The Impacts of Stereotypical and Counter-Stereotypical Imagery on Female and Male Students: Lessons for Implementation

Similar documents
The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

PERCEIVED BARRIERS AND ENDORSEMENT OF STEREOTYPES AMONG ADOLESCENT GIRLS OF COLOR IN STEM. Allison Scott, Ph.D. Alexis Martin, Ph.D.

Gender and Discipline Specific Differences in Mathematical Self-Efficacy of Incoming Students at a Large Public University

Lecturer: Dr. Emmanuel Adjei Department of Information Studies Contact Information:

PSYCHOLOGY. The Psychology Major. Preparation for the Psychology Major. The Social Science Teaching Credential

Addendum: Multiple Regression Analysis (DRAFT 8/2/07)

A mixed-methodology study of the origins and impacts of the gender gap in college physics Lauren E. Kost

EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY (EPSY)

Analysis of Rheumatoid Arthritis Data using Logistic Regression and Penalized Approach

Does AIDS Treatment Stimulate Negative Behavioral Response? A Field Experiment in South Africa

Ruffalo Noel-Levitz Student Satisfaction Inventory Results: All Students Gallaudet University Spring 2018 Report

Comparability Study of Online and Paper and Pencil Tests Using Modified Internally and Externally Matched Criteria

EPI 200C Final, June 4 th, 2009 This exam includes 24 questions.

Emotional Maturity and Adjustment in First Year Undergraduates of Delhi University: An Empirical Study

Reading and maths skills at age 10 and earnings in later life: a brief analysis using the British Cohort Study

You must answer question 1.

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

Supplementary Online Content

Self-Efficacy in the Prediction of Academic Performance and Perceived Career Options

Write your identification number on each paper and cover sheet (the number stated in the upper right hand corner on your exam cover).

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

Stereotype Threat in America. Morgan Watkins. University of Kentucky

Psychology Department Assessment

INTRODUCTION TO ECONOMETRICS (EC212)

Description of components in tailored testing

Psychology Departmental Mission Statement: Communicating Plus - Psychology: Requirements for a major in psychology:

Homework 2 Math 11, UCSD, Winter 2018 Due on Tuesday, 23rd January

GERONTOLOGY MINOR & GERONTOLOGY CERTIFICATE

Sexual Behavior in the Elderly

AU TQF 2 Doctoral Degree. Course Description

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1

Intro to Expert Jigsaw 2. PEERs Seminar Week 7

The Role of Motivation in Health Education

Online Appendix A. A1 Ability

UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS RIO GRANDE VALLEY Rehabilitation Counseling (MS) Program Requirements

APA Annual Conference (Division 7), August , Chicago, IL. Submission Type: Individual Poster Proposal

MORGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DRUG POLICY

Spatial Visualization Measurement: A Modification of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test - Visualization of Rotations

No certificates awarded. See Alcohol and Drug Studies for certificates offered at surrounding community colleges.

Kent State University Case Study Competition Submission. Group Leader: Danielle Rueger Emily Groce, Justin Beckman, Mel Dickey

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 9. Executive Summary

Available from Deakin Research Online:

INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATIONS: A STUDY AMONGTHE COLLEGE STUDENTS

Help-Giving as a Factor in Perceived Group Helpfulness and Member Satisfaction in Small Counseling Groups

Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage

Protocol analysis and Verbal Reports on Thinking

Tutorial #7A: Latent Class Growth Model (# seizures)

Final Exam - section 2. Thursday, December hours, 30 minutes

2004 MAKING ACHIEVEMENT POSSIBLE SURVEY SUMMARY REPORT

College of Education. Rehabilitation Counseling

WHY THE OPPORTUNITY/ACHIEVEMENT GAPS ARE SO PERSISTENT AND WHAT WE CAN DO TO CLOSE THEM

5/22/12. First Preventive Dental Exam: Disparities in Need Cost + Behavioral Insights! Mini-tour of Milwaukee! Acknowledgements!

