Food Intake as a Mechanism of Temperature Regulation*

Similar documents
SLENDESTA POTATO EXTRACT PROMOTES SATIETY IN HEALTHY HUMAN SUBJECTS: IOWA STATE UNIVERSITY STUDY Sheila Dana, Michael Louie, Ph.D. and Jiang Hu, Ph.D.

946 Chapter 33 The Animal Body: Basic Form and Function

Continuous or Intermittent Calorie Deficits: Which is Better for Fat Loss?

LESSON 3.2 WORKBOOK. What is fast and slow metabolism?

Chapter 12. Temperature Regulation

Homeostasis. Robert Bear David Rintoul. Based on Homeostasis by OpenStax College. 1 Homeostatic Process

Homeostasis * Robert Bear David Rintoul. Based on Homeostasis by OpenStax. 1 Homeostatic Process

Middle distance triathlon Nutrition guide. An overview of the scientific literature and its application

Special Report: The Miracle Supplement For Weight Loss And Optimum Health! By Joel Kaye, MA

Regulation of Body Temperature. Ass. Prof. Dr. ADEL AMRAN DEPARTMENT OF PHYSIOLOGY

Worksheet Questions, Chapter 8, Body Composition and Weight Control

Energy Metabolism and Body Temperature

Schwabe, Emery & Griffith (1938), basing oxygen consumption on body

EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE SUPPORTING THE CONCEPTION OF ADAPTATION ENERGY

Pre-Lab #7: Nutrition

Ancestral Weight Loss Registry Demographics and Preliminary Results Through February 2012

CURVE OF SUGAR EXCRETION IN SEVERE DIABETES.

Metabolism ANS 215 Physiology and Anatomy of Domesticated Animals

Long-Term Weight Control

(*) (*) Ingestion, digestion, absorption, and elimination. Uptake of nutrients by body cells (intestine)

Why we get hungry: Module 1, Part 1: Full report

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology

Motivation and its sources

Research Institute, Brompton, London, S.W.)

The respiratory quotient is the relation by volume of the carbon dioxide DEXTROSE AND LE VULOSE IN HUMANS

Overton,1 who has worked exhaustively at the subject, looked upon. considered by some to be due to the state of the fluid originally in the

ALL RIGHTS RESERVED MEDICAL DISCLAIMER

: /18

BCH 445 Biochemistry of nutrition Dr. Mohamed Saad Daoud

AQA B3.3 Homeostasis LEVEL 1

33.3: Homeostasis. Homeostatic Process. Control of Homeostasis. Negative Feedback Mechanisms

Temperature, Regulation, Thirst, and Hunger

METABOLISM OF d-mannohepttjlose. EXCRETION OF THE SUGAR AFTER EATING AVOCADO

Homeostasis and Feedback

!"#$%&'()*+,-./012&3!"#$%N!"#$%&'()OMMM QMMM!"#$%&'!"#()*+,-.!"#$%&'()*+,-./012priclJ kvirob^=e ÖäóÄìêáÇÉI=ÖäáéáòáÇÉF=!"

Chapter Nine. Temperature Regulation, Thirst, and Hunger

Chapter 20. Assisting With Nutrition and Fluids

Hypoglycemia, Sick Days/DKA and Hospitalization

OBSERVATIONS ON THE REACTION TIME TO CUTANEOUS

Hypoglycemia. Patient Education Diabetes Care Center. How to cope with low blood sugar. General Facts on Low Blood Sugar. Causes

Chapter 23. Nutrition Needs. Copyright 2019 by Elsevier, Inc. All rights reserved.

