Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions

Similar documents
Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

A Structured Interview for the Assessment of the Five-Factor Model of Personality: Facet-Level Relations to the Axis II Personality Disorders

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Client Personality and Preference for Counseling Approach: Does Match Matter?

The Doctrine of Traits. Lecture 29

Rosenthal, Montoya, Ridings, Rieck, and Hooley (2011) Appendix A. Supplementary material

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI

The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

Traits & Trait Taxonomies

The five-factor personality structure of dissociative experiences

Personality: Definitions

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

The Difference Analysis between Demographic Variables and Personal Attributes The Case of Internal Auditors in Taiwan

Multiple Act criterion:

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3 Similar observations in all subsets of the disorder. Personality Disorders. General Symptoms. Chapter 9

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

To link to this article:

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items

Glasgow, 1055 Great Western Road, Glasgow G12 0XH, UK

The relationship between personality and performance of football referees

Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties

The relationship of five personality factors with the feeling of happiness among students

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

9 - SCREENING MEASURES FOR PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Understanding the coach-athlete relationship from a cross-cultural perspective

They re Not Abnormal and We re Not Making Them Abnormal : A Longitudinal Study

Relationship between Teachers' Personality Traits and Self Efficacy: An Empirical Analysis of School Teachers in Karaikal Region (Puducherry)

Encyclopedia of Counseling Personality Theories, Traits

Chapter 8: Regression

Similarity and Assumed Similarity in Personality Reports of Well-Acquainted Persons

AN EMPIRICAL RESEARCH ON ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS: STUDENTS PERCEPTIONS

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method

ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN SELF-RATED HEALTH AND PERSONALITY

M.A. Alhad & S.S. Turnip Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia

Relationships Between Personality, Computer Stereotypes and Preference. Melissa Bagley, Stephanie Moulton, Evan Rubin, Deborah Ward

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students

Personality Traits and Labour Economics

Journal of Research in Personality 33, (1999) Article ID jrpe , available online at on BRIEF REPORT

Personality Disorders. Mark Kimsey, M.D. March 8, 2014

Personality traits predict current and future functioning comparably for individuals with major depressive and personality disorders

College Smartphone Dependency: Relationships Between Social- Emotional Well-Being and Personality

Matters. Assessment & Development. Vol. 1 No. 3. Autumn 2009 ISSN Official Publication of The Psychological Testing Centre

Flux, Pulse, and Spin: Dynamic Additions to the Personality Lexicon

Evidence of Differential Meta-Accuracy People Understand the Different Impressions They Make

Affect, Behavior, Cognition, and Desire in the Big Five: An Analysis of Item Content and Structure

The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors

Altar Working Systems and Strategies. Pastor: Ball

Naorem Binita Devi Faculty member, department of psychology, Mzu, Tanhril, Aizawl

Preliminary Conclusion

Five Factor Model Prototype Matching Scores: Convergence Within Alternative Methods

Commentary: The role of intrapersonal psychological variables in academic school learning

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT

Report on the Ontario Principals Council Leadership Study. Executive Summary

Author's personal copy

CHAPTER 2 Personality Traits: A Good Theory

Prediction of Attitudes Towards Narcotics and Prediction of Increased Risk for Depression in Recruits with New Psychological Tools

BODY IMAGE IN DANCERS

Personality characteristics:

The Big Five and HEXACO questionnaires are full of affect and so, probably, are your questionnaires

Running head: ATTACHMENT STYLES AND DIFFERENT PERSONALITY TRAITS

Evaluating Circumplex Structure in the Interpersonal Scales for the NEO-PI-3

Chiba University AKIO WAKABAYASHI 1 *

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND STRESS MANAGEMENT

RATING MENTAL WHOLE PERSON IMPAIRMENT UNDER THE NEW SABS: New Methods, New Challenges. CSME/CAPDA Conference, April 1, 2017

Impulsivity is Important

Investigating the relationship between assertiveness and personality characteristics

EXAMINING MODERATORS OF META-ACCURACY AMONG PARTICIPANT DYADS NATHANIEL L. MARINO. A thesis submitted to the. Graduate School-Camden

Research Report for Adaptive Testing Assessment

Pride. Jessica L. Tracy. University of British Columbia

The State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI)

An application of belief-importance theory with reference to the Big Five and trait emotional intelligence

The Relationship between Personality Styles and Academic Self- Efficacy among Jordanian EFL Learners

