Self-Efficacy And Psychological Skills During The Amputee Soccer World Cup

Similar documents
Mood and Anxiety Scores Predict Winning and Losing Performances in Tennis

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 30 (2011) WCPCG-2011

The Relationship Between Task Cohesion and Competitive State Anxiety

The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive Anxiety on Rowing Athletes

The Influence of Somatic Anxiety on Sport Performance among Taekwondo Athletes

Andreas Bund University of Darmstadt. Self-Confidence and Sports Performance

The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive Anxiety on Sepak Takraw Athletes

Examining the Relationship between Team Cohesion, Comparative Anxiety and Self-Confidence among Ethiopian Basketball Teams

A COGNITIVE-BEHAVIOURAL ANALYSIS OF MENTAL TOUGHNESS IN NATIONAL RUGBY LEAGUE FOOTBALL TEAMS 1

A Season Long Case Study Investigation of Collective Efficacy In Male Intercollegiate Basketball

Competitive Anxiety and Coping Strategies in Young Martial Arts and Track and Field Athletes

Psychological skills of elite archery athletes

RJSS. The Effect of Performance Strategic on Team Cohesion and Success in Elite Volleyball Players and Relation with Experience in National Team

The Relationship of Competitiveness and Achievement Orientation to Participation in Sport and Nonsport Activities

The Differences between the Winning and Losing Team Anxiety Level and Self - Confidence in the Malaysia Final FA Cup 2016.

CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence. Practice questions - text book pages

The relationship between anxiety and performance

ANALYSIS OF SOURCES OF SPORT CONFIDENCE AMONG KERALA FOOTBALL PLAYERS

D.O.I: Young Researchers Club, Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad University, Kurdistan, Iran 2

SELF-EFFICACY AMONG MALE AND FEMALE INTERVARSITY SOCCER PLAYERS

Precompetition Self-Confidence: The Role of the Self

Dispositional Flow State among Open Skill Athletes: A Predictor and Quantification of Sport Performance

A comparative study of psychological skills of different level Indian boxers

Influence of Self-management on Dance Confidence and Dance Achievement of University Student Majoring in Dance

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY PAST PAPER QUESTIONS

EFFECTS OF COMPETITION ANXIETY ON SELF-CONFIDENCE IN SOCCER PLAYERS: MODULATION EFFECTS OF HOME AND AWAY GAMES By Hyunwoo Kang 1, Seyong Jang 1

The Relationship Between Confidence and Performance Throughout a Competitive Season

Positive and negative motivational self-talk affect learning of soccer kick in novice players, mediated by anxiety

Exploring the relationship between selfefficacy

The Influence of Type of Sport and Time of Season on Athletes' Use of Imagery

An Examination of Mental Toughness over the Course of a Competitive Season

BOTSWANA TEAM SPORT PLAYERS PERCEPTION OF COHESION AND IMAGERY USE IN SPORT

Imagery and observational learning use and their relationship to sport confidence

SUMMARY, CONCLUSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS

CHAPTER 7: Attribution theory, confidence and self-efficacy. Practice questions at - text book pages 124 to 125 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS.

Mental Toughness in Sport: Critical Reflections and Future Considerations

Types of Attentional Focus Key Several types of attentional focus are appropriate for specific sport skills & activities.

Bringing out the Best: Utilizing Bandura s Model of Self-Efficacy to Expand Current Concepts of Coaching Efficacy

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes.

Study of Meditational Role of Self-Esteem in the Relationship Between Perfectionism and Competitive Anxiety Elite Athletes

The Relationships Among Competitiveness, Age and Ability In Distance Runners

Competitive orientations and motives of adult sport and exercise participants

CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY. Robin S. Vealey

A critical assessment of psychological skills training in sports RAVINDRA BALIRAM KHANDARE AND SUWRUDHESH HARIBHAU PADOLE

The effect of gender differences on the level of competitive state. anxiety and sport performance among Rowing Athletes

Successful football referees should have an excellent ability to cope

Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete

An exploration of pre-performance routines, self-efficacy, anxiety and performance in semiprofessional. Jonathan Hazell

Thought for the day. When you re through learning, you re through. -John Wooden

1.1 Problem statement 1.2 Objectives 1.3 Hypotheses 1.4 Structure of the Dissertation 1.5 References 1.1 PROBLEM STATEMENT

SELF EFFICACY AND INTRINSIC MOTIVATION IN SOUTH WEST ETHIOPIAN PUBLIC UNIVERSITIES MALE FOOTBALL TEAMS: A PSYCHOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE

Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment

Mindset For Optimal Performance: Essential Mental Skills DR. RICK MCGUIRE DIRECTOR OF SPORT PSYCHOLOGY ANNE SHADLE M.ED.