Homeostasis. Achievement Criteria. Excellence Criteria. Demonstrate understanding of how an animal maintains a stable internal environment 10/02/2016

Biology & The Characteristics of Life

AQA B3.3 Homeostasis LEVEL 3

Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study


Homeostasis Enlightenment

Chapter 13. Motivation and Emotion

Weekly Questions for Dieters

FAMILIAL ASPECTS OF OBESITY

Villarreal Rm. 170 Handout (4.3)/(4.4) - 1 Designing Experiments I

READTHEORY Passage and Questions

s PERSONAL FITNESS PLAN

1. Most organisms are active in a limited temperature range

establishing perfusion and of collecting and analysing the effluent fluid 1934]. Comparable increases in serum potassium were obtained when

Metabolic Abnormalities in the Burn Patient Part 1

Food and Water Intake and Weight Regulation in the Pigeon

How Many Carbs Per Day For A Diabetic To Lose Weight

Fact Sheet #55 November Program on Breast Cancer. and Environmental Risk Factors (BCERF)

AUGUST 2018 SAFETY MEETING HEAT HAZARDS

JEROME, M.B. (Lond.), Lecturer in Medical Pharmacology and Materia Medica. (From the Pharmacological Department, Oxford.)

Elementary Program Unit 5.3

Enzymes. Essential Question: Why are the actions of an enzymes important to us?

Personal Fitness Plan [Type the document subtitle] 3/10/2014 Class Period MW T/TH

H2O to Go! Hydration. It s easier than you think to get dehydrated. No water, no go...

Medicine, Cambridge, England, and Wuppertal, B.A.O.R.

DRUG USE OF FRIENDS: A COMPARISON OF RESERVATION AND NON-RESERVATION INDIAN YOUTH

excreted, in spite of its constant presence in the blood. Similarly, a salt-free diet will rapidly cause the practical disappearance of chlorides

New Food Label Pages Diabetes Self-Management Program Leader s Manual

NAME: HOUR: DATE: NO: 3 Factors that affect your Basal Metabolic Rate (BMR) 5 Factors that affect the food you choose

YOUR PERSONALIZED WEIGHT LOSS ANALYSIS

METABOLIC RATE, BLOOD SUGAR AND THE UTILIZATION OF CARBOHYDRATE

Hunger Motivation. Human beings get hungry and need to eat, do so, and then get full, which is the point at which they cannot eat anymore (satiety).

BodyGem by HealthETech Now Available at Vital Choice Health Store

A day at the sauna. Boardworks Ltd How does the body react to change?

The Digestive System and Body Metabolism

a. This is the same as for the general public, but people with diabetes, like the rest of the public, often eat more salt than they need.

FATIGUE STUDIED IN REACTION TIME EXPERIMENTS. By. furnish results of a general kind, and does not enable one to arrive at a

An introduction to the COCVD Metabolic Phenotyping Core

Exercise and Weight Management

about Eat Stop Eat is that there is the equivalent of two days a week where you don t have to worry about what you eat.

Diabetes Emergency Kit

How to boost performance in your players:

ENERGY REQUIREMENTS UNIT 3: FOOD NUTRIENTS. The Two Classes of Nutrients. 1. Energy Yielding: Carbohydrate, Fat, Protein

INTERMITTENT FASTING FOR FAT LOSS TRY THIS FOR JUST 7 DAYS AND MEASURE YOUR PROGRESS

PERSONAL FITNESS PLAN

21 Insider Tips To Fitness Success Fitness Success Secrets Of Top Fitness Experts

Introduction Causes Of Out Of Control Hunger...3. Dehydration...3. Hyperthyroidism...4. Your Hormones Are off...4. You Skip Meals...

NAME: HOUR: DATE: NO: 3 Factors that affect your Basal Metabolic Rate (BMR) 5 Factors that affect the food you choose

NUTRITION CHALLENGE 2015 DIVISION ST. CROSSFIT FEBRUARY 2015

preliminaryfinding. Current theories of iron metabolism would explain dispute over urinary iron, which is agreed by all to be very small and

PIONEER FEEDS DAIRY CATTLE AND CALF FEEDING TECHNICAL INFORMATION.

