The thin win: Implicit preference for slim models in advertising

Similar documents
Warmth and competence dimensions of brands as a result of implicit perceptions

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1

Completing a Race IAT increases implicit racial bias

Free Associations as a Measure of Stable Implicit Attitudes

Revised Top Ten List of Things Wrong with the IAT

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes

Anti-fat, pro-slim, or both?

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Howard Sheth Model. The model claims that a person s purchase decision is often influenced by more than one individuals.

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

Strategies for Reducing Racial Bias and Anxiety in Schools. Johanna Wald and Linda R. Tropp November 9, 2013

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts.

Awareness of implicit bias : what motivates behavior change?

Organizational Behaviour

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Topics for today Ethics Bias

Unconscious Knowledge Assessment

Elimination of Implicit Bias By Adapting to Various Personalities

be introduced to examples of beauty trends in history and in other cultures. Next they will

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

!""#$%&'#%$#(#)!$)*&+,&+-*&#!+& & &.& & Adding Significance to the Implicit Association Test. Peter Stüttgen. Joachim Vosgerau.

Application of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to a Study of Deception

Implicit Consumer Preferences and Their Influence on Product Choice

Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes. Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University

Anti-Fat Prejudice: Is there discrimination towards obesity?

Unconscious Bias Training. Programme Overview. 1 Day or ½ Day? +44 (0) /

Patricia Power, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes. Ian Stewart

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

2 Critical thinking guidelines

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute

Attitudinal Dissociation: What Does it Mean? Anthony G. Greenwald and Brian A. Nosek

Implicit Bias: A New Plaintiff Class Action Strategy First Wal-Mart and Iowa What is next?

Reaching for the (Product) Stars: Measuring Recognition and Approach Speed to Get Insights into Consumer Choice. Oliver Genschow

Implicit Bias: Making the Unconscious Conscious

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects?

The Three Appeals of Argument Logical Appeal (logos) Ethical Appeal (ethos) Emotional Appeal (pathos)

Working Toward the Utility of the Future by Understanding and Addressing Bias

The Influence of One s Own Body Weight on Implicit and Explicit Anti-fat Bias

Colin Tucker Smith Ghent University. Kate A. Ratliff Tilburg University. Brian A. Nosek University of Virginia

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Using Implicit Measures in Attitude and Personality Research. Wilhelm Hofmann University of Chicago Booth School of Business

Huber, Gregory A. and John S. Lapinski "The "Race Card" Revisited: Assessing Racial Priming in

Does One Bad Apple(Juice) Spoil the Bunch? Implicit Attitudes Toward One Product Transfer to Other Products by the Same Brand

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

How Racism Correlates with Perceptions and Attributions of Healthcare Disparities

Emotions and Motivation

Implicit and Explicit Anti-fat Bias among a Large Sample of Medical Doctors by BMI, Race/Ethnicity and Gender

Emotions. These aspects are generally stronger in emotional responses than with moods. The duration of emotions tend to be shorter than moods.

Servant and Transformational Followership as a Consumptive Experience

Services Marketing Chapter 10: Crafting the Service Environment

Slide 3.1. Learning and memory

Unconscious Gender Bias in Academia: from PhD Students to Professors

THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX

TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM

Implicit and explicit cognitions in physical activity Using the Single Category Implicit Association Test (SC-IAT)

Counter-conditioning as an intervention to modify anti-fat attitudes

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CHAPTER 7: Attribution theory, confidence and self-efficacy. Practice questions at - text book pages 124 to 125 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS.

IMPLICIT BIAS: UNDERSTANDING AND ADDRESSING ITS IMPACT. ALGA Regional Training Dr. Markisha Smith October 4, 2018

Attitudinal Dissociation. Introduction. What Does It Mean? Anthony G. Greenwald Brian A. Nosek

Body image and explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes: The mediating role of physical appearance comparisons

Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions

How Does the Gender of Spokesperson Affect the Audience s Perception

Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

UNCORRECTED PAGE PROOFS

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Consumer Behavior, Ninth Edition. Schiffman & Kanuk

Unconscious Bias and the Hiring Decision

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16)

Affective Cue and Message Ambiguity in Persuasion: Implications for Green Advertising

CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence. Practice questions - text book pages

The measurement of media literacy in eating disorder risk factor research: psychometric properties of six measures

What is Social Cognition?