THE EFFECT OF COPING STRATEGIES TECHNIQUES ON RUNNING ATHLETES

Lim BH 1 & Balbir SG 2. Sports Centre, University of Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

Mental Skills Training Triathletes. Establishing a Sport Psychology Process

THE EFFECT OF COPING STRATEGIES TECHNIQUES ON TAEKWONDO ATHLETES

ANTECEDENTS OF FLOW AND THE FLOW-PERFORMANCE RELATIONSHIP IN CRICKET

Mental Skills Training for Athletes & Coaches: Developing Mental Plans

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note

Predicting swimming performance using state anxiety

A Model of Offensive and Defensive Mental Skills

Can Self-Doubt Be Beneficial to Performance? Exploring the Concept of Preparatory Efficacy

TSI Mental Plan Development as. Part of the Coaching Process. Starter Kit for (Mental) Performance Management. Guido Geisler, PhD

Psychology of Sport and Exercise

Archery: Emotion Intensity Regulation to Stay in the Zone during Olympic Competition

The Relationship between Competitive State Anxiety and Sport. Performance on Football Players

JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL ACADEMIC RESEARCH FOR MULTIDISCIPLINARY Impact Factor 1.393, ISSN: , Volume 2, Issue 2, March 2014

THE EFFECT OF COPING STRATEGIES TECHNIQUES ON SWIMMERS

DEVELOPMENT OF THE PSYCHOLOGICAL SKILLS INVENTORY FOR CHINESE ATHLETES. Xiaochun Yang. B.Sc, Wuhan Institute of Physical Education, 1993

Psychology and performance in sport. Dr. Jane Walsh

on motor performance Yannis Theodorakis Democritos University of Thrace F I N A L Manuscript submitted: June, 10, 1995

Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Canadian Positive Psychology Conference 2014 Successfully Infusing Positive Psychology into School Sporting Programs.

THE EFFECTS OF MENTAL TOUGHNESS TRAINING ON ATHLETIC COPING SKILLS AND SHOOTING EFFECTIVENESS FOR NATIONAL HANDBALL PLAYERS

Sport Psychology. QCE Physical Education (2019) ACHPER Conference 2018

A Comparison of Competitive-Orientation Measures

The Relationship between Mental Toughness and Affect Intensity. Lee Crust. The University of Hull

Self-Reported Psychological States and Physiological Responses to Different Types of Motivational General Imagery

THE ANXIETY LEVEL OF HOCKEY ATHLETES AS THE COMPETITION PROGRESSED. Mary Magdalene Teh. Abstract

Brazilian Journal of Biomotricity ISSN: Universidade Iguaçu Brasil

EFFECTS OF INTERVENTION UPON PRECOMPETITION STATE ANXIETY IN ELITE JUNIOR TENNIS PLAYERS: THE RELEVANCE OF THE MATCHING HYPOTHESIS '

Exploring the 4 W s of self talk within elite level fast bowlers: Where, When, What and Why?

The Continued Development of the Athletic Self- Appraisal Scale

Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity IMAGERY TRAINING INTERVENTION IN ACROBATIC GYMNASTS

IMPACT OF SELF-CONSCIOUSNESS ON CHOKING UNDER PRESSURE IN BASKETBALL PLAYERS

Effect of sports participation on locus of control and personality traits and self -efficacy among interuniversity sportsperson

Running head: USING EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN COACHING ATHLETES. Controlled Trial. Anna Barlow & Adrian P. Banks. University of Surrey, UK

Achievement goal orientations and the use of coping strategies among Winter Olympians

Pre-competitive levels of arousal and self-confidence among elite and non-elite equestrian riders