PATIENT NAME: DATE OF DISCHARGE: DISCHARGE SURVEY

Motivation and Emotion

SlimLine Setpoint Theory

Obesity & the Fructose Belly

CHAPTER 10: Diet and nutrition & effect on physical activity and performance Practice questions - text book pages

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

Transcription:

CLASSICS IN OBESITY Food Intake as a Mechanism of Temperature Regulation* John R. Brobeck During the conduct of certain studies upon the regulation of energy exchange, two hypotheses have been adopted for testing by experiment. The first of these, viz., that the processes of energy exchange are integrated and regulated by the hypothalamus, has been presented elsewhere, together with data with which it appears to be in agreement (43). The second hypothesis is related to the first, and states that one of the bases upon which this integration is achieved is the effect which each of the four factors of energy exchange has upon body temperature regulation. According to this view, both food and work represent important sources of heat for the organism, and the amount of food eaten or muscular activity undertaken is determined in part by the animal's need for heat and its ability to lose heat to the environment. This second hypothesis implies that food intake should bear a definite relationship to environmental temperature, since the latter is the variable which more than any other determines an organism's need for heat. At a high temperature where loss of heat is difficult, food intake should be low, lest by eating and assimilating food the body acquire more heat than it can dispose of. At a low temperature, food intake should be high because the body can use extra heat in defending itself against hypothermia. It is conceivable, however, that if the temperature is low enough, the animal may not eat enough to supply all of the extra heat it needs, simply because the capacity of the gastrointestinal tract may not be great enough to care for that much food. If this is true, a certain temperature should be found where food intake is maximal, and any further lowering of temperature should not evoke a corresponding rise of food intake. *From the Laboratory of Physiology, Yale University School of Medicine. Aided by a grant from the Fluid Research Fund, Yale University School of Medicine. This paper reports research undertaken in cooperation with the Quartermaster Food and Container Institute for the Armed Forces, and has been assigned number 178 in the series of papers approved for publication. The views or conclusions contained in this report are those of the author. They are not to be construed as necessarily reflecting the views or indorsement of the Department of the Army. Reprinted with permission from the Yale Journal ofbiology and Medicine. 1948;20 545-552. Copyright 0 1997 NAASO. The following experiments were undertaken to explore the truth of these inferences. Method Sixty male rats of the Sprague-Dawley strain were used from the age of 2 to the age of 4% months. They were classified according to body weight into groups of 6, and then housed for 17 days in community cages at a temperature of 82-84 F. To allow acclimatization. Dry calf meal and drinking water were always present. Each group was subsequently used for from 1 to 3 experiments involving exposure for 18 hours to a controlled environmental temperature. At 11:OO A.M. the 6 rats to be used that night were weighed and placed in individual cages at 82-84"F., with drinking water, but no food. At 5:OO P.M. they were weighed again, body temperature was measured, (6) and they were moved to the constant-temperature room where they were given measured amounts of dry calf meal and drinking water. At 1190 A.M. the next day they were weighed a third time, body temperature was again measured, and food and water intakes were determined. The rats were then returned to a community cage in the control room (82-84"F.), where they were allowed at least 2 weeks for re-acclimatization before they were used again. The range of temperature chosen for the experiments was from 65 to 97 F. Every exposure was made in the same room, where rate of circulation of air, though unknown, was constant, and the temperature of radiating surfaces was determined by temperature of ambient air. Whenever a rat spilled food (6 instances), data were discarded; in addition, data were discarded in 3 other instances because one rat was found to be conspicuously heavier than others of his group. The total of 16 exposures presented in figures 1 through 4 includes 8 where 6 rats were used, 7 with 5 animals, and one with only 4 rats. Taken altogether, they represent a series of 16 exposures of 18 hours duration, to constant temperatures ranging from 65 to 97"F., in which reactions of male rats previously acclimatized to 82-84 F. were studied. Each point on figures 1 through 4 represents an average value for one of the 18-hour exposures. OBESITY RESEARCH Vol. 5 No. 6 Nov. 1997 641