Halesworth & District. Malcolm Ballantine

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Big Five FFM Five-Factor Model. 1 Neuroticism 3 (Openness to experience) O + O - 4 (Agreeableness) 5 (Conscientiousness) N E O A C

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Personality traits and Internet usage across generation cohorts: Insights from a nationally representative study

Facial symmetry and the Ôbig-fiveÕ personality factors

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note

IPT West Midlands. Dr Marie Wardle Programme Director

Everything DiSC 363 for Leaders. Research Report. by Inscape Publishing

INFLUENCE OF PERSONALITY ON CONDUCT DISORDER AMONG SECONDARY SCHOOL ADOLESCENTS

The Relation Between Happiness and Number of Siblings. Michael W. Passer. University of Washington

A STUDY OF PERSONALITY CHARACTERISTICS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL DISTRESS AMONG NURSING STUDENTS

The HEXACO Personality Inventory and Academic Performance in MIS Course

The Effects of Uncertainty on Interpersonal Relations. in Terms of Prolonged Satisfaction Ratings. John Smith. Roanoke College

Psychology in Your Life

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT RESEARCH AND REVIEW

Meta-analysis of the Relationship between the Five-Factor Model of Personality. and Holland s Occupational Types

Personality means how a person affects others and how he understands and views himself as well as the pattern of inner and outer measurable traits,

Trait Approaches to Personality

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Personality and Conflict Resolution Styles

Transcription:

1 British Journal of Medical Psychology (2004), 00, 1 6 q 2004 The British Psychological Society www.bps.org.uk Brief report Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions Dionyssios Caralis 1 and Nick Haslam 2 * 1 New School University, Australia 2 University of Melbourne, Australia Out-patients (N ¼ 76) completed measures of interpersonal motives and construals derived from relational models theory and a five-factor model personality questionnaire. A broader range of associations between personality dimensions and relational tendencies was obtained than interpersonal circle theorists propose, with dimensions other than extraversion and agreeableness having distinctive relational correlates. The interpersonal domain of personality may therefore be somewhat broader than many mainstream personality theorists suppose. We expect people with different personalities to approach their relationships in distinctive ways. However, little research has systematically addressed links between interpersonal tendencies and broad personality dimensions such as the Five-Factor Model (FFM; Costa & McCrae, 1992). These links have been explored chiefly through the Interpersonal Circle ( IC; Kiesler, 1983). Researchers have established that extraversion is associated with the IC s dominance/submission axis, and agreeableness with its warmth/coldness axis (Soldz, Budman, Demby, & Merry, 1993). Accordingly, FFM and IC theorists have usually defined the interpersonal domain by only two of the five primary dimensions of personality. Although they might allow that the three additional dimensions neuroticism, openness and conscientiousness might have a bearing on interpersonal behaviour, they have conceptualized them in asocial ways (i.e. as negative emotionality, cognitive flexibility and self-restraint) and neglected their interpersonal implications. Thus, Gurtman (1991) argues that if a personality trait fails to correlate with the IC s two axes it is non-interpersonal. * Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Nick Haslam, Department of Psychology, University of Melbourne, Portaille, Victoria 3010, Australia (e-mail: optiknew.com).

2 Dionyssios Caralis and Nick Haslam This delimitation of the interpersonal domain can be called into question. Personality dimensions beyond extraversion and agreeableness might be associated with distinctive interpersonal tendencies, and these might not be fully captured by the IC. Although extraversion, agreeableness and their corresponding IC axes certainly represent important coordinates of interpersonal life, it is possible that they can be supplemented by additional dimensions for a fuller appreciation of interpersonal tendencies. It has recently been argued (Haslam, Reichert, & Fiske, 2002), for example, that the IC does not capture some of the emergent properties of relationships (e.g. inequality, authority relations, communality). Tendencies to construe relationships in terms of these properties and to be motivated to engage in them such as to perceive one s relationships in egalitarian terms or to seek out hierarchical relationships cannot be located on the IC. Indeed, such relational tendencies were found to be only weakly associated with the IC axes, and predicted personality disorder (PD) symptoms independently of them (Haslam et al., 2002). One approach to understanding relational tendencies is afforded by Relational Models Theory (RMT). RMT (Fiske, 1991) was developed out of a synthesis of social theories and ethnographic fieldwork in west Africa, and takes a more explicitly cognitive and cultural approach to sociality than most interpersonal theories employed in clinical psychology. The theory proposes that there are four fundamental models that people use to interpret, construct and evaluate relationships. These models are understood to be universal cognitive structures with which people coordinate their interpersonal lives, according to implementation rules and parameters supplied by their local cultures. Communal sharing (CS) relationships are based on an understanding that relational partners are equivalent and undifferentiated and have a shared identity, and are common within romantic couples, families and sporting teams. Equality matching (EM) relationships, which are common among friends, are based on a sense of egalitarian balance: people keep track of favours and obligations and maintain balance by in-kind reciprocity, equal divisions of labour, and turn-taking. Authority ranking (AR) relationships, common in hierarchical organizations and between parents and children, are based on status asymmetries: one person leads and takes precedence and the other follows and defers. Market pricing (MP) relationships follow a principle of proportional equity, each person seeking to gain a suitable rate of return for their investment of time, money or effort, and are common within work groups. This RMT taxonomy has received extensive support as a model of people s cognition about relationships ( Fiske & Haslam, 1996; Haslam, 1994), and has stimulated research across the social and behavioural sciences (Haslam, 2004). To date, individual differences in the implementation of the relational models (RMs) have received little attention, and have only been linked to abnormal personality. Haslam et al. (2002) showed that people who had unusually strong motives to engage in relationships of each type, or who construed their personal relationships in terms of each RM to an unusual extent, tended to display distinctive patterns of PD symptoms (e.g. paranoid individuals tended to have relative strong motives for MP relationships and tended not to construe relationships in CS terms). In the present study, we therefore aimed to extend the earlier study by assessing the associations of RM-based relational