A COMPARISON OF MOVEMENT IMAGERY ABILITY AMONG UNDERGRADUATES SPORT STUDENTS

A comparative study of psychological skills in relation to performance of different level Indian female boxers

RELIABILITY OF TEST OF PERFORMANCE STRATEGIES- COMPETITION SCALE (TOPS-CS) AMONG YOUTH ATHLETES: A PRELIMINARY STUDY IN MALAYSIA

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 152 ( 2014 ) ERPA 2014

Running title: ATTRIBUTIONS AND EFFICACY EXPECTATIONS. Rees, T. (2007). Main and interactive effects of attribution dimensions upon

REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE

Young table tennis players involvement in the practice: a test of Eysenck and Calvo processing efficiency theory

Transcription:

August, 2002 Volume 4, Issue 2 Self-Efficacy And Psychological Skills During The Amputee Soccer World Cup James Lowther Wimbledon Football Club Selhurst Park Stadium and Andrew Lane & Helen Lane School of Sport, Performing Arts, and Leisure University of Wolverhampton ABSTRACT The present study explored relationships between psychological skills, self-efficacy, and performance among soccer players participating in the Amputee World Cup. Players completed the Test of Performance Strategies (Thomas, Hardy, & Nelson, 1999). One hour before competition, participants completed a two-item self-efficacy measure designed to assess confidence in attaining performance goals for each competition. Participants completed a two-item self-referenced performance measure within an hour of competition. Results indicated self-efficacy showed reciprocal relationships with performance. Several psychological skills related to self-efficacy and performance. Relaxation skills used in training and competition were associated with high self-efficacy and successful performance. Future research should investigate the effectiveness of applied sport psychology interventions designed to enhance self-efficacy through increasing the usage of psychological skills in training and competition Introduction It has been suggested that applied sport psychology interventions should be founded on a basis of theory and research (Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Murphy & Tammen, 1998; Lane & Terry, 2000). A great deal of research effort has concentrated on attempting to predict performance from psychological variables. Evidence showing relationships between psychological variables could be used to guide sport psychology 23

interventions, and it has been proposed that sport psychologists can enhance performance by enhancing psychological skills (Murphy & Tammen, 1998). The present study focuses on the relationship between rated psychological skills, self-efficacy measures, and perceptions of soccer performance among a soccer team playing at the Amputee World Cup. One variable shown to influence performance is self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997). Selfefficacy is defined as the beliefs of confidence individuals have in their ability to execute courses of action or attain specific performance outcomes (Bandura, 1977, 1982, 1997). Efficacy expectations are proposed to influence initiating behavior, how much effort will be applied to attain an outcome, and the level of persistence applied to the task in the face of difficulties and setbacks (Bandura, 1997). Examination of self-efficacy theory suggests that efficacy expectations are associated with successful performance (Bandura, 1997; Moritz, Feltz, Fahrbach, & Mack, 2000). Variations in self-efficacy-performance relationships have been ascribed to methodological inconsistencies between studies rather than challenging theoretical assumptions (Moritz et al., 2000). The way self-efficacy is measured has been found to influence the strength of self-efficacy-performance relationships. Moritz et al. (2000) demonstrated that weaker self-efficacy-performance relationships were evident when self-efficacy was measured using general measures of confidence such as the Competitive Sport Anxiety Inventory-2 (Martens, Vealey, & Burton, Bump, & Smith, 1990), which includes items such as "I feel calm' and 'I feel self-confident'. Stronger selfefficacy and performance relationships are evidenced when self-efficacy measures assess the same behaviors required in delivering the actual performance (Bandura, 1997). For example, how confident are you of scoring a penalty would be an appropriate selfefficacy measure for research investigating relationships between self-efficacy and penalty taking performance. Thus, it is suggested that researchers should seek to develop self-efficacy measures specific to the research environment. There are several possible ways sport psychologists can attempt to enhance selfefficacy. One approach is to manipulate one or more of the four principle sources proposed by Bandura (1997). Additionally, Schinke and da Costa (2001) described a process through efficacy-related information is likely to be garnered by athletes playing in major competition. Their review highlights the number of ways through which athletes can gain efficacy expectations. It should be noted that the experience of participating in major games is likely to increase knowledge of how to perform successfully in major competition, and if the individual feels he/she can cope with task demands, it should lead to increased self-efficacy. To compliment this learning process, sport psychologists encourage athletes to develop psychological skills. Sport psychologists tend to assess athlete's psychological skills as part of a needs analysis. Psychological skills are seen as the basis for developing interventions. For example, psychological skills can give an insight into how an individual will cope in competition, and thus increase self-efficacy. For example, individuals who report using relaxation skills should be able to use these skills to relax before competition. Recent research has suggested that the Test of Performance Strategies (TOPS; Thomas, Hardy, & Nelson, 1999) is the instrument of 24