The Association between Free Will Beliefs and Stereotypes: People's Belief in Fatalism Promotes Gender Stereotypes

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

Module - 6 CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

Reporting Intentional Rating of the Primes Predicts Priming Effects in the Affective Misattribution Procedure

UBC Social Ecological Economic Development Studies (SEEDS) Student Report

Supplement to Nosek and Smyth (2005): Evidence for the generalizability of distinct implicit

Demonstrations of Implicit Anti-Fat Bias: The Impact of Providing Causal Information and Evoking Empathy

Raymond C. Hawkins II. Fielding Graduate University; The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, USA

The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Perceptions of Mental Illness

A model of parallel time estimation

RESEARCH ARTICLE. GENDER DIFFERENCES IN RISK TAKING: EVIDENCE FROM A MANAGEMENT INSTITUTE Amit Kapoor 1

Overview of the Logic and Language of Psychology Research

CAN PROMOTING CULTURAL DIVERSITY BACKFIRE? A LOOK AT THE IMPLICIT EFFECTS OF PSYCHOLOGICAL REACTANCE. Jeffrey Adam Gruen

Explicit Control Of Implicit Responses Simple Directives Can Alter IAT Performance

Illusion of control is all about the relationship between the conscious and the sub-conscious mind.

Understanding Ethnic Identity in Africa: Evidence. from the Implicit Association Test (IAT) *

Postgraduate Diploma in Marketing June 2017 Examination Exploring Consumer Behaviour (ECB)

Applied Social Psychology Msc.

Supplementary Materials. Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals

Implicit Attitude Generalization Occurs Immediately; Explicit Attitude Generalization Takes Time Kate A. Ranganath and Brian A.

Looking up versus looking down: attractiveness-based organizational biases are moderated by social comparison direction

The Category-Focus Implicit Association Test. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Journal link: This article does not

A Short Form of Sweeney, Hausknecht and Soutar s Cognitive Dissonance Scale

What Drives Priming Effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure?

A guide to peer support programs on post-secondary campuses

Transcription:

The thin win: Implicit preference for slim models in advertising 1. Introduction Research in consumer psychology suggests that much of human behaviour is influenced by non-conscious, uncontrolled, unobserved processes in memory (Bargh, 2002; Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji, 2003). However, increased attention to these implicit processes and associations within academic work did not necessarily stimulate the use of such methods in marketing research practice. Stüttgen, Vosgerau, Boatwright, & Messner, (2009) emphasise the attractiveness of implicit methods outside academia, but also mention the debate over these methods reliability and validity as a main hurdle for wide-spread use in commercial marketing research. Explicit measures remain the most important component of any marketing research program. This research therefore, intends to expand knowledge on consumer research using methods studying implicit processes. Non-conscious, automatic and unobserved processes in memory are important in advertising. Research in this field has relied almost exclusively on explicit measures, dependent on conscious evaluation and deliberation. Although these theories are essential to researchers in the field, they often neglect the potential role of implicit or automatic processes (Perkins, Forehand, Greenwald, Maison, 2008). This study demonstrates that implicit measures add incremental value beyond explicit measures. 2. Background from the literature For the present study we apply the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to the context of attractiveness in advertising. We first present a literature review of the two topics. Attractiveness in advertising In advertising, highly attractive models are widely used as endorsers (Bower, 2001; Gulas & McKeage, 2000). Because physical attractiveness is an attribute of high value, decorative, attractive models in advertisements are mostly female (Saad, 2004). Products accompanied by an attractive model evoke a more positive attitude towards that product and a higher willingness to buy the product (Buunk & Dijkstra, 2011). Content analysis studies over extended time periods demonstrated that the frequent use of women as young and attractive decorative models is longitudinally stable and culturally invariant (Saad, 2004). For further information and theoretical background on the what is beautiful is good -stereotype see Swami & Furnham (2008) and the meta-analysis of Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani & Longo (1991) and Hosoda, Stone-Romero & Coats (2003). The underlying mechanism in this stereotyping is straight forward. Models of the same gender presented in advertisements serve as comparison targets for the viewer. The positive responses to attractive models are primarily the result of assimilation. Following social comparison theory individuals feel (often on the implicit level) that they are similar to, or may in the future become like, this attractive model (Buunk, 2011). For a review see also Buunk & Gibbons (2007). In a recent study Buunk & Dijkstra (2011) demonstrated that women were willing to pay more for the product when it was promoted by an attractive model. The women in this same study also showed a more positive attitude toward the product, when it was promoted by an attractive as opposed to a moderately attractive model. In Western societies almost half of the women express concerns of body dissatisfaction (Ferguson, Munoz, Contreras, & Velasquez, (2011); Ferguson, B. Winegard, & B. M. Winegard, (2011). This dissatisfaction may be blamed to misplaced media and advertising