In-Person Orientation

RCTC PROGRAM PLAN GOAL 2: CRITICAL THINKING MAY BE MET BY ANY COURSE IN MNTC 1-10 GOALS

EPHE 575. Exercise Adherence. To Do. 8am Tuesday Presentations

Participants in the Program

Section One: Becoming a Recognized Student Organization

CREATIVE INFERENCE IN IMAGERY AND INVENTION. Dept. of Psychology, Texas A & M University. College Station, TX , USA ABSTRACT

Thriving in College: The Role of Spirituality. Laurie A. Schreiner, Ph.D. Azusa Pacific University

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy

Counseling and Psychological Services University of Pennsylvania 3624 Market Street, First Floor West Philadelphia, PA 19104

College of Arts and Sciences. Psychology

The Temporal Stability of Gender Differences in MIS Students

COWLEY COLLEGE & Area Vocational Technical School

PEDIATRIC MANAGEMENT OF BLOOD LEAD LEVELS LESS THAN 10 UG/D: WHY IS THIS NECESSARY?

Community-Based Strategies for Cancer Control

Prospect Theory and Gain-Framing: The Relationship Between Cost Perception and Satisfaction

NEW YORK CITY COLLEGE of TECHNNOLOGY DENTAL HYGIENE DEPARTMENT

Statistics Anxiety among Postgraduate Students

Comprehensive Statistical Analysis of a Mathematics Placement Test

Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask

Kinesiology (M.S.) Kinesiology Admission. Mission. Goals

Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Latest developments in WHO estimates of TB disease burden

Centering Predictors

In this module I provide a few illustrations of options within lavaan for handling various situations.

Self-Authorship: The Key to Student Learning Aspirations

Eliciting Patients Preferences in Kidney

Strategies for handling missing data in randomised trials

Our Steps Forward: Collaborating with Trusted Partners to Address the Unique Health Needs of African Americans

Sensory Cue Integration

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

AWARE Program and Residence Life: A Sustained Model Partnership for Alcohol Abuse Prevention at the University of Wyoming

Review Questions for Exam 2 Math 263

Subjective randomness and natural scene statistics

Observation Activity Based on Student Development Theories. Alicia B. Braswell. Georgia Southern University

B.A. IN PSYCHOLOGY. B.A. in Psychology 1. PSY 430 Advanced Psychopathology PSY 440 & PSY 441

USE OF A CONDITIONAL QUANTILES METHOD TO PREDICT FUTURE HEALTH OUTCOMES BASED ON THE TRAJECTORY OF PEDIATRIC END-STAGE LIVER DISEASE (PELD) SCORES

Utilizing Student Development Theory to Guide Advising Conversations

We must be aware of and fight unconscious bias in healthcare

THE APPLICATION OF ORDINAL LOGISTIC HEIRARCHICAL LINEAR MODELING IN ITEM RESPONSE THEORY FOR THE PURPOSES OF DIFFERENTIAL ITEM FUNCTIONING DETECTION

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

Interpreter Services SSR Report

PSYCHOLOGY, B.S. (BEHREND)

Department of Sociology and Social Work

Clinical Psychology - Choose one of the following courses: PSYC 3401 Abnormal Psychology 3

GENDER-BASED DIFFERENCES IN SCHOOL-AGED CHILDREN S DIVERGENT THINKING

Advanced Consumer Research

Econometrics II - Time Series Analysis

Transcription:

The Impacts of Stereotypical and Counter-Stereotypical Imagery on Female and Male Students: Lessons for Implementation ABSTRACT NaLette M. Brodnax nbrodnax@iq.harvard.edu Harvard University