Results The range of 65-97 F. utilized here was broad enough to provide both cold and heat stress for rats previously acclimatized to a temperature of 8244 F. To this stress the rats reacted in characteristic fashion by calling into play their mechanisms of temperature regulation. During cold exposure, these mechanisms were effective and adequate (Fig. 2), since the animals always succeeded in avoiding hypothermia. They even experienced in the cold (65-76 F.) a slight rise in temperature which was quite different from the slight fall which occurred at more nearly neutral temperatures (7690 F.). This difference is perhaps accounted for by the observation that at colder temperatures the rats were awake, alert, and active at the time final measurements were begun, while at the neutral temperatures they were quiet or asleep at the end of the exposure. Final body temperature, then, seemed to depend upon whether the rats were active (as they were in the cold) or quiet or asleep (as they were at temperatures near their level of acclimatization). At still higher temperatures (above 90 F.) they began to exhibit FOOD INTAKE. fiq. 1, k cal. 45.. *. 0 0-- 236-237 g 0- - 284-37 " - - 3 15-3 30 *I 3. BODY TEMPERATURE Of CHANGE, fig. 2 8? true fever. Though they were inactive and ate little, they were unable to lose heat as rapidly as it was produced within their bodies; consequently, their temperature rose until a new thermal equilibrium was reached at a higher level. At 97 F. they attained such as equilibrium only after their body temperature had risen an average of about 3 F. Data similar to these may be found in a report published earlier by Herrington (1 l), whose observations upon body temperature in rats have been confirmed. Hemngton also found that the "standard metabolism" of his animals was high at low temperatures, low at neutral temperatures, and high again at high temperatures-a phenomenon the significance of which will be considered below. Measurements of food intake confirmed in general the inferences set forth in the introduction. Food intake varied with the environmental temperature; it was low at high temperatures and high at low temperatures, while during exposure to cold there appeared to be a limit to the amount the rats would eat within 18 hours (Fig. 1). The first of these points may be illustrated by comparing in parallel columns the reactions of two groups of rats nearly equal size, exposed to 72" and 94"F., respectively (Table 1). Although their age and size were comparable, and although their body temperature rose by the same amount, the group at 94 F. ate only one-fourth as much as the group at 73 F. Hemngton's data," already noted, indicate that at 94 F. standard metabolism is not low; on the contrary, it is higher than at neutral temperatures. There is reason to believe, therefore, that under these conditions food intake is not proportional to energy expenditure, since above 90 F. the intake falls as the rate of metabolism rises. (The rats naturally draw upon stored energy reserves and lose weight when this happens, Fig. 3.) Nor is food intake determined by body temperature, since both groups presented in table 1 experienced equal rises of body temperature. Rather, food intake seems to be determined by the conditions of temperature regulation ex- Table 1. 72" F. 94" F. Figure 1 : Relationship between food intake and environmental temperature. Each point represents a group of 5 or 6 rats (with the exception of one group of 4 rats). Figure 2: Relationship between overnight change ir! body temperature and environmental temperature.. Number of rats Average body weight Average body temp. change Average food intake Average water intake Average weight change Standard metabolism* *From figure 1, Hemngton. (I 1) 6 301 gm. +0.9" F. 48 kcal. 21 ml. +5 gm. 900 kcal.1 m2/24 hrs. 5 296 gm. +0.9" F. 12 kcal. 33 ml. -19 gm. 950 kca1.l m2/24 hrs. 642 OBESITY RESEARCH Vol. 5 No. 6 Nov. 1997