ideals of beauty (Ferguson et al., 2011). In consequence, one can speculate that more realistic pictures in advertising would be beneficial and more credible as a source (Bower, 2001). Indeed, D Alessandro & Chatty (2011) suggest that marketing practice (they cited the Dove Real Beauty campaign) can use more realistic, and possibly socially responsible depictions of a female body shape that matches their target audience. And indeed a quick search on the internet demonstrates that the company Unilever is well known for its Dove advertising campaigns featuring women of all ages, shapes and sizes, under the advertising claim "Campaign for Real Beauty". For a more in-depth scientific interpretation of the Dovecampaign see Millard (2009). In the context of judging others during a survey interview the social desirability bias may distort the answers. The motivation behind socially desirable responding is a need of subjects to obtain approval by responding in a culturally appropriate and acceptable manner. Therefore, when answering questions on a sensitive topic, individuals tend to give answers that portray themselves (including others) in a more positive light (Lee, 2011). The present study may therefore provide results distorted due to perceived pressure to provide socially acceptable answers. In particular, we expect that explicit answers will be different from the implicit preference (Dimofte, 2010; Gattol, Sääksjärvi, & Carbon, 2011). At the same time we expect the IAT scores (see below) to be immune to faking (Greenwald et al., 2009). Attractiveness works subconsciously (Praxmarer, 2006), or on the implicit level. If people are made aware to the attractiveness effect in some form, they may cognitively control the effect and change behaviour (Praxmarer, 2006). Attractiveness in advertising seems therefore a good example to explore the IAT more in-depth. The present research aims at demonstrating the usefulness of implicit measures such as the IAT. The Implicit Association Test Since the 1980s, much effort has been devoted to reaction time as an indication of automatic processes and activation of attitudes as well as associations. These processes have been mostly studied in the context of attitudes, stereotypes and prejudices (Greenwald et al., 2009). The development of such implicit measures is considered the most important contribution in social cognition research in recent time (Hofmann, et al.2005). In their meta-analysis Greenwald et al. (2009) identified 184 independent samples with a total of 14 900 subjects for the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Perkins et al. (2008) reviewed applications in consumer psychology. They concluded that some studies find an adequate predictive validity, while some others did not. The authors concluded, that the implicit methods were superior in predicting behaviours when issues of stereotyping and prejudice are of concern. For the present study this is clearly the case. Respondents may use stereotypes towards beautiful women and may, in consequence, also be prejudiced. A recent study in Canada is in support of this claim. Brochu and Morrison (2007) demonstrated the presence of explicit and implicit antifat prejudice, with male participants showing greater negativity toward overweight targets. Between explicit and implicit measures of weight bias no significant relation occurred. This again confirms as mentioned above - the socially desirable responding used by the participants. Perkins et al. (2008) also found that the IAT is a useful measure, when consumers are under time constraints (see also Friese, Hofmann, & Wänke, (2008); Friese, Wänke & Plessner, 2008). This may again apply when exposed to advertising.

