Substance consumption and willingness to drive a comparison of illegal drugs and alcohol

Similar documents
Grand Rapids Effects Revisited: Accidents, Alcohol and Risk

Drug Driving in NSW: evidence-gathering, enforcement and education

Results from GPS in Serbia SMART questionnaire. Biljana Kilibarda Institute of Public Health of Serbia

Australian secondary school students' use of tobacco, alcohol, and over-thecounter and illicit substances in 2014

OECD Paris, September 2008

Drink driving in Denmark and available measures

Report on Drugged Driving in Louisiana. Quantification of its Impact on Public Health and Implications for Legislation, Enforcement and Prosecution

Alcohol interlock pilot project of AFN Dresden

Driving Drunk, Driving High: A Comparison of Student Attitudes Towards Driving while Drunk Versus Driving while High on Cannabis

Basic knowledge about Drugs Week # 3

Cannabis, Cannabinoids and Driving

Current New Zealand BAC Limit. BAC (mg/100ml)

Drug and Alcohol Awareness

DRINKING AND DRIVING. Alcohol consumption, even in relatively small quantities, increases the risk of road crashes.

Sampling Methodology

St. Cloud State University Tobacco, Alcohol, and Other Drug Use

Development Department Research Programme Research Findings No. 102

Drugs and Alcohol Abuse Policy

South Australian Alcohol and Other Drug Strategy

Regranting of Drivers Licences in Germany: Individual Differentiation vs Formalism

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference

Drugs and Driving: Detection and Deterrence

ALCOHOL THE FACTS WHAT YOU OUGHT TO KNOW ABOUT ALCOHOL.

Alcohol consumption in the Ongoing New Zealand Household Travel Survey

Fit to Drive: A European view on Traffic Safety

According to the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 1,825 students between 18 and 24 die each year from alcohol related incidents

Standardized Field Sobriety Testing Refresher

1 Cannabis problems in context: understanding increasing in treatment demand

Alcohol and Drug Consumption by Québec Truck Drivers

Special Topic: Drugs and the Mind

TO PUNISH AND/OR TO TREAT THE DRIVER UNDER THE INFLUENCE OF ALCOHOL AND/OR OTHER DRUGS. M. R. Valverius, M.D. SYNOPSIS

Rapid screen of Urine Samples for amphetamines, cocaine, methamphetamine, opiates (heroin), marijuana and MDMA (ecstasy)

Characteristics and Predictors of Recidivist Drink-Drivers

The Prevalence of Illegal Drug Use

Review of Drug Impaired Driving Legislation (Victoria Dec 2000) and New Random Drug Driving Legislation Based on Oral Fluid Testing

Reasons cited by students against the decriminalisation of Cannabis. Other Total Missing Data Total

The Art of being Human

NIGHTLIFE QUESTIONNAIRE RECREATIONAL USEV7 core module / 2018

Norfolk & Suffolk Crime Prevention Guidance Note Legal Highs

TEDI Drug Checking Consultation and Counselling Guidelines

The prevalence and impairment effects of drugged driving in New Zealand June 2017

SPEED-02: On the efficiency of a psychological rehabilitation program

GERMANY. Recorded adult (15+) alcohol consumption by type of alcoholic beverage (in % of pure alcohol), Spirits 20%

4/27/2016. About CCSA. Impaired Driving Research at CCSA. Background. Presentation Overview. Lessons Learned

DRIVING, DVLA, AND ADVISING PATIENTS: TOP TIPS

CANAM INTERVENTIONS. Addiction

0 questions at random and keep in order

MARIJUANA USE AMONG DRIVERS IN CANADA,

Name Class Date. 7. state in which the body is poisoned by alcohol and physical and mental control is reduced

The End of the Decline in Drink Driving in Britain?