choice. The TOPS assesses eight psychological skills used in training and eight skills used in competition. There are clear differences between training and competition and environments, and the TOPS can facilitate testing these differences. Developing psychological skills should lead to increased self-efficacy. Psychological skills assessed by the TOPS such as imagery and self-talk could be used to enhance selfefficacy provided the images and self-talk describe favorable performance accomplishments. Other psychological skills assessed by the TOPS include controlling negative emotions and relaxation skills. Emotional control and relaxation are skills that should assist controlling negative emotions. Bandura (1997) proposed that negative emotions could undermine self-efficacy. Additional factors assessed by the TOPS might also be associated with self-efficacy. The factor described as activation, which is characterized by the ability to increase energy might be a consequence or antecedent of self-efficacy. Likewise, automaticity, characterized by an autotelic state might be a consequence or antecedent of self-efficacy. It is acknowledged that neither automaticity nor activation fit neatly into the four sources of self-efficacy proposed by Bandura (1977). Psychological skills such as self-talk and imagery should relate with self-efficacy toward achieving performance related goals in soccer. Demonstration of links between psychological skills, self-efficacy, and performance should provide an empirical basis for applied sport psychology interventions. Further, empirically grounded interventions are preferable because they can be tested. The aim of the present study was to investigate relationships between self-efficacy and performance among a soccer team participating in the 2001 Amputee World Cup. Participants Method Participants were 15 male soccer players (Age range 19-28 years) in the England team at the 2000 Amputee World Cup. Players had an average of 11 years of playing experience (SD = 2.81 years) of soccer. On average, players had 2.4 years (SD = 4.84 years) of international experience. It should be noted that the limited number of national and international teams makes it difficult to gain competitive experience. Seven players had been playing internationally for 6 years. The Amputee World Cup was for male players only. Measures Psychological Skills. The TOPS is a 64-item measure of psychological skills. Exploratory factor analysis indicated an eight-factor solution for competition factors and an eight-factor solution for training factors. Factors are common to training and competition except negative thinking for competition and attentional control for training. Items were rated on a 5-point scale anchored by never (1) to always (5). The four-items were summed to produce factors scores, meaning that factor score could range from 5-20. 25

Self-efficacy. A key principle guiding how self-efficacy was measured was to ensure concordance between efficacy expectations and performance. A difficulty in developing such a measure was that soccer performance involves a number of different skills / competencies. For example, a defender's (there are usually four defenders) goal might be to prevent the opposing team from shooting or heading towards goal. By contrast, a forward's goal might be to shoot and score goals. We addressed this difficulty by asking each player what goal they had set for each game. These were individual goals for each player rather than team goals, although goals such a scoring goals will clearly help the team. Players were encouraged to set goals in which attainability depended on their performance rather than the team's performance. Examples of goals set included 'to play to my ability', 'to score a goal', 'to play well', 'to stay tight with defenders' and to give 100% effort'. Self-efficacy was assessed asking how confident each player was in attaining this goal, and individuals set a goal for each game. A second self-efficacy question asked each player to rate his confidence in the above prediction. Self-efficacy items were rated on a nine-point scale anchored by 'Extremely unconfident' (1) and 'Extremely confident' (9). Performance. Performance was assessed using a self-reference measure using two items, including "How do you feel about your performance in the last game?", and "To what extent did your performance relate to your pre-game expectations?' This is the same performance measure used by Lane and Chappell (2001) in a study of mood and performance relationships in basketball. Lane and Chappell (2001) reported an alpha coefficient of.87. Items were rated on a nine-point scale anchored by 'Extremely dissatisfied' (1) and 'Extremely pleased' (9). As goals were individually set, whether the team won, lost or drew games was considered of secondary performance. Alpha coefficient of internal consistency was.76, hence comparable with the alpha value reported by Lane and Chappell (2001). Procedure The first author, who was also the team's coach, contacted officials from the England team to gain permission to assess players in the team. As testing involved some disruption to the players normal preparation routines, permission was needed. On gaining permission from officials, players were asked to participate in the study. Players were informed of the purpose of the research. Informed consent was obtained. Players could either use their name or an identification number. It should be noted that physiological and psychological assessment of players had been done before, and thus players were aware of what was involved. Players were informed that assessments were not being used for selection purposes. As the coach was doing a dual role of coach and researcher, some explanation is warranted. A key issue is whether having a dual role has altered the data. Previous research has involved the members from the support team also collecting data for research purposes (Terry, 1993; 1994; 1995a; 1995b). Terry (1995a) outlines ways in which psychological data should be gathered to reduce the likelihood of participants reporting false responses. He emphasized the importance of informing players that 26