Summary of the literature review Advertising triggers non-conscious and automatic processes in consumers. Many of those consumers will display some level of body dissatisfaction and will also tend to give socially appropriate answers. The IAT seems to be the method of choice in this setting. We expect that slim is associated with good. In the IAT the reaction time to the slim/good combination will be shorter than the time for the slim/bad combination (operationalized as D-measure; generally called IAT effect). We test for a general IAT effect (H1: D-measure different from zero), for differences in gender (H2) and differences to the explicit measures and stated preference (H3: no association between the D-measure and the explicit preferences). 3. Research method For this IAT study we used a picture / word format, since the visuals in an advertising campaign are normally a central element. Pictures were taken mainly from the internet and pairs of attractive / less attractive key visuals for advertising were produced using commercially available software. The two pictures of each pair of photos were practically identical and included a women, a product logo and some background. We only manipulated the size of the women (slim and well-rounded). In two cases we used identical women and in two cases we used different women, but from very similar appearance, and again one slim model and one well-rounded model. In order to be as close as possible to the daily experience of advertising consumers we used four products as potential examples: a car (brand Rolls Royce ), a shampoo (brand En Gedi ), a watch (brand Piaget ) and a sun lotion (brand Hawaiian Tropic ). This resulted in four nearly identical twin adverts. A pre-test demonstrated that the adverts were almost identical to the daily experience of the pre-testers. For the explicit part of this research a very brief questionnaire was designed. First the participants had to indicate their personal preference for either one of the twin adverts (a choice task). They also indicated their gender and age. This explicit part included at the end a standardised instruction for the Implicit Association Test (IAT). The IAT measure of implicit preferences for the two categories slim and well-rounded included the following pleasant and unpleasant stimuli (German translations of words was used in the IAT runs). Pleasant (good) words: happy, beautiful, pleasure, success, love, laugh, joy, attractive; unpleasant words (bad): pain, hurt, failure, bad, evil, cruel, ugly, repulsive. These stimuli were used previously in other published reports on the IAT. Stimuli representing the two target categories (slim / well-rounded) were the images mentioned above. The two pictures for each brand were similar in size and form. Participants were informed that they were taking part in a consumer study about advertising preferences. First, participants were asked to complete the questionnaire that measured explicit preferences of the two alternative adverts. Immediately after completing the questionnaire, respondents completed the IAT task. They went through this procedure individually. The IAT software (Meade, 2009) involved five classification tasks: 1) single categorization for the two target concepts: slim versus well-rounded; 20 trials 2) single categorization for the attributes: good versus bad; 20 trials 3) combined categorization task and data collection trials: slim model pictures combined with good, versus well-rounded model pictures combined with bad; 40 trials data collection 4) single categorization for the attributes (as in task 1) but with reversal of the side of the screen on which the two attribute labels appeared; 40 trials 5) combined categorization task and data collection trials (as task 3) but reversed categorization of target categories

Participants were recruited from the university of the authors in Switzerland (60 volunteers). After completion the participants were debriefed and thanked for the support. 4. Results A total of 60 people participated in this study. 29 were female and 31 were male. The age ranged from 18 to 59 years with an average of 28.9 years (standard deviation of 10.6 years). Table 1 presents the results of the explicit choice task. Male participants clearly indicated a preference for slim models in all four brand advertisings. The female participants also picked the slim models except for the Piaget brand (the model was maybe a little too attractive). Table 1: Explicit choice task (frequencies). The asterisk indicates statistically significant differences between slim and well-rounded (binomial test; p 5%). Male Female Total slim Wellrounded slim Wellrounded slim Rolls Royce 28* 1 31* 0 59* 1 Hawaiian 29* 0 25* 6 54* 6 Piaget 20* 9 14 17 34 26 En Gedi 26* 3 24* 7 50* 10 Wellrounded For the IAT data the software (Meade, 2009) uses the scoring algorithm of Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji (2003). This algorithm uses practice-block data, includes penalties for errors and uses individual-respondent standard deviations to calculate the D-measure. In other words, the reaction times to the slim/good combination will be shorter than for the slim/bad combination in the case of an implicit association. Large D-measures indicate strong associations between slim and good and weak associations between well-rounded and good. The IAT scores have a mean of 0.465 ± 0.301 milliseconds. This average response time is significantly different from zero (H1; t-test; p 5%). This indicates a significant implicit preference for the slim models. The participants implicitly associated slim with good and well-rounded with bad. We also analysed the data on the basis of gender (H2). The IAT scores for the female respondents have a mean of 0.268 ± 0.287 milliseconds and for the male respondents a mean of 0.696 ± 0.228 milliseconds. For both sexes the average response time is significantly different from zero (t-test; p 5%). This indicates that both, males as well as females, have a significant implicit preference for the slim models. The males have a statistically significant higher preference for the slim models in the advertisements (t-test; p 5%). In order to test for a social desirability bias (H3), we divided the respondents into three groups. A first group consisted of individuals preferring only slim models (n=28). The second group included all respondents, that selected the well-rounded model in one out of four cases (n=21) and the last group picked two slim and two well-rounded models (n=11). None of the respondents picked more than two well-rounded models. In all three groups the IAT-effect was present (t-test; p 5%). An analysis of variance demonstrated no group effect (ANOVA; F=1.29; p 10%). Also the Scheffé procedure for ANOVA (data not shown) showed no

