Brain Activity and Attention Switching

Similar documents
Easy Read report about the Team Up Evaluation

Our plan for making the lives of people with autism and their families and carers better

Meeting a Kid with Autism

Information on ADHD for Children, Question and Answer - long version

Overview. Meeting Length 90 minutes. Senses [Meeting 1]

Building Friendships: Avoid Discounting

Fruits and Vegetables 2 nd grade lesson plan

Going to the Dentist. Expressing Obligation and Necessity

BBC LEARNING ENGLISH 6 Minute English Buttons

Nacro Housing Review

ORIENTATION SAN FRANCISCO STOP SMOKING PROGRAM

Otitis Media and Conductive Hearing Loss By Damien Howard PhD

Time for Change. The Challenge Ahead

CBT+ Measures Cheat Sheet

ENABLE Scotland. Annan ACE. Annual Report 2017

Co-Diagnosis is changing dentistry

keep track of other information like warning discuss with your doctor, and numbers of signs for relapse, things you want to

What is Down syndrome?

Script: Placebo Effect Matt Biggers, Amelia Hulbert, Kathryn Valchar

Attention and Concentration Problems Following Traumatic Brain Injury. Patient Information Booklet. Talis Consulting Limited

Preparing for your Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)

London. London. Last year, as Speaking Up and Advocacy Partners, we:

ENABLE Scotland. Elgin ACE. Annual Report 2017

Controlling Worries and Habits

Quick guide to autism

Mindfulness for living well with a LTC

If it works for children with autism it will work for many others.

ENABLE Scotland. Inverness ACE. Annual Report 2017

Improving Your Sleep Course. Session 4 Dealing With a Racing Mind

2013 JadaCastellari.com all rights reserved

Next Level Practitioner

Discussion Starter: Autism Awareness. A mini-reader & Lesson Ideas Created by: Primarily AU-Some 2013 & 2014

Brainlab Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) for Pediatric Patients

Intro to Diabetes J. MacLaren 09/2009

IT S ALL IN YOUR MIND

ENABLE Scotland. East Dunbartonshire ACE Youth. Annual Report 2017

ENABLE Scotland. Edinburgh ACE. Annual Report 2017

Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions

ENABLE Scotland. Glasgow ACE. Annual Report 2017

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

Speech Spatial Qualities -C

Reducing Social Threats

Vulnerable Adults Housing & Well-Being Support. Easy Read Version Consultation Questionnaire

An Interview with a Chiropractor

POsitive mental health for young people. What you need to know about Children and Adolescent s Mental Health Services (CAMHS) in Buckinghamshire

Speech Spatial Qualities

ENABLE Scotland. Perth ACE. Annual Report 2017

We have a new service in Lincolnshire called Appropriate Adults

100 TOEIC GRAMMAR QUESTIONS. by Jeffrey Hill

THE INTEGRITY PROFILING SYSTEM

DOCTOR: The last time I saw you and your 6-year old son Julio was about 2 months ago?

What makes us special? Ages 3-5

Listen to Arshia talking to Dr. Davis. Fill in the missing words. Dr. Davis: All, right we re all set, you can get.

Mental Health Strategy. Easy Read

National Inspection of services that support looked after children and care leavers

Managing conversations around mental health. Blue Light Programme mind.org.uk/bluelight

What You Need to Know About. TB Infection TUBERCULOSIS

Hello My Name Is Primary Health NW

Minutes from the meeting on. Present People who came to the meeting. Leicestershire County Council. Personal Assistant. Notes of the last meeting

CHARTER FOR INVOLVEMENT. National Involvement Network

PLACEMENT & DIAGNOSTIC TEST

HOW DO YOU REALLY KEEP YOUR KIDS SAFE FROM ADDICTION?

Draft 0-25 special educational needs (SEN) Code of Practice: young disabled people s views

The 5 Things You Can Do Right Now to Get Ready to Quit Smoking

Living a Healthy Balanced Life Emotional Balance By Ellen Missah

I AM GOING ON A KETOGENIC DIET. What is a Ketogenic Diet and why do I need to go on it? Written by Emma Williams MBE CEO/Founder Matthew s Friends

Education. Patient. Century. in the21 st. By Robert Braile, DC, FICA

Challenging conversations in the Emergency Department

Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions

Einstufungsbogen. Beginner. 1. I from Italy. a) is b) are c) am d) be. 2. This is my friend. name is Peter. a) Her b) Our c) Yours d) His. 3. Paul is.