psychological data would not be used for selection purposes. In the present study, the coach did not select the team. The team did not have a sport psychologist as part of the support team. An acknowledged limitation of self-efficacy measures is leniency and bias. In the present study, it is not the absolute self-efficacy scores that are important, but how they vary in relation to psychological skills scores and performance. The TOPS (Thomas et al., 1999) was completed by players at least three days before the first game. Self-efficacy was assessed approximately 1 hour before competition. Selfrated performance was assessed within 1 hour after the competition. Results The team played six games. Descriptive statistics for self-efficacy, performance, and TOPS scores are contained in Table 1. As Table 1 indicates, self-efficacy scores tended to be stable ranging from 13.33 to 13.73 (F 5,70 = 1.90, p >.05, Eta-squared =.11). There was a greater range in performance scores although these did not differ significantly (F 5,70 = 2.14, p =.07, Eta-Squared =.13). Players reported significantly higher scores on competition factors than training factors for emotional control (t =5.05, p <.001), goal setting (t = 3.01, p <.01), relaxation (t = 6.41, p <.001), and activation (t = 4.41, p <.01). This finding is consistent with differences between training and competition psychological skills reported by Thomas et al. (1999). 27

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for self-efficacy and Test of Performance Strategies Scores N M SD Self-efficacy 1 15 13.33 2.06 Performance 1 15 10.67 3.64 Self-efficacy 2 15 13.73 2.40 Performance 2 15 12.73 4.23 Self-efficacy 3 15 13.60 2.87 Performance 3 15 13.00 3.14 Self-efficacy 4 15 13.13 2.00 Performance 4 15 13.20 2.15 Self-efficacy 5 15 14.13 2.64 Performance 5 15 13.40 2.39 Self-efficacy 6 15 15.27 1.62 Performance 6 15 13.53 2.07 Competition Self-Talk 15 11.53 3.29 Competition Emotional Control 15 9.47 2.83 Competition Automaticity 15 11.33 3.41 Competition Goal-Setting 15 15.60 3.68 Competition Imagery 15 13.33 3.33 Competition Activation 15 14.47 3.09 Competition Negative Thinking 15 11.00 2.33 Competition Relaxation 15 12.47 1.89 Training Goal-Setting 15 12.67 1.91 Training Emotional Control 15 12.40 1.68 Training Automaticity 15 12.53 2.10 Training Relaxation 15 8.00 3.14 Training Self-Talk 15 12.47 2.83 Training Imagery 15 11.47 3.80 Training Attentional Control 15 12.47 1.36 Training Activation 15 9.67 2.02 We examined self-efficacy and performance relationships on a game-by-game basis (see Figure 1). Results indicated that self-efficacy showed moderate correlation for each competition other than game 5. Performance showed moderate relationships with 28

subsequent self-efficacy measures, although the relationship was weak following game 3 and game 5. We combined self-efficacy and performance into single measures and investigated relationships between self-efficacy scores and performance, self-efficacy and TOPS scores, and TOPS scores and performance. Correlation results for self-efficacy and performance relationships indicated a significant association (r =.57, p <.01). This finding suggests that high self-efficacy scores were associated with successful performance. Relationships between TOPS and self-efficacy, and TOPS and performance are contained in Table 2. As Table 2 indicates, competition activation, competition and training relaxation, training automaticity, and training imagery showed the strongest relationships. Competition activation showed weak to moderate relationships with selfefficacy (r =.41) and performance (r =.38). The direction of the relationship indicated that successful performance and high self-efficacy was associated with the usage of activation skills. 29