30 20 BODY WEIGHT CHANGE, GRAMS fig. 3 eody Welght WATER INTAKE. mc., 0 I #,I. 6W W 80 90 &F. Figure 3: Relationship between overnight change in body weight and environmental temperature. Figure 4: Relationship between water intake and environmental temperature. isting at any given time. During exposure to cold where heat was needed, food intake was high, while during exposure to heat, food intake was low. The nature of the reaction suggests that food intake is regulated by the body as though it is a normal mechanism of temperature regulation. This may be a roundabout and complicated way of saying what everyone knows, namely, that appetite fails in hot weather, although heretofore no adequate explanation of the mechanism of this failure seems to have been given. It may be noted that a relationship between temperature and food intake similar to the one described here has been observed also in human subjects by Johnson and Kark (12), although there is a notable difference between their data and these, inasmuch as theirs relate to chronic, these to acute exposures. That there is a limit to the amount of extra heat a rat can obtain from its food is suggested by the observation that at lower temperatures food intake seemed to reach a constant level. Furthermore, unlike the intake at neutral temperatures (which was related to body weight) the average intake at 65 F. was exactly the same in 3 groups whose average weight was 237, 297, and 330 grams, respectively. Fortyeight Calories or 16 gm. of dry calf meal appeared to be the maximal amount of food the rats could eat during the first 18 hours of exposure to these moderately cold temperatures. This maximum did not appear to have increased after they had gained an average of 100 grams in weight. It was no surprise to find that the intake came to a constant level at low temperatures, in view of the inferences stated above. But failure of larger rats to eat more than did smaller ones in the cold was completely unexpected; since no exposures were made at still colder temperatures to verify the significance of this observation, it is to be accepted with reservations. The shape of the curve reproduced in figure 1 leads one to believe that reactions to lower temperatures should be further studied. Records of water intake yielded no new information, unless the precision with which the rats regulated their water intake (when average values are considered) may be worthy of comment (Fig. 4). At temperatures from 72 to 86 F. the animals gained about 5 gm. within 18 hours (Fig. 3).* At 90 F. no gain occurred, while at still warmer temperatures a loss of weight was noted. One group exposed to 96 F. lost an average of 35 gm. overnight, or about 2 gm. per hour. Yet they did not seem to be harmed by the experience, since upon return to the control room they resumed normal activity almost immediately, and recovered without evident difficulty. Although from these data the composition of their weight loss can not be determined, Herrington s observations suggest that it represented an energy deficit. He found that standard metabolism is elevated at temperatures above 82 F. (28 C.), where food intake in the present experiments was found to be depressed. Energy expenditure may be high, then, where energy intake is low; the rats must be drawn upon stored reserves to supply the deficit, and in doing so, they lost weight rapidly. Discussion Progressively more attention is now being given to the idea that hunger, appetite, and satiety may have important components which do not arise from activity of any part of the gastro-intestinal tract. Grossman, Cummins, and Ivy (9), as well as Harris, Ivy, and Searle (10) have pointed out the central nature of hunger, while Adolph (1) has expressed the concept in these words: Food acceptance and the urge to eat in rats are found to have relatively little to do with a local condition of the gastro-intestinal canal, little to do with the organs of taste, *This is not to be regarded as their net gain per day, since the rats did most of their feeding at night and ate little by day. Moreover, they had been fasted for 6 hours before each 18-hour exposure. OBESITY RESEARCH Vol. 5 No. 6 Nov. 1997 643