Information sheet: Alcohol facts

The Use, Misuse and Abuse of Alcohol, and Psychoactive Drugs among Older Persons

Effects of Cannabis on Psychomotor Skills and Driving Performance - a Metaanalysis of Experimental Studies

Prevention of illegal drug consumption in the Federal Centre for Health Education (BZgA)

Composite Prevention Profile: City of Chicago, Illinois

Drugs. January 30, , 2008 Pearson Education, Inc. Upper Saddle River, NJ 07458

NSW Summit on Alcohol Abuse August NSW Parliament. NSW Department of Education and Training

2014 Florida Youth Substance Abuse Survey

Drug Prevention Policy CONTROL OF LEGAL SUBSTANCES

2016 Florida Youth Substance Abuse Survey

2016 Florida Youth Substance Abuse Survey

Survey of U.S. Drivers about Marijuana, Alcohol, and Driving

Overview. Check Who Drives Quality level. Executive summary. Quality level : 1

Introduction to Sensitive Topics and Interviewing for Alcohol Use Practice of Medicine 1 January 7, 2003

Drug Intervention and Education. Dr. Kevin Raper Compass Point Counseling

Marijuana s Effects on Actual Driving Performance

UNIVERSITY STATEMENT FOR STUDENTS ON SUBSTANCE USE/MISUSE

Blue Tuna.

NORWAY. Recorded adult (15+) alcohol consumption by type of alcoholic beverage (in % of pure alcohol), Other 2% Wine 31%

ALCOHOL IMPAIRED IMPAIRED

drug trends bulletin Driving behaviours among people who inject drugs in South Australia, Key findings december 2011 ISSN

Jenny Chalmers David Bright Rebecca McKetin. Australian Research Council Linkage grant Bureau of Crime Statistics and Research

Screening for Drivers under the Influence of Drugs The new Drugwipe for Sweat and Saliva Testing

DRINK AND DRUG-RELATED DRIVING

DRIVERS INVOLVED IN ACCIDENTS AND SUSPECTED UNDER INFLUENCE

Designer drugs and road safety in Japan

Drugs. Year Group 8 Experience The Street. Setting the scene...

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst

Facts About Drinking. HHHH HH Partial/Cloze Dictations 33. Introduction

ROMANIA. Upper-middle Income Data source: United Nations, data range

Alcoholism markers in their relation to prevention and rehabilitation

An Evaluation of the High Risk Offender Scheme

Conventional and emerging drugs of abuse: the current situation

DRUG-FREE AND ALCOHOL-FREE WORK PLACE

NIDA-Modified ASSIST Prescreen V1.0 1

Drugs and Alcohol. Legal Position for Drug and Alcohol Management

SLOVENIA. Recorded adult (15+) alcohol consumption by type of alcoholic beverage (in % of pure alcohol), Spirits 13%

RESPONSE TO THE DEPARTMENT FOR TRANSPORT S CONSULTATION PAPER

Q1. The graph shows the risk of addiction and risk of harm to the body for some drugs.

KAZAKHSTAN. Upper-middle Income Data source: United Nations, data range

Youth Involvement In Traffic Accidents In Japan-New Trends

European Legal Database on Drugs

Culture, Perceptions and Social Norms. Dr. Jeff Linkenbach, National MOST Of Us Institute Montana State University

Alcohol Impaired Driving Recruiting and Preparing Peer Leaders

Appendix A: Classroom Fact-Finding Worksheet Answer Key

What are they? Why do people take these drugs?

Alcohol & Drug Abuse Prevention Team. Student Survey Report 2018

University of Wisconsin-Stevens Point Alcohol and Other Drug Use Survey Data Spring 2011

Taking Opioids Responsibly for Your Safety and the Safety of Others: Patient Information Guide on Long-term Opioid Therapy for Chronic Pain

Transcription:

Substance consumption and willingness to drive a comparison of illegal drugs and alcohol H.-P. Krüger and M. Vollrath Center for Traffic Sciences (IZVW), University of Wuerzburg, Germany Keywords Illegal drugs, alcohol, willingness to drive Abstract The relationship between illegal drug consumption and the willingness to drive under the influence of illegal drugs was examined using the data of a field study in 1998 funded by the BAST. Young drivers at discotheques were contacted and asked to participate in a short interview concerning drugs and driving. Subjects who had consumed drugs and were driving at this evening or at similar occasions were asked for an extended interview and a drivingsimulator test. Moreover, blood, urine and saliva samples were taken. Additionally, sober control subjects and subjects under the influence of alcohol were included. Due to the patterns of drug consumption, the analyses are constrained to the use of amphetamines / ecstasy, cannabis and alcohol. The results show that on the one hand users of illegal drugs are much more likely to drive under the influence of these drugs as (alcohol) drinkers. On the other hand, a substantial percentage of the drug users is not willing to drive after consuming illegal drugs. Moreover, a comparable positive relationship between level of consumption and willingness to drive under the influence of substances is found for users of illegal drugs and alcohol drinkers. The probability to refrain from driving under the influence of a psychoactive substance is increased when trips under the influence of these substances are condemned, when they are regarded as being risky and when there is a large likelihood of being detected by the police. In drug users, these attitudes are more liberal than in alcohol drinkers and the likelihood of detection for driving with illegal drugs is estimated very low. This explains the larger willingness to drive after consuming illegal drugs. However, modifying these attitudes and increasing detection rates may thus prove an effective tool to reduce this risky behaviour. As increasing detection rate will probably also influence the attitudes. This seems to be the most urgent task of preventing drug-impaired driving. Introduction While factors influencing the decision to drive under the influence of alcohol have been examined extensively, it remains uncertain if similar factors are important with regard to driving under the influence of illegal drugs. Additionally, illegal drug consumption (even without driving under the influence of illegal drugs) poses a special problem in Germany: If someone is caught having consumed illegal drugs, the governmental licensing agencies may re-draw

the drivers licence. It can only be re-granted if a medical-psychological examination ( medizinisch-psychologische Untersuchung MPU) has been passed by the driver in which he or she has made certain that he or she doesn t consume illegal drugs any more. Thus, the ability to drive depends on refraining from drug use. While this practice may be criticized from several points of view, an the underlying assumption remains to be proven: It is argued that illegal drug consumers are either not able or not willing to refrain from consuming drugs when driving and thus should not be allowed to drive at all. This assumption will be examined in the context of this paper by comparing attitudes and behaviour of drivers under the influence of illegal drugs (cannabis, amphetamines and ecstasy) and alcohol. Methods The study was conducted in three larger cities in Bavaria, Germany (Munich, Nuremberg and Wuerzburg). In and around these towns, 29 discotheques were selected where a large part of the visitors attended by car and where experts rated drug use as highly probable. Between July and November of 1998, 66 events were visited. 54.5% of those were so-called Techno- Parties where amphetamines and ecstasy were supposed to be the dominant drugs. The other 45.5% consisted of Heavy Metal, Independent and various events with cannabis as the dominant drug. 62 of the events took place on Friday or Saturday night. Depending on the time schedule of the discotheques, the investigation times were either between midnight and 6 a.m. or between 10 p.m. and 4 a.m. For the investigation, a camper van was used where a driving simulator was installed. Additionally, two tents were erected for an extended interview and a medical examination. A research team consisted of 6 researchers. Two of these conducted short interviews with potential subjects in order to select participants for the intensive investigation. One researcher performed an extended interview, another attended the driving simulator. A fifth researcher provided coordination between the different researchers. A medical doctor took blood, urine and saliva samples and conducted a short medical examination. In order to find the subjects of interest, different selection criteria were defined: First of all, when researchers contacted a group of incoming or leaving people, they asked who the driver was and selected him or her for the short interview. If no driver was present, people were asked if anybody was driving regularly at comparable events (but just not at the evening of the study). Thus, either a driver or a potential driver was selected for the short interview. In the short interview, subjects were asked about drug use and driving under the influence of drugs. The answers provided the basis for the second step of the selection process. First of all, subjects under the influence of drugs were selected. Additionally, five groups of control subjects were searched for: a performance control group (no drug use during the last year), an alcohol control group (BAC between 0.03% and below 1.1%), a group of drug users currently not under the influence of drugs (long-term effects of drug use), drugs users and drivers who do not, however, drive under the influence of drugs, and subjects admitting driving under the influence of alcohol. The latter two groups were not examined in the driving simulator but just questioned extensively. Subjects meeting these criteria were asked to participate in the intensive investigation including driving simulator, extensive interview and a medical examination. For the extensive investigation subjects were rewarded with DM 60 (about $30 US). Overall, 3081 subjects were selected for the short interview and 2779 participated (90.2% responder rate). From these, 832 were asked to participate in the extensive investigation, 503 took part in at least some parts of the investigation and n = 483 subjects provided answers in the extended interview concerning the consumption of illegal drugs and alcohol and driving under the influence of illegal drugs and alcohol. The following results are based on this sample of 473 drivers.

