Blanchard et ai., 1990; Garbe, Kemble, & Rawleigh, 1994; Williams & Scott, 1989) which are sensitive to both anxiolytic and anxiogenic drug treatments (Blanchard et ai., 1990; Blanchard, Tauklis, Rodgers, Magee, & Blanchard, 1993; Garbe, Kemble, & Strunk, 1993; Kemble & Gordon, 1996; Zangrossi & File, 1992). Although this experimental manipulation is clearly useful, it should be noted that a reasonably wide range of both predator and nonpredator odors have roughly equivalent effects on risk assessment (Garbe et ai., 1994). Furthermore, repeated prior defeat experience (Kemble & Bolwahnn, in press) does not increase odor reactivity. If odorinduced risk assessment is generally insensitive to major experimental manipulations, then its usefulness for the study of defensiveness may be somewhat compromised. Alternatively, it is possible that some relevant stimulus conditions remain untested. Illumination condition appears to be a likely candidate. Not only is open field activity affected by illumination levels (e.g., Welker, 1959) but Lester and Faneslow (1992) find that light We thank Pherotech Inc. for donating the synthetic predator odorant which was used in Experiment 1. Reprint requests may be sent to Ernest Kemble, Division of Social Sciences, University of Minnesota-Morris, Morris, MN 56267.

168 KEMBLE AND GOBLIRSCH and dark conditions differ in their ease of association with electric shock. Because our previous experiments were invariably conducted under white light illumination, it seemed possible that this lighting condition suppressed risk assessment to some extent. If so, substitution of red for white light might elevate levels of defensiveness further. The following experiments explored this possibility. General Method Subjects The subjects were experimentally naive male CD-1 mice weighing 36.7-54.7 g. The subjects were individually housed in 29.5- x 18.5- x 12.0-cm clear polycarbonate cages having a wood shavings substrate. The mice were housed under a 12-hr light/dark cycle and tested during the light phase. All subjects received ad lib access to Purina Lab Chow and water throughout the experiment. The mice were randomly assigned to weight balanced groups designated for testing under white or red fluorescent illumination. Apparatus Testing was conducted in a 46.0- x 24.0- x 21.0-cm clear polycarbonate cage covered with a clear Plexiglas sheet. The cage was divided into 29.5- x 24.0- x 21.0-cm and 16.5- x 24.0- x 21.0-cm compartments by a 0.6 cm wire mesh screen. The fear-evoking stimuli were placed in the larger compartment and the mice in the smaller. Illumination was provided by 6 white or 6 red 34-W fluorescent tubes mounted 1.7 m above the testing compartment. The frequency and duration of risk assessment behaviors were scored with the aid of a computer data acquisition system. Procedure The mouse and fear-evoking stimulus were placed in their respective compartments under the designated illumination condition. The frequency and duration of four risk assessment behaviors (defensive burying, immobility, stretched attention, and flatback approach) as previously described by Blanchard et al. (1990) were then recorded for 10 min by an observer quietly seated 1.0-1.2 m from the test chambers. We found that levels of risk assessment recorded in this way were virtually identical (all ps >.10) to those obtained when behaviors were observed through a one-way vision screen. When possible, groups were compared by means of t tests. Some behaviors were absent among substantial numbers of subjects, however, and the groups were compared by Mann-Whitney U tests. Experiment 1 As mentioned previously, a variety of predator and nonpredator odors produce similar moderate elevations of risk assessment when testing is

ILLUMINATION CONDITION AND RISK ASSESSMENT 169 8 7 (.) 6-5 L. w. 4 (C 3 I: 2 1 p<.10 p<.oo5 (.) '" "-' 5 4.-.., (C 3 L. 2 (C I: 1 White Red LIGHT [ONDITION Figure 1. Mean frequency (upper panel) and duration (lower panel) of stretched attention under white or red light conditions. conducted under white light (Garbe et ai., 1994). In this experiment risk assessment was measured under either white or red fluorescent illumination in response to a synthetic predator odorant. This stimulus effectively deters feeding for extended periods in field settings (Sullivan, Crump, & Sullivan, 1988) and produces slight but significant elevations of risk assessment in white light (Kemble & Bolwahnn, in press).