difference between groups. In conclusion, these results suggest that the explicit and the implicit preference are independent of each other. 5. Discussion In the present study the IAT scores have a mean of 0.465 ± 0.301 milliseconds. This is a surprisingly high level indicating a very strong preference from the slim models. In their meta-analysis Greenwald et al. (2009) cite only studies in the area of political preferences at the same effect size level. According to their analysis consumer preference studies have a smaller effect size of 0.32 compared to for example 0.24 for gender IATs and 0.27 for race IATs. This very clear preference for slender models is confirmed by the explicit choice of the respondents. Anecdotal evidence from the debriefing sessions suggest that respondents were quite honest in their explicit preference. They claimed that in other social situations (other than the interview situation) they would provide more socially desirable answers. The high IAT scores from this study may also have to do with the level of representation of the words and pictures (Foroni & Bel-Bahar, 2009). The higher the level of representation the higher the IAT effect. Since our advertisements were very realistic, one would logically expect a higher than average IAT effect size. The somewhat surprising result, that even women prefer slim models may be explained drawing on evolutionary psychology theory (Saad, 2004, Ferguson, 2011). Men use characteristics of beauty to discriminate among potential mates and, in consequence, physical appearance is an important component of reproductive success for women. Put in other words, body dissatisfaction mentioned in the introduction may have more to do with concerns about finding mates and less with displaced media and advertising ideals (Eagly et al., 2011). Ferguson et al. (2011) recently confirmed these findings. Their study showed that body dissatisfaction increased when competitive females were present. In their study they found little evidence for television exposure impacting body dissatisfaction. Our initial point of departure was that individuals make several immediate and automatic appraisals. There is an nearly automatic tendency to categorise a women as attractive or unattractive (Gulas & McKeage, 2004). This automatic response and attractiveness appraisal affected the attitude toward the endorser (the model), which in turn has a positive impact on customer satisfaction in the future (Söderlund & Julander, 2008). Our study confirms the findings of Swami and Furnham (2008). The what is beautiful is good -stereotype applies to advertising as well. Numerous studies and reviews of literature have consistently shown that physical attractiveness and appearance have an important effect on the judgments that humans make about others (Swami & Furnham, 2008, Hosoda et al. 2003, Eagly et al. 1991). These findings combined with our results suggest that the Dove advertising campaigns featuring women of all ages, shapes and sizes (Millard, 2009) may not be appropriate for most products. Dove was successful maybe because the campaign visuals were distinct from other visuals in the same product category. In conclusion, this study demonstrates the usefulness of the IAT for studying consumer behaviour. Its application in marketing management practice is strongly encouraged (see also Stüttgen et al., 2009). 6. Limitations and suggestion for further research There are several limitations of the present study. The pictures and advertising combinations that were used are a selection of beauty enhancing products. This may be different for other products (Praxmarer, 2006). Also, the participants were recruited in a single location and in Switzerland and, thus, the generalisability may be questioned.