Results Drivers were questioned about illegal drug consumption during the last year and the last month. 67% of the drivers who indicated drug consumption during the last year stated that they had been driving with illegal drugs. 40% of them drove with illegal drugs within the last 30 days. 75% of the drivers who indicated drug consumption during the last 30 days had been driving with illegal drugs. 64% of them did so within the last 30 days. Thus, a quite strong correlation between drug use and willingness to drive with illegal drugs is found. For alcohol, drinking and driving is more likely to be separated. Only 14% of the drivers who were sober at the time of the study indicated that they had been driving with a (self-estimated) BAC of 0.05% and above within the last 30 days. For drivers under the influence of alcohol at the time of the study, this percentage rises to 27%. Moreover, illegal drug drivers state to have been driving 10 of 30 days while alcohol drivers tell of 4 trips under the influence of alcohol during the last 30 days. Thus, on the one hand drug users show a larger willingness to drive under the influence of drugs as compared to the willingness of alcohol drinkers to drive under the influence of alcohol. On the other hand, a quite large percentage of drug users state that they do not drive when having consumed drugs. Thus, illegal drug use does not automatically imply driving under the influence of illegal drugs. Table 1: Willingness to drive under the influence of alcohol and drugs with regard to the quantity of alcohol and drugs consumed. Alcohol consumption Driving under the influence of alcohol Doesn t Drives drive Low 32 56.1% High 22 32.8% Overall 54 43.5% Drug use Medium 38 40.0% Strong 12 9.0% Heavy 1 0.8% Overall 51 14.7% 25 43.9% 45 67.2% 70 56.5% Driving with illegal drugs Doesn t Drives drive 57 60.0% 122 91.0% 118 99.2% 297 85.3% Overall 57 67 124 Overall 95 134 119 348 With regard to the quantity of drugs consumed, strong correlations were found which are shown in Table 1. Drug users were divided into three groups: (1) Medium drug use: cannabis, only, in up to 10 of 30 days, (2) strong drug use: cannabis, only, in 11 to 30 of 30 days or amphetamines / ecstasy in up to 10 of 30 days, (3) heavy drug use: amphetamines / ecstasy in 11 to 30 of 30 days or use of other drugs like cocaine, LSD, heroine. Subjects who did not

consume any drugs were divided into two groups with regard to alcohol consumption (n = 11 of these subjects were excluded because they did not consume any alcohol): (1) low alcohol consumption: below 45 grams pure alcohol per drinking occasion or less than 75 grams pure alcohol per drinking occasion and only up to 9 of 20 days with alcohol consumption. (2) high alcohol consumption: 45 to 75 grams of pure alcohol in 10 to 20 of 20 days or more than 75 grams of pure alcohol per drinking occasion. With regard to alcohol consumption, 43.9% of drivers with a low consumption are willing to drive with a (self-estimated) BAC of 0.05% and above as compared to 67.2% of drivers with a high alcohol consumption. A comparable correlation is found with regard to drug consumption: 60% of subjects with medium drug use indicate a willingness to drive with illegal drugs as compared to 91% of subjects with strong drug use and 99.2% of subjects with a heavy drug use. Thus, for alcohol as well as for illegal drugs the willingness to drive under the influence of a substance depends on the quantity and frequency of substance consumption: The stronger the substance use the larger the probability to drive under the influence of the substance. However, illegal drug users are more willing to drive under the influence of illegal drugs than alcohol drinkers are willing to drive with alcohol. How can this be explained? On the one hand, cannabis users are convinced to be able to drive as well under the influence of cannabis as without drugs (1). This conviction is probably due to the fact that drivers think that they are able to compensate any negative drug effects. Experimental studies in the driving simulator as well as in real traffic have shown that drivers under the influence of cannabis drive more slowly than without drugs (e.g., 1, 2). This effect is opposite to that of alcohol where drivers tend to drive faster under the influence of alcohol. The combination of the subjectively low danger exerted by drug consumption with the conviction to be able to compensate any drug effects contributes to the high willingness to drive under the influence of illegal drugs. Cannabis Amphetamines Cocaine Tranquillizers Ecstasy LSD Opiates How bad is driving with...? 1 Beer 4 Beers 0 5 10 heavy strong medium abstinent Figure 1: Subjective evaluation of driving under the influence of illegal drugs and alcohol with regard to quantity of drug use. As reference lines, the subjective evaluation of driving with 1 beer (0.5 liter) and 4 beers (2 liters) are given. On the other hand this conviction is highly correlated with the subjective evaluation of illegal drug driving ( How bad is it to drive under the influence of illegal drugs? ). Figure 1 gives the subjective evaluation (0: not bad at all, 10: extremely bad ) with regard to quantity of drug use and the kind of drug used. Subjects who do not consume drugs regard driving under the influence of any drug comparable to driving with 4 beers (2 liters of beer, clearly above