170 KEMBLE AND GOBLIRSCH Method Eighteen mice were tested under either white (N 9) or red (N 9) light conditions. Synthetic predator odor was provided by a 5.0- x 0.3-cm slow release cylinder containing a 1: 1 mixture of 2-propylthietane (PT) and 1, 2 dithiolane (PDT). This pungent odorant reduces plant feeding by two species of meadow voles in the field (Sullivan et ai., 1988). The odor source was placed in a glass petri dish in the center of the larger compartment 30 min before testing. Results The mean frequency of stretched attention is depicted in the upper panel of Figure 1. Testing under red light significantly increased the frequency of this behavior, U(9, 9) 6, P <.005. Durations (lower panel) were also increased by red light but group differences attained only a marginal level of significance (p < 10). Although there was also a slight increase in the frequency (White, M 1.3; Red, M 2.2) and duration (White, M 0.7 sec; Red, M 1.6 sec) of defensive burying, these differences were not statistically reliable (ps >.10). The remaining risk assessment behaviors were quite infrequent in both groups with no suggestion of reliable group differences (ps>.10). Experiment 2 As Experiment 1 makes clear, testing under red light does indeed increase odor-induced risk assessment. This procedure might therefore be useful for experimental manipulations in which slightly higher baseline levels of defensiveness are desirable. As in previous experiments, however, levels of defensiveness to the odorant were rather low, even under red light, and the effects were restricted to a single risk assessment behavior. In contrast, exposure of rats to live cats evokes intense and protracted risk assessment (e.g., Adamec & Shallow, 1993; Blanchard & Blanchard, 1988). Although this procedure clearly produces a major elevation of defensiveness, the great intensity and duration of the behaviors may also reduce its usefulness for some experimental applications (e.g., some drug treatments). A procedure which induces more moderate effects would, therefore, seem desirable. Preliminary observations suggested that live rats in white light produce substantial, but short-term, elevations in risk assessment. This experiment therefore explored the possibility that rats produce higher levels of defensiveness than odors, and that such elevations are modulated by illumination condition. Method Subjects The subjects were 18 mice which were tested under white (N 9) or

ILLUMINATION CONDITION AND RISK ASSESSMENT 171 50 (.) 40 - L. 30 ra 20 10 p<.005 p<.oo 1 (.) 50 40.-.., 30 L. 20 10 Red LIGHT CONDITION White Figure 2. Mean (SEM) frequency (upper panel) and duration (lower panel) of stretched attention under white or red light conditions. red (N 9) light. Three male and 2 female hooded rats weighing 410-530 g served as fear-evoking stimuli. Apparatus and Procedures The apparatus was that used in Experiment 1. At the beginning of the test, one rat and one mouse were simultaneously placed in the

172 KEMBLE AND GOBLIRSCH apparatus. The wire mesh barrier prevented contact between mouse and rat. Each rat served as the fear-evoking stimulus for 3 or 4 mice. Otherwise, procedures were also identical. Results The frequency of stretched attention is depicted in the upper panel of Figure 2. It can be seen that rats evoked substantially higher levels of this behavior in white light than did the predator odor, and that testing under red light roughly doubled its frequency, t(16) 7.56, P <.001. Stretched attention durations showed a similar increase under red light, t(16) 3.75, P <.005. The frequency, but not duration (p >.10), of immobility (White, M 6.4; Red, M 11.2) was also increased by red light, 1(16) 2.16, P < 05. Although the frequency (White, M 11.8; Red, M 18.3) and duration (White, M 12.4 sec; Red, M 22.6 sec) of flatback approaches were somewhat elevated under red light, these differences were not statistically reliable (ps >.10). Both the frequency and duration of defensive burying were similar in the two groups with no suggestion of reliable group differences (ps >.10). Discussion Risk assessment, evoked by either odors or a live rat, was clearly potentiated when red light was substituted for white. Because the mice exposed to red light in these experiments were tested during the 12-hr light portion of the light/dark cycle, it is not clear whether the effects were due to red light per se or to the abrupt transition from white to red illumination. Testing during the normally dark portion of the light/dark cycle would be useful in addressing this issue. In any case, this manipulation does clearly elevate responsiveness and should increase the range of experimental applications which can be fruitfully investigated with this paradigm. Although it is conceivable that a higher concentration of the predator odor might produce,similar effects, we (Garbe et ai., 1994) found that four novel odorants, differing considerably in pungency, produced similar modest elevations of defensiveness. Moreover, additional (unpublished) research by the above authors failed to reveal any further elevations when the amount of one odorant (citronella) was increased by a factor of 10. Taken together, the data suggest that a reasonably wide range of odors evoke only mild elevations of risk assessment behaviors. Because live rats provide a rich array of stimuli, those responsible for elevated defensiveness obviously cannot be identified from these data. Further research employing more narrowly defined partial predator stimuli should be useful for this assessment. Evans, Macedonia, and Marler (1993), for example, find that predator-like visual stimuli evoke both vocal and nonvocal fear responses in chicks whose levels are dependent on both the speed and apparent size of the stimulus.