Relational models 3 tendencies with dimensions of normal (i.e. non-pathological) personality. We predicted that relational tendencies would be associated with personality dimensions beyond extraversion and agreeableness, supporting the claim that the interpersonal domain of personality is broader than these factors. More specifically, we hypothesized that extraversion and agreeableness would correlate with tendencies to engage in close (CS and EM) relationships. We further predicted that neuroticism would be associated with tendencies to engage in AR relationships, given the generalized association between AR and PD symptoms found by Haslam et al. (2002). Finally, we predicted that Openness would be associated with egalitarian (EM) tendencies. Method Participants Participants were 76 psychiatric out-patients (43 women, 33 men; mean age ¼ 42:9 yrs) receiving treatment at two New York area clinics. The sample was multi-ethnic and most had primary diagnoses of mood or anxiety disorders. Participants were excluded if a brief screening showed evidence of organic brain disturbance or significant cognitive impairment. Materials NEO-PI R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) This 240-item self-report inventory is the most widely used FFM measure, and has strong evidence of reliability and validity. Modes of Relationship Questionnaire (MORQ; Haslam & Fiske, 1999) The MORQ assesses people s relative tendencies to construe a sample of their personal relationships in terms of the four RMs. It was implemented in similar fashion as in Haslam et al. (2002). Participants freely listed 20 acquaintances and then rated their relationship with every second acquaintance on a 20-item version of the original 32-item measure, using the five highest-loading items for each RM scale. Mean scale scores across the 10 rated relationships were then calculated. Because the MORQ is a measure of a person s preference for each RM relative to the others, these mean scores were ipsatized (i.e. scored as deviations from the person s mean standard score). Scale reliabilities were excellent ðmean a ¼ :86Þ: Relationship Profile Scale (RPS; Haslam et al., 2002) The RPS assesses participants motives for engaging in relationships of the four types. Unlike the MORQ, therefore, it assesses motivations rather than cognitive construals, and offers a complementary perspective on relational tendencies (i.e. people might wish for certain kinds of relationships but construe their present relationships in different terms). Participants read paragraph descriptions of relationships governed by each RM used in previous research (Haslam & Fiske, 1992) and rated nine items