Table 2 Correlations between TOPS scores and Self-efficacy scores Combined Self-efficacy Combined performance Competition Activation.41.38 Competition Automaticity.40.01 Competition Emotional Control -.13.07 Competition Goal -Setting.21.28 Competition Imagery.05.23 Competition Negative Thinking -.17 -.02 Competition Relaxation.57.35 Competition Self-Talk.29.10 Training Activation.02 -.03 Training Attentional Control.25.61 Training Automaticity.41.31 Training Emotional Control.04.19 Training Goal Setting.07.33 Training Imagery.42.69 Training Relaxation.38.44 Training Self-Talk -.11.03 Competition relaxation related to self-efficacy (r =.41) and performance (r =.35), whereby the more players used relaxation skills in competition, the higher the selfefficacy scores, and better the performance scores. Training automaticity related to selfefficacy (r =.41) and training automaticity moderately related to performance (r =.31). High self-efficacy and successful performance was associated with the ability to perform automatically. Training imagery correlated with self-efficacy (r =.42) and training imagery correlated with performance (r =.69). Relationships indicated that using imagery was associated with high self-efficacy and successful performance. Training Relaxation correlated with self-efficacy (r =.38) and performance (r =.44), whereby using relaxation skills in training was associated with high self-efficacy and successful performance. Discussion The purpose of the present study was to investigate relationships between psychological skills, self-efficacy, and performance. Identification of variables that relate with performance can provide applied sport psychologists with valuable information. Psychological skills are seen as the backbone for an applied sport psychology intervention (Murphy & Tammen, 1998; Thomas et al., 1999). For example, imagery 30

skills can be used to enhance self-efficacy, reduce anxiety and relax. In the present study, we focused on relationships between psychological skills, self-efficacy and performance. If sport psychologists are to try to enhance self-efficacy, measures of self-efficacy should relate with performance. Results of the present study demonstrated a moderate relationship between self-efficacy and performance. This finding is consistent with metaanalysis results of Moritz et al. (2001), and lends further support to the predictive effectiveness of self-efficacy in sport (Bandura, 1997). Previous research has shown that self-efficacy-performance relationships to be strongest when the individual has a clear understanding of task demands (Bandura, 1997). When the task is difficult to assess, participants will struggle to give accurate estimates of confidence to perform successfully. Soccer is an open-skilled sport involving 11-players co-acting with the aim to score more goals than the opposition. In the present study, soccer players were experienced and therefore were likely to have a relatively clear understanding of the demands of playing soccer. Schinke and da Costa (2001) outlined the importance of experience of playing in major games in developing self-efficacy. However, despite knowledge of the demands of soccer, the player will not know how well the opposing team will perform. Thus, it is possible for a player to be able to accurately estimate how well he will play, but can only guess how well the opposition might play. We suggest that as the players were experienced, and had a relatively thorough knowledge of task difficulties, this might have influenced the strength of selfefficacy and performance relationships and weaker relationships might have occurred if players were inexperienced. It should be noted that findings from the present study derive from a very small sample size. Results generalize to players from one team over time, and these findings might not generalize to other teams. Findings showing TOPS scores related to self-efficacy and performance could be used to devise sport psychology interventions. Competition activation scores related to both self-efficacy and performance. A similar finding was shown for relaxation skills, but in the case of relaxation, scores for relaxation in competition and training setting related to both performance and self-efficacy. The ability to control negative emotions through activating arousal or relaxing is consistent with theoretical proposals (Bandura, 1997). Further relationships indicated that imagery skills used in training were associated with relaxation skills used in competition and in training. A limitation of imagery usage as assessed by the TOPS is that the type of imagery is not explored. Thus, it is not known whether players used internal imagery, external imagery, or kinesthetic imagery. Thus, interventions that encourage using imagery during training and teaching athletes relaxation skills should lead to improved psychological skills, self-efficacy, and performance. It should be noted that self-efficacy expectations are likely to developed as the competition progresses (Schinke & da Costa, 2001). Factors such as developing experience of playing in different venues, learning the strengths and weaknesses of the opposition are likely to increase efficacy expectations. Thus, psychological skills are likely to account for a relatively minor proportion of self-efficacy variance. Given the 31