7. References Bargh, J. A. (2002). Losing consciousness: Automatic influences on consumer judgment, behavior, and motivation. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 280-285 Bower, A. B. (2001). Highly attractive models in advertising and the women who loathe them: The implications of negative affect for spokesperson effectiveness. Journal of Advertisin,g 30, 51-63 Brochu, P.M. & Morrison, M.A. (2007). Implicit and Explicit Prejudice Toward Overweight and Average-Weight Men and Women: Testing Their Correspondence and Relation to Behavioral Intentions. Journal of Social Psychology, 147, 681-706 Buunk, A. P. & Dijkstra, P. (2011). Does attractiveness sell? Women's attitude toward a product as a function of model attractiveness, gender priming, and social comparison orientation. Psychology & Marketing, 28, 958-973 Buunk, A. P. & Gibbons, F. X. (2007). Social comparison: The end of a theory and the emergence of a Weld. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102 3-21 D'Alessandro, S. & Chetty, B. (2011). Real or Relevant Beauty? Body Shape and Endorser Effects on Brand Attitude and Body Image. Psychology & Marketing, 28, 43-878 Dimofte, C. V. (2010). Implicit Measures o f Consumer Cognition: A Review. Psychology & Marketing, 27, 921-937 Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G. & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but.: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109-128 Ferguson, C. J., Munoz, M.E., Contreras, S. & Velasquez, K. (2011). Mirror, Mirror on the Wall: Peer Competition, Television Influences, and Body Image Dissatisfaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30, 458-483 Ferguson, C. J., Winegard, B. & Winegard, B. M. (2011). Who is the fairest one of all? How evolution guides peer and media influences on female body dissatisfaction. Review of General Psychology, 15, 11-28 Foroni, F. & Bel-Bahar, T. (2009). Picture-IAT versus Word-IAT: Level of stimulus representation influences on the IAT. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 321-337 Friese, M., Hofmann, W. & Wänke, M. (2008). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47 397-419 Friese, M., Wänke, M. & Plessner, H. (2006). Implicit consumer preferences and their influence on product choice. Psychology & Marketing, 23, 727-740 Gattol, V., Sääksjärvi, M. & Carbon, C.C. (2011). Extending the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Assessing Consumer Attitudes Based on Multi-Dimensional Implicit Associations. PloS ONE, 6, 1-11 Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A. & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: I. An Improved Scoring Algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197-216 Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L. & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-Analysis of Predictive Validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17-41 Gulas, C. S. & McKeage, K. (2000). Extending social comparison: An examination of the unintended consequences of idealized advertising imagery. Journal of Academy of Marketing Science, 29, 17-28

Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H. & Schmitt, M. (2005). A Meta- Analysis on the Correlation Between the Implicit Association Test and Explicit Self-Report Measures. Personality and social psychology bulletin, 31, 1369-1385 Hosoda, M., Stone-Romero, E. F. & Coats, G. (2003). The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Job-Related Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Studies. Personnel Psychology, 56, 431-462 Lee, Z. & Sargeant, A. (2011). Dealing with social desirability bias: An application to charitable giving. European Journal of Marketing, 45, 703-719 Meade, A. W. (2009). FreeIAT: An Open-Source Program to Administer the Implicit Association Test. Applied Psychological Measurement, 33 643 Perkins, A., Forehand, M., Greenwald, A. G. & Maison, D. (2008). Measuring the Nonconscious: Implicit Social Cognition on Consumer Behavior. In C. P Haugtvedt, P. M.; Kardes, (Eds.): Handbook of Consumer Psychology. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc Inc, 461-476 Praxmarer, S. (2006). Is beauty best even for the less beautiful?. Marketing ZFP, 2006, 103-112 Saad, G. (2004). Applying evolutionary psychology in understanding the representation of women in advertisements. Psychology & Marketing, 21, 593-612 Söderlund, M. & Julander, C.-R. (2008). Physical Attractiveness of the Service Worker and Its Effects on the Customer in the Moment of Truth. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 16 216-226 Stüttgen, P., Vosgerau, J., Boatwright, P. & Messner, C. (2009). Adding Significance to the Implicit Association Test (Working paper). David A. Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University. Retrieved from http://www.andrew.cmu.edu/user/pstuettg/academic_files/iat.pdf Swami, V. & Furnham, A. (2008). The psychology of physical attraction. New York : Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group