Teacher Lesson Plan Let s Learn about Allergies Kindergarten. Respecting Differences. Getting an Adult for Help

Prueba de nivel. 1. I from France. a) is b) are c) am d) be. 2. This is my friend. name is Peter. a) Her b) Our c) Yours d) His

You will also be required to meet with your Mentor to workshop and implement feedback for approximately 6 hours during the Shadowing process.

Living well today...32 Hope for tomorrow...32

ADHD. What you need to know

The Cinderella Hypothesis

The Role of the Physician Leader in Emergency Medicine

The brain and behaviour

Emotional Changes After a Traumatic Brain Injury

DEPRESSION. Teenage. Parent s Guide to

Co-Production Agreement for Health and Social Care in Hackney and the City of London

Building A World-Class WELLNESS PROGRAM

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT

Disclosing medical errors to patients: Recent developments and future directions

Good Communication Starts at Home

Making Connections: Early Detection Hearing and Intervention through the Medical Home Model Podcast Series

An Introduction to Huntington s Disease

Lesson #T5A: Amygdala the Emotional Elephant FOR GROUPS

For young people living with someone s excessive drinking, drug use or addiction

The Recovery Journey after a PICU admission

A report about. Anxiety. Easy Read summary

Mental Wellbeing in Norfolk and Waveney

What is primary progressive aphasia?

ENABLE Scotland. Kilmarnock ACE. Annual Report 2017

DEAF CHILDREN WITH ADHD AND LEARNING DIFFICULTIES

Upper and Lower RPD. Increasing VDO? Please Help. Photos in Thread.

Healthy Mind Healthy Weight

My Review of John Barban s Venus Factor (2015 Update and Bonus)

Participant Information Sheet

Letter to the teachers

Transcription:

Brain Activity and Attention Switching Kate Woodcock, Chris Oliver and Glyn Humphreys Cerebra Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham The Research described in this report was funded by the Medical Research Council The Cerebra Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT Website: www.cndd.bham.ac.uk E-mail: cndd-enquiries@contacts.bham.ac.uk 1 of 10

BRAIN ACTIVITY AND ATTENTION SWITCHING FEEDBACK REPORT On DATE PARTICIPANT took part in a research study on brain activity and attention switching at the Cerebra Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders at the University of Birmingham. In the study we aimed to look at how the brain works to allow people to switch their attention (changing from thinking about something in one way to thinking about the same thing in a different way) and how this might be different between people with Prader-Willi syndrome and people without the syndrome. Why was it important for us to look at how the brain works during attention switching in people with Prader-Willi syndrome? People with Prader-Willi syndrome can often be very resistant to changes to routines, expectations or plans. People can also show a lot of temper outbursts (sometimes called tantrums ) that seem to be very difficult for the people with the syndrome to control. In our previous research we found that one of the times at which people with Prader-Willi syndrome can be very likely to show temper outbursts is when unexpected changes happen (e.g. changes to routines or to what the person was expecting). In other research we have carried out we found that the resistance to changes to routines, expectations or plans that people with Prader-Willi syndrome often showed was linked to their ability to switch attention. So, people with Prader- Willi syndrome generally found it difficult to switch their attention, and the more difficult they found it, the more resistance to change they showed. This means that attention switching seems to be closely linked with some very difficult behaviours in people with Prader-Willi syndrome. By looking at how the brains of people with Prader-Willi work when they have to switch their attention we might be able to understand why they find attention switching so difficult. This could help us to think of ways to make it easier for people to switch their attention. 2 of 10