1 Background Despite high wages, favorable working conditions, and strong demand for workers, the number of women obtaining degrees in technology and engineering has been in steady decline since the 1980s. The reasons for women s underrepresentation in technology degree programs include their lack of exposure to technology, lower intellectual self-confidence, and lower tolerance for risk (Corbett and Hill, 2015; Margolis and Fisher, 2002; Margolis, 2008; Sax, 2008; Butterfield and Crews, 2012). While gender-based exposure, self-confidence, and risk tolerance help to explain why women are less likely to choose a technology pathway, women who overcome these barriers and pursue technology may encounter an additional deterrent: situational threats. Situational threats, referred to as social identity threats by psychologists, arise in situations where women expect to be devalued on the basis of their gender (Murphy et al., 2007). College women who perceive an identity-based threat are less likely to show continued interest in technology. This study reports on an intervention designed to increase college women s participation in technology. As part of a large-scale, randomized experiment, college freshmen were encouraged by an academic advisor to consider technology courses and were provided with an informational brochure on technology course o erings. Students in the treatment group received one of three brochures, which varied by the representation of men and women featured in the imagery. We compare men and women who received all-female or all-male brochures, relative to students who received mixed-gender brochure. We find that women were more likely to enroll in a technology courses, but did not exhibit di erences by imagery. Men were more likely to enroll with the allmale brochure, and much less likely to enroll with the all-female brochure. To provide guidance for the implementation of similar policy designs in the future, we consider possible explanations for students responses to the di erent brochures. 2 Research Design The experiment described below was implemented with approximately 4,000 incoming freshman attending new student orientation at the flagship campus of a Midwestern state university. Students attended orientation in a series of two-day cohorts, with approximately 250 students in each cohort. Students were randomly assigned to treatment and control groups based on the start date of the two-day orientation cohort that they joined. On the first day of each two-day orientation session, students attended a planning workshop in which they received an overview of the degree attainment process and prepared a list of planned courses for their first semester. During the workshop, students were provided with a planning worksheet and a list of open courses that do not require prerequisites. Students in the control group used the list and other materials to complete the worksheet, which became the basis for registration on the second day of orientation. Students in the treatment group received the same materials as the control group, but also received a large, glossy brochure that markets technology courses across a variety of academic areas. The list of technology-focused courses inside the brochure was organized by the type of general education requirement fulfilled (e.g., Arts and Humanities, Natural and Mathematical, etc.). The back of the brochure included elective courses of potential interest to students in a variety of majors. The planning workshop was facilitated by an academic advisor, who distributed the brochure and encouraged students to consider that technology skills are useful for any major, do not require prior experience, and can help to expand students opportunities. Images on the inside of the brochure provide a non-stereotypical presentation of students interacting with technology in order to counter the stereotype of technology as masculine. To evaluate 1

the e ects of this imagery on students enrollment decisions, we assigned students in the treatment group to one of three brochure conditions: a non-stereotypical (control) condition with mixedgender imagery, a stereotypical condition with images of only men interacting with technology, and a counter-stereotypical condition with images of only women interacting with technology. 3 Data Collection The university provided admissions data, demographics, and enrollment records for each student. The outcome variable is a binary indicator for whether the student enrolled in any technology course. The treatment variables were coded to represent the di erent situational contexts on the basis of treatment arm assignments. We included indicator variables for each treatment arm based on the imagery in each brochure. The all-female imagery arm represents the counter-stereotypical condition, the all-male imagery arm represents the stereotypical condition, and the mixed-gender imagery indicator is included to represent the baseline. Table 1: Descriptive Statistics and Covariate Balance: Treatment Arms Control Mixed Arm F vs. Mixed Arm M vs. Mixed Female 0.500 0.520 0.505 0.014 0.502 0.018 0.500 0.500 0.500 0.500 Minority 0.115 0.105 0.113 0.008 0.110 0.005 0.319 0.307 0.316 0.313 Age 18.424 18.379 18.412 0.033 18.385 0.006 0.510 0.384 0.461 0.400 Ability 24.704 24.791 24.480 0.311 24.490 0.302 2.995 3.075 2.978 2.960 N 1,377 1,295 924 584 Standard deviations reported below means; *p<0.10; **p<0.05; ***p<0.01 4 Data Analysis Let Yi be a continuous latent variable for which we only observe the outcome, Y i, enrollment in a technology course: ( 1, if enrolled Y i = 0, otherwise The probability of enrolling in a technology course can be estimated as follows: Pr(Y i ) = logit 1 ( 0 + k X k )= exp ( 0 + k X k ) 1+exp( 0 + k X k ) where X k represents the set of predictors. The data generation process involved students attending one of 23 new student orientation cohorts, interacting with an academic advisor, and choosing courses based on those interactions. As a result, students enrollment decisions may be influenced by cohort or advisor factors or both. 2