proposals of Schinke and da Costa (2001) findings showing TOPS scores related consistently to self-efficacy findings throughout the course of the competition suggest that psychological skills strongly relate to self-efficacy. The present study had at least two limitations that should be considered when interpreting the results. First, the small sample size makes it difficult to generalize findings from the present study to a wider audience. Using a probability value of <.10 and conducting multiple tests clearly increases the likelihood of finding significance when there is none. Results showing effect sizes consistent with theoretical predictions offer some support for theory and empirical evidence. However, some results are difficult to explain. For example, successful performance was associated with low self-efficacy following the second game. We suggest that this result is an anomaly. It does serve to question whether other results are equally anomalies. Future research should investigate the reproducibility of these findings to a different sample and larger sample. Research should also extend this line of investigation to other sports. Second, the self-efficacy measure comprised only two items. From a psychometric perspective, a valid measure should assess the full range of behaviors relevant to the target construct. Attaining self-set goals will invariably mean that players would need to demonstrate competence in a number of different behaviors. Future research should involve a thorough investigation of the competencies underpinning performance in soccer. Identification of such strategies could be used to develop a valid measure of selfefficacy for use in soccer. In conclusion, the present study explored relationships between psychological skills scores for training and competition, self-efficacy, and performance. Results show that self-efficacy showed reciprocal relationships with performance. Activation and relaxation influenced self-efficacy and performance, thus interventions to enhance performance could involve teaching soccer player relaxation skills in competition and training. 32

References Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-147. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman. Hardy, L., Jones, J. G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding psychological preparation for sport; theory and practice of elite performers. Wiley, Chichester. Lane, A. M., & Terry, P. C. (2000). The nature of mood: Development of a conceptual model with a focus on depression. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 16-33. Lane, A. M., & Chappell, R. H. (2001). Mood and performance relationships at the World Student Games basketball competition. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24, 182-196. Martens, R., Vealey, R. S., Burton, D., Bump, L., & Smith, D. E. (1990). Development and validation of the Competitive Sports Anxiety Inventory-2. In R. Martens, R. S. Vealey, & D. Burton (Eds.), Competitive anxiety in sport. (pp. 117-178). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Moritz, S.E., Feltz, D.L., Fahrbach, K.R., & Mack, D.E. (2000). The relation of selfefficacy measures to sport performance: A meta-analytic review. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 280-294. Murphy, S., & Tammen, V. (1998). In search of psychological skills. In Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (edited by J.L. Duda), pp. 195-209. Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. Schinke, R. J. & da Costa, J. L. (2001). A plausible relationship between support infrastructure and major games performance. Athletic Insight (On-Line), 3. Available: http://www.athleticinsight.com/vol3iss2/supportinfrastructure.htm. Terry, P. C. (1993). Mood state profile as indicators of performance among Olympic and World Championship athletes. In S. Serpa, J. Alves, V. Ferreira, & A. Paulo-Brito (Eds.) Proceeding of the VIIIth ISSP World Congress of Sport Psychology. pp. 963-967. Lisbon: ISSP. Terry, P. C. (1994). Preperformance mood profiles of the England cricket team during the 1993 "Ashes" series. Paper presented at the 10th Commonwealth and International Scientific Congress "Access to Active Living," Victoria, BC, Canada. 33

Terry, P. C. (1995a). The efficacy of mood state profiling among elite competitors: a review and synthesis. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 309-324. Terry, P. C. (1995b). Discriminant capability of preperformance mood state profiles during the 1993-1994 World Cup Bobsleigh. Journal of Sports Sciences, 13, 77-78. Thomas, P. R., Murphy, S. M., & Hardy, L. (1999). Test of performance strategies: Development and preliminary validation of a comprehensive of athletes' psychological skills. Journal of Sports Sciences, 17, 697-711. 34