What is attention switching? Attention switching is changing from thinking about something in one way (that might lead to acting in a particular way) to thinking about the same thing in a different way (and then acting in a different way). Switch I need to eat the spaghetti I can use the fork to eat the spaghetti I need to make an omelette I can use the fork to whisk the eggs Here is an example of attention switching: 1) you see a fork next to a plate of food that you want to eat 2) you think about the fork as a tool for eating 3) you eat the food with the fork 4) you finish eating and wash the fork 5) you need to make an omelette 6) SWITCH 7) you think about the fork as a tool with prongs that can work like a whisk 8) you whisk the eggs with the omelette How would difficulties with attention switching be linked to resistance to changes? Changes to routines or to expectations can force people to switch their attention and so place a high demand on people s attention switching ability. The school bus is here. Tom drives the bus. I will say Hello Tom and he will say Good morning Lucy. You are used to catching the same bus everyday. The bus is yellow and has a driver called Tom who wears a hat. You know that when the bus comes you will say Hello Tom and Tom will answer Good morning. CHANGE CHANGE A bus arrives, but Tom is not driving. Sarah is driving the bus. I can t say Hello Tom Tom won t say Good morning Lucy. You must change from thinking about the bus as the bus that Tom drives to thinking about the bus with a different driver. You must switch your attention to be able to act in the right way and greet Sarah. 3 of 10

How did we look at attention switching? To look at how people switch their attention we asked participants to do a computer task that involved a lot of attention switching. We showed participants a screen like one of these: Picture of the buttons that participants were asked to press in response to the pictures When peoplearticipants were shown a screen with the word WHAT? above the picture, we asked them to press the button that showed the same picture as the one they could see. So in the above left side picture participants would have had to press the red circle (left side) button. When participants were shown a screen with the word WHERE above the picture, we asked them to press the button that was on the same side as the picture. So in the above right side picture participants would have had to press the left side (red circle) button. This means that when the screen showed WHAT? for one button press and then WHERE for the next, participants had to switch their attention. They had to change from thinking about the picture as a red circle or a blue square, to thinking about the picture as a thing on one side of the screen or the other. How did we look at how people s brains work when they are switching their attention? To look at how peoples brains work when they are switching their attention we used a technique called functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) to take hundreds of photographs of peoples brains while they were doing the attention switching task. Peoples brains contain millions of brain cells (neurons). Different areas of the brain contain different types of brain cells that are designed to do particular jobs. This means that when people are doing different things, the cells in some areas of the brain have to do a lot of work, while the cells in other areas of the brain have to do much less. When brain cells work they need more energy and this causes changes to the blood flow to those brain cells. 4 of 10

We used a magnetic resonance imaging machine to take photographs of the blood flow to all the different parts of the brain while participants were doing the attention switching task. This helped to show us how much work different areas of the brain were doing while the participants were having to switch their attention. We then compared these photographs to ones which we took while participants were not having to switch their attention (they were doing a similar task but the task didn t involve any attention switching). This showed us which parts of the brain were particularly involved in attention switching rather than in anything else that participants may have been doing at the same time (e.g. moving their eyes, reading WHAT? Or WHERE etc.) When we talk about the photographs that we took of people s brains we really mean images that the magnetic resonance imaging machine creates by measuring magnetic signals that the brain gives out when it is in the machine. Parts of the brain that are doing different amounts of work ( and so have different blood flow) give out magnetic signals of different strengths just as different objects that you photograph with a camera are different colours because of the different amounts of light that they reflect. What did we find out? First we looked at the participants who do not have Prader-Willi syndrome. By comparing the images that we took while people were attention switching to those that we took while people were not switching, we created new images that showed us which brain areas were working particularly hard to allow people to carry out attention switching rather than anything else. We called these brain areas switching brain areas. Because everyone s brain is slightly different it was important for us to look at everyone s brain together. We created an average image of everyone s switching brain areas to show us which brain areas are really important for switching no matter which individual characteristics one person s brain might have. The pictures on the next page show the switching brain areas averaged across the eight participants that visited us who did not have Prader-Willi syndrome. We found three different switching brain areas that were important: 1) An area stretching from the left to the right side of the brain in a place we call the parietal lobes. 2) An area stretching from the left to the right side of the brain including a place we call the anterior cingulate. 3) An area at the very front of the brain on the left hand side in a place we call the frontopolar cortex. 5 of 10

From research that other people have carried out before us we know that these brain areas match with the areas that are normally found to be important when people are switching their attention. 6 of 10

Switching brain areas averaged across the eight participants who did not have Prader-Willi syndrome Looking at the brain from the right hand side of the person Looking at the brain from above the person s head Looking at the brain from behind the person, but as if the back part of the brain were transparent so we can see the inside of the brain as if it were sliced through the middle from ear to ear 7 of 10