To address the lack of independence between observations, we specified a mixed-e ects generalized linear model, as follows: logit(y e )= 0[i] + 1 (Mixed) + 2 (Arm F*Female) + 3 (Arm M*Female) + 4 (Arm F*Male) + 5 (Arm M*Male) + 6 (Female) + 7 (Minority) + 8 (Age) + 9 (Ability) + 10 (Term Day) where 0[i] is the set of random intercepts for each student. 5 Findings Students have many courses to choose from, and the brochure comprises approximately 30 courses across fields, so the probability of enrolling in one of those courses is small, ranging from about 1.5% to 5%. Comparing men and women in the control group, the probability that any single course enrollment will be in technology is more than twice that of women. Receiving the intervention with the mixed-gender brochure led to increases in the probability of technology enrollment for both men and women, but the increase in probability is greater for men (0.94 ppt) than it is for women (0.43 ppt). For women, the all-female and all-male arms suggest a slightly smaller probability of enrolling, but these di erences were not statistically significant from zero. For men who received an all-female brochure were about as likely to enroll in a technology course as men in the control group, while men who received the all-male brochure were more likely to enroll in a technology course than any other group of students. 6 Conclusions The results suggest that changes to the situational context do not fully explain gender di erences. For women, the intervention made a significant di erent in technology enrollment, but the specific brochure did not matter. This suggests that the intervention appealed to women s preferences or operated by some mechanism other than the situational context. For men, the results were not explained by situational context theories, given the strong negative response to the all-female imagery and the small positive response to the all-male imagery, relative to the mixed-gender brochure. Of the studies pertaining to the e ects of domain stereotypes on interest and participation, only one study found negative e ects on men (Cheryan and Plaut, 2010). However, in contrast to this study, the authors found that men s lack of interest in the domain was predicted by dissimilarity with those in a female-dominated field, where men are under-represented. This study finds that changes to the social context can decrease men s participation in technology, where men are over-represented. 3

Table 2: Technology Enrollment Take-up Rates by Gender and Condition Panel A Female Mixed Arm F vs. Mixed % Di. Arm M vs. Mixed % Di. Enrolled 128 100 28 63 65 Not Enrolled 545 367 178 230 315 Total 673 467 206 293 380 % Enrolled 19.02% 21.41% 2.39 ppt 12.57% 21.50% 2.48 ppt 13.03% Panel B Male Mixed Arm F vs. Mixed % Di. Arm M vs. Mixed % Di. Enrolled 223 146 77 105 118 Not Enrolled 383 302 81 178 205 Total 606 448 158 283 323 % Enrolled 36.80% 32.59% -4.21 ppt -11.44% 37.10% 0.30 ppt 0.82% Panel C All Mixed Arm F vs. Mixed % Di. Arm M vs. Mixed % Di. Enrolled 351 246 105 168 183 Not Enrolled 928 669 259 408 520 Total 1, 279 915 364 576 703 % Enrolled 27.44% 26.89% -0.56 ppt -2.04% 29.17% 1.72 ppt 6.27% M-F Gap 17.78 ppt 11.18 ppt 15.60 ppt 4

Table 3: GLMM Estimates of Treatment E ects Mixed Enrollment 0.2618 (0.0833) Arm F Female 0.0625 (0.1759) Arm M Female 0.0841 (0.2149) Arm F Male 0.1260 (0.1211) Arm M Male Female 0.3141 (0.1456) 0.8268 (0.0723) Minority 0.0103 (0.1002) Age 0.0309 (0.0313) Ability Term Day Constant 0.1084 (0.0365) 1.0660 (0.0308) 3.3697 (0.0679) Observations 47,419 Log Likelihood -11,502.4900 Akaike Inf. Crit. 23,028.9800 Bayesian Inf. Crit. 23,134.1900 Note: p<0.1; p<0.05; p<0.01 5

Bibliography Butterfield, J. and Crews, T. (2012). Casting a wider net: A longitudinal study exploring gender di erences, influences and attitudes impacting academic major selection in computing. Computer and Information Science, 5(2):2 10. Cheryan, S. and Plaut, V. C. (2010). Explaining Underrepresentation: A Theory of Precluded Interest. Sex Roles, 63(7-8):475 488. Corbett, C. and Hill, C. (2015). Solving the equation: The variables for women s success in engineering and computing. Technical report, American Association of University Women. Margolis, J. (2008). Stuck in the shallow end: Education, race, and computing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Margolis, J. and Fisher, A. (2002). Unlocking the Clubhouse: Women in Computing. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA. Murphy, M., Steele, C. M., and Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues a ect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18(10):879 885. Sax, L. J. (2008). The gender gap in college: Maximizing the potential of women and men. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. 6