the legal limit). Drug users differentiate between different drugs. This effect is seen most strongly for cannabis: The higher the quantity and frequency of drugs consumed, the less driving with cannabis is condemned. Driving with amphetamines and cocaine is evaluated as somewhat worse, however less bad than driving with alcohol. On the one hand, this differentiation reflects the actual risk exerted by the drugs. On the other hand, subjective evaluation of driving with illegal drugs clearly depends on the quantity of drugs consumed. Last but not least the amount and effectiveness of police controls exerts a decisive influence. Figure 2 shows the answers to the question How likely is it that drug consumption is detected in a police control?. For alcohol, drivers indicate a detection rate of 40.3% and 45.5% for one and four beers (0.5 liters), respectively. For cannabis and stimulants this percentage lies below 5 %. 1 Beer 4 Beers Stimulants Cannabis Other Drugs 40.3 45.5 4.8 4.1 8.3 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 Percentage detected Figure 2: Estimated percentage of substance use detected in police controls with regard to the kind of substance used. Discussion Summing up the results, both driving under the influence of alcohol and drugs depends on similar factors. Drivers are more likely to refrain from driving under the influence of a substance if they believe the substance to be dangerous and if they think it is bad to drive under the influence of the substance. This conviction results from the objective danger posed by the drug, by legal consequences of driving under the influence of the substance and by the probability to be detected when driving under the influence of the substance. The subjective evaluation will be less negative, if more of the substance is consumed. Thus, the large willingness to drive under the influence of illegal drugs as compared to alcohol does not depend on other decision processes in drug users but is due to a lower subjectively estimated danger posed by illegal drugs, a less negative evaluation of drug use while driving and a lower subjective probability to be detected when driving under the influence of illegal drugs. On the one hand, illegal drug users are thus more likely to drive under the influence of illegal drugs as compared to alcohol. On the other hand, some percentage of drug users refrains from driving when being under the influence of illegal drugs. Moreover, influencing factors are similar when alcohol and illegal drugs are compared. However, the negative subjective evaluation of drug effects and driving under the influence of drugs as well as the probability to be detected when driving with illegal drugs is lower as compared to alcohol. For alcohol it has been shown that increasing the probability to be detected by the police is the most effective tool to reduce driving under the influence of alcohol. Thus, this also seems to be the most

important factor with regard to illegal drugs. From this point of view the most urgent task for drug driving prevention remains to increase the possibilities to detect drug use when driving. References 1. Vollrath M, Löbmann R, Krüger H-P, Schöch H, Widera T, Mettke M. Fahrten unter Drogeneinfluss Einflussfaktoren und Gefährdungspotential. Berichte der Bundesanstalt für Straßenwesen, Mensch und Sicherheit M 132. Wirtschaftsverlag NW, Bremerhaven, 2001. 2. Robbe, HWJ. Influence of Marijuana on driving. Institute for Human Psychopharmacology, Maastricht, 1994.