Next we looked at all of the people who had Prader-Willi syndrome. We looked at each of the switching brain areas that were important in the people who did not have Prader-Willi syndrome. We wanted to see if the same areas were important for attention switching in people with the syndrome. Which switching brain areas, important in people without Prader- Willi syndrome, are also important for attention switching in people with the syndrome? Looking at the brain from the right hand side of the person Here we can see that the parietal lobes did not work hard during switching (x) in people with Prader-Willi syndrome, but the frontopoloar cortex did ( ). Looking at the brain from above the person s head Here we can see that as well as the parietal lobes not working hard during switching in people with Prader-Willi syndrome (x), neither did the anterior cingulate (x). Looking at the brain from behind the person Here we can see again that the anterior cingulate did not work hard during switching in people with Prader-Willi syndrome (x), but the frontopolar cortex did ( ). 8 of 10

So only one of the three typical switching brain areas (brain areas that worked particularly hard during switching in people without Prader-Willi syndrome) worked hard during attention switching in people with Prader-Willi syndrome. This area was the area at the very front of the brain in the frontopolar cortex. Finally, we wanted to see if there were any brain areas that worked harder during attention switching in people with Prader-Willi syndrome than in people without the syndrome. To do this we created images that showed us any areas where two things happened at the same time: 1) the area worked particularly hard when people were switching (it was a switching brain area ) 2) the area worked harder in people with Prader-Willi syndrome than in people without the syndrome Which brain areas work harder when people with Prader-Willi syndrome are attention switching than when people without the syndrome are switching? Looking at the brain from the right hand side of the person We can see that an area right at the front of the brain in the frontopolar cortex worked harder during switching in people with Prader- Willi syndrome than in people without the syndrome. Looking at the brain from above the person s head Here we can see the same area in the frontalpolar cortex. Looking at the brain from behind the person, but as if the back part of the brain were transparent so we can see the inside of the brain as if it were sliced through the middle from ear to ear Here we can see the same area in the frontalpolar cortex. So an area at the very front of the brain (in the region known as the frontopolar cortex) worked harder during attention switching in people with Prader-Willi syndrome than in people without the syndrome. 9 of 10

What does this mean? When people with Prader-Willi syndrome switch their attention they use different areas of their brain to people who do not have the syndrome. Brain areas known as the parietal lobes and the anterior cingulate are usually important when people are switching their attention. But these areas are not used for switching by people with Prader-Willi syndrome. These brain areas are the ones that are normally responsible for changing the priority that the brain gives to paying attention to different aspects of objects and events, so these areas actually let an attention switch happen. So, because of how the brain has developed in people with Prader-Willi syndrome, the brain does not seem to work in the right way to let attention switching happen as it normally should. This may be why people with the syndrome find it very difficult to switch their attention. So when people with Prader-Willi syndrome are forced to switch their attention because of changes to routines or expectations (e.g. if the bus driver changes from Tom to Sarah), their brain does not work in the right way to let them carry out the switch easily (e.g. they would find it very difficult to greet Sarah instead of Tom). A brain area known as the frontopolar cortex actually works harder when people with Prader-Willi syndrome are switching their attention than when people without the syndrome are switching. This area of the brain may be responsible for letting people know that a switch has happened. So the brains of people with Prader-Willi syndrome seem able to tell them when a change has happened that would need them to switch their attention, but their brains are less able to deal with this change and actually carry out the switch. Having said this, people with Prader-Willi syndrome can switch their attention even using completely different parts of the brain to those normally needed to carry out switching. It might be that people with Prader-Willi syndrome have been able to develop strategies to help them to switch their attention even when some parts of their brains that should be responsible for switching do not work properly (this might be why other parts of the brain e.g. the frontopolar cortex work harder). This is promising because it means that in future if we can find out which parts of the brains of people with Prader-Willi syndrome work particularly well then we might be able to work out how to teach these areas to compensate for the areas that do not work as well. So, these results tell us that people with Prader-Willi syndrome seem to have a problem with the way particular parts of their brains work that is responsible for them not being able to switch their attention well. When changes to routines or expectations force people with the syndrome to switch their attention it is not through stubbornness or disobedience that people do not want the change to happen. There is a physical problem with the hardwiring of people s brains that causes switching, and so unexpected changes, to be extremely difficult. 10 of 10