ILLUMINATION CONDITION AND RISK ASSESSMENT 173 Moving/looming stimuli also increase defensiveness of rats (Blanchard, Mast, & Blanchard, 1975) and a considerable range of other species (Ydenberg & Dill, 1986). In addition, the calls of predatory birds increase defensiveness in both mice (Hendrie & Neill, 1991) and birds (Hauser & Caffrey, 1994). Systematic investigation of such stimulus variables may be useful in identifying the most effective fear-inducing stimuli and further contribute to our understanding of anxiety-like states in animals. This possibility is currently under investigation. References ADAMEC, R. E., & SHALLOW, T. (1993). Lasting effects on rodent anxiety of a single exposure to a cat. Physiology and Behavior, 54, 101-109. BLANCHARD, D. C., & BLANCHARD, R. J. (1988). Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 43-68. BLANCHARD, R. J., & BLANCHARD, D. C. (1990). An ethoexperimental analysis of defense, fear, and anxiety. In M. McNaughton & G. Andrews (Eds.), Anxiety (pp. 124-133). University of Otago Press: Dunedin. BLANCHARD, R. J., BLANCHARD, D. C., WEISS, S. M., & MEYER, S. (1990). Effects of ethanol and diazepam on reactivity to predatory odors. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 35, 775-780. BLANCHARD, R. J., MAST, M., & BLANCHARD, D. C. (1975). Stimulus control of defensive reactions in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 81-88. BLANCHARD, R. J., TAUKLlS, H. R., RODGERS, R. J., MAGEE, L. K., & BLANCHARD, D. C. (1993). Yohimbine potentiates active defense responses to threatening stimuli in Swiss-Webster mice. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 44, 673-681. EVANS, C. S., MACEDONIA, J. M., & MARLER, P. (1993). Effects of apparent size and speed on the response of chickens, Gallus gallus, to computergenerated simulations of aerial predators. Animal Behavior, 46, 1-11. GARBE, C. M., KEMBLE, E. D., & RAWLEIGH, J. M. (1994). Novel odors evoke risk assessment and suppress appetitive behaviors in mice. Aggressive Behavior, 19,447-454. GARBE, C. M., KEMBLE, E. D., & STRUNK, P. C. (1993). Effects of chlordiazepoxide on odor-induced risk assessment behaviors in mice. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 314-316. HAUSER, M. D., & CAFFREY, C. (1994). Anti-predator response to raptor calls in wild crows, Corvus brachrhynchos. Animal Behaviour, 48,1469-1471. HENDRIE, C. A., & NEILL, J. C. (1991). Exposure to the calls of predators of mice activates defensive mechanisms and inhibits consummatory behaviour in an inbred mouse strain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 15, 479-482. KEMBLE, E. D., & BOLWAHNN, B. L. (in press). Immediate and long term effects of novel odors on risk assessment in mice. Physiology and Behavior.

174 KEMBLE AND GOBLIRSCH KEMBLE, E. D., & GORDON, C. J. (1996). Yohimbine increases novel odorinduced risk assessment behaviors. The Psychological Record, 46, 179-186. LESTER, L. S., & FANESLOW, M. S. (1992). Nocturnality as a defensive behavior in the rat: An analysis in terms of selective association between light and aversive stimulation. The Psychological Record, 42, 221-253. SULLIVAN, T. S., CRUMP, D. R., & SULLIVAN, D. S. (1988). Use of predator odors as repellents to reduce feeding damage by herbivores. III. Montane and meadow voles (Microtus montanus and Microtus pennsylvanicus). Journal of Chemical Ecology, 14,363-377. WELKER, W. I. (1959). Escape, exploratory and food seeking responses of rats in a novel situation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 1 06-111. WILLIAMS, J. L., & scon, D. K. (1989). Influence of conspecific and predatory stressors and their associated odors on defensive burying and freezing responses. Animal Learning and Behavior, 17, 383-393. YDENBERG, R. G., & DILL, L. M. (1986). The economics of fleeing from predators. In J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, M. Busnel, & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior (vol. 37, pp. 305-317). New York: Academic Press. ZANGROSSI, H., & FILE, S. E. (1992). Chlordiazepoxide reduces the generalized anxiety, but not the direct responses, of rats exposed to cat odor. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 43, 1195-1200.