and very much to do with quantitative deficiencies of currently metabolized materials. It would be satisfying to know how these deficiencies act in the neuromuscular system that canies out the ingestion. At present there is no sure knowledge of particular sensory areas or afferent pathways. It may be remembered that all kinds of animals have urges to eat, but few have any one pattern of structures. Adolph s statement reflects a growing tendency on the part of many physiologists to question the autonomy which in the past has been ascribed to the stomach and small intestine in regulating food intake. Granting that normally all the nourishment taken by higher animals passes through the digestive tract, it is difficult to believe that these viscera are in themselves capable of maintaining food intake at its usually constant level from day to day, and also able to initiate the great variety of adjustments with which an organism responds whenever the composition of its diet or environmental conditions are changed. The data reported here are considered to be in accord with the views of the authors mentioned above, to the effect that the central nervous system participates in this regulation. Moreover, these observations appear to reveal something of the nature of the central regulation. Adolph (1) has observed, The investigator gains the feeling that some complex of internal compositions intermittently drives the animal to eat so that some resultant concentration or stimulus is kept just above or below a threshold value (p. 122). One might add that none of the various concentrations or stimuli thus far proposed has satisfactorily explained the urge to eat (cf., Morgan (13)). The suggestion that the blood sugar level is the critical factor may be taken as an example; although Bulatao and Carlson (7) were able to show that insulin, which provokes hypoglycemia, increases hunger contractions, Scott, Scott, and Luckhardt (14) were unable to demonstrate the existence of normal, spontaneous fluctuations in the blood sugar level which might control eating reactions. It is this apparent failure of other hypotheses to explain the regulation of food intake which has led to the present attempt to relate food intake and temperature regulation. As Rubner, and later, Benedict and Carpenter (2), and Strang and McClugage (15) proved, total heat production sharply rises following a meal, and then declines again over a period of a few hours. At the same time, the temperature of the skin rises and falls, indicating that some of the extra heat is being dissipated through cutaneous vasodilatation. Booth and Strang (3) believed these skin temperature changes to be related to the onset of satiety in both the normal and the obese human subjects they studied. The relationship which they described may arise from the fact that both cutaneous vasodilatation and cessation of eating are mechanisms of temperature regulation, called into play to prevent hyperthermia. If this is true, the amount of food taken at any one time will depend upon how much extra heat is produced from the food, and how rapidly the extra heat is lost from the body. The latter of these factors, depending as it does upon environmental temperature, may further account for the relationship between food intake and temperature as it has been disclosed by the present experiments. Expressed in its broadest form, the hypothesis presented here states that animals eat to keep warm, and stop eating to prevent hyperthermia. This hypothesis appears to have the virtue of attempting to account for regulation of food intake in lower animals which, although not homeothermic, do have certain abilities to protect themselves against thermal stress (8). To accomplish these reactions are required only cells capable of metabolizing foodstuffs and liberating heat, together with cells sensitive to heat and able to activate or inhibit cells responsible for taking of food. In simple organisms all of these reactions might conceivably be carried on by a single cell. Yet the work of Booth and Strang (3) as well as observations by Johnson and Kark (12) (see above) suggest that the behavior of man may also fit into this general pattern. One need not add that even though food intake may serve as a mechanism of temperature regulation, this is surely not the only factor capable of modifying the urge to eat. The nature of the various factors and their relative importance require further study. Summary When exposed for 18 hours to temperatures between 65 and 76 F., adult male rats increased their food intake, gained weight normally, and avoided hypothermia. At temperatures above 92 F., food intake was low, water intake was somewhat increased, and the rats lost weight rapidly and experienced fever. These data indicate that food intake is not necessarily determined by total energy expenditure, nor by the animal s body temperature. Rather, food intake appears to be controlled as if it is a mechanism of temperature regulation. The amount of food eaten appears to be determined, at least partly, by the organism s ability to dissipate the heat of food metabolism (Rubner s Specific dynamic action ). References 1 Adolph, E.F.: Am. J. Physiol., 1947, 151, 110-25. 2 Benedict, F.G., and T.M. Carpenter: Food ingestion and energy transformations, with special reference to the stimulating effect of nutrients. Washington, Carnegie Institution of Washington, 1918, publ. no. 261, 355 pp. 3 Booth, G., and J.M. Strang: Arch. Int. Med., 1936,57, 53343. 4 Brobeck, J.R.: Am. J. Physiol., 1945, 143, 1-5. 5 Brobeck, J.R.: Physiol. Rev., 1946, 26, 541-59. 6 Brobeck, J.R., Mary Wheatland, and J.L. Strominger: Endocrinology, 1947, 40, 65-72. 644 OBESITY RESEARCH Vol. 5 No. 6 Nov. 1997

7 Bulatao, E., and A.J. Carlson: Am. J. Physiol., 1924, 69, 107-15. 8 Dill, D.B.: Life, heat and altitude. Cambridge, Harvard Univ. Press, 1938, 211 pp. 9 Grossman, M.I., G.M. Cummins, and A.C. Ivy: Am. J. Physiol., 1947, 149, 100-102. 10 Harris, S.C., A.C. Ivy, and Laureen M. Searle: J. Am. Med. ASSO., 1947, 134, 1468-75. 11 Herrington, L.P.: Am. J. Physiol., 1940, 129, 123-39. 12 Johnson, R.E., and R.M. Kark: Science, 1947, 105, 378-79. 13 Morgan, C.T.: Physiological psychology. N.Y. McGraw-Hill, 1943, 623 pp. 14 Scott, W.W., C.C. Scott, and A.B. Luckhardt: Am. J. Physiol., 1938, 123, 24347. 15 Strang, J.M., and H.B. McClugage: Am. J. M. Sci., 1931, 182, 49-8 1. OBESITY RESEARCH Vol. 5 No. 6 Nov. 1997 645