4 Dionyssios Caralis and Nick Haslam assessing the perceived importance of and desire for each kind of relationship. Scale reliabilities were excellent ðmean a ¼ :89Þ: Results and discussion Table 1 presents correlations among the measures. As predicted, many associations are evident, every personality dimension having at least two relational correlates. Extraversion and agreeableness were associated, as expected, with tendencies to construe and desire close relationships. Extraverts had stronger motives to engage in communal (CS) relationships and construed their relationships in a more egalitarian (EM) manner than introverts, but did not have stronger AR and MP motives, challenging the simple view that extraversion is associated with generalized sociability. Agreeableness was associated with communal but not egalitarian relating, implying that it is specifically linked to close relationships that do not demand strict balance. More agreeable individuals also construed relationships in asymmetrical terms (AR) less than others, and found these relations less desirable. Table 1. Correlations between FFM dimensions and RMs measures (decimal omitted) Personality dimension Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Agreeableness Conscientiousness Communal sharing (CS) Construal 225* 14 202 39*** 24* Motivation 227* 40*** 10 23* 20 Equality matching (EM) Construal 231** 25* 23* 202 21 Motivation 203 16 208 213 208 Authority ranking (AR) Construal 30** 220 217 228* 225* Motivation 203 08 228* 224* 211 Market pricing (MP) Construal 24* 217 01 217 220 Motivation 02 02 221 215 204 * p, :05; ** p, :01; *** p, :001: As predicted, participants higher in openness favoured egalitarian relationships more, and also found authority relationships less appealing. By implication, traditionalism and rigidity are associated with tendencies to perceive relationships in hierarchical terms and to discount egalitarian arrangements. Neuroticism has a rich relational profile. More neurotic participants construed and desired communal relationships less than others, and were less apt to relate in an egalitarian fashion. Correspondingly, they construed relationships in more asymmetrical

Relational models 5 and calculative or instrumental (MP) ways, the former result consistent with prediction. Whether this pattern reflects a disinterest in closer and balanced relationships or a withdrawal from their difficulties is unclear. No predictions were made about differential correlations between the FFM dimensions and the motivational (RPS) and cognitive (MORQ) measures of particular relational tendencies. These correlations were usually not markedly discrepant, and they were in the same direction for most combinations of RMs and FFM dimensions. However, some interestingly discrepant correlations were found. Neuroticism, for example, was associated with a tendency to cognize relationships in AR terms but not with a motivation to engage in such relationships. Neurotic individuals may persistently think about the social world in asymmetric terms without desiring or enjoying and perhaps even hating asymmetrical relationships. An identical pattern of associations was previously obtained for avoidant PD (Haslam et al., 2002), and arguably conveys the neurotic s conflicted sociality in a plausible way. Findings such as these support the value of assessing relational tendencies both motivationally and cognitively. The findings of this study are preliminary and clearly require replication. The sample is somewhat modest, and although it was sufficiently powerful to detect many associations between the relational tendencies and the FFM dimensions a larger sample would enable a more sensitive investigation. More importantly, it remains to be seen whether the study findings would generalize to a non-clinical sample. Although FFM dimensions are commonly employed in clinical research and they were all reliably assessed with ample variance in the present sample, their associations with the RM measures might differ among people who are not suffering from mental disorders. How the wide-ranging associations obtained between personality and relational tendencies should be theorized can also be debated. It could be argued that the relational tendencies are just loosely associated correlates of FFM dimensions that are not meaningfully interpersonal, or more radically that all of these dimensions are intrinsically interpersonal to some degree. Regardless, the present study s findings supply preliminary evidence that the interpersonal component of the FFM cannot be restricted to two of its dimensions, as FFM and IC theorists tend to assume. Rather, all of the primary personality factors have interesting and plausible relational correlates. Neuroticism, for instance, is not just emotional instability or negativity an asocial or intrapersonal tendency but it also has a distinctive interpersonal profile (i.e. distancing from close relationships and preoccupation with rank). Taking an RMbased approach may therefore enrich and expand our view of the interpersonal domain, with respect to normal personality as well as PDs. References Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

6 Dionyssios Caralis and Nick Haslam Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of human relations. New York: Free Press. Fiske, A. P., & Haslam, N. (1996). Social cognition is thinking about relationships. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 143 148. Gurtman, M. B. (1991). Evaluating the interpersonalness of personality scales. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 670 677. Haslam, N. (1994). Categories of social relationship. Cognition, 53, 59 90. Haslam, N. (Ed.) (2004). Relational models theory: A contemporary overview. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Haslam, N., & Fiske, A. (1992). Implicit relationship prototypes: Investigating five theories of the elementary cognitive forms of social relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 441 474. Haslam, N., & Fiske, A. P. (1999). Relational Models Theory: A confirmatory factor analysis. Personal Relationships, 6, 241 250. Haslam, N., Reichert, T., & Fiske, A. P. (2002). Aberrant social relations in the personality disorders. Psychology & Psychotherapy: Theory Research and Practice, 75, 19 31. Kiesler, D. J. (1983). The 1982 Interpersonal Circle: A taxonomy for complementarity in human transactions. Psychological Review, 90, 185 214. Soldz, S., Budman, S., Demby, A., & Merry, J. (1993). Representation of personality disorders in circumplex and five-factor space: Explorations with a clinical sample. Psychological Assessment, 5, 41 52. Received 20 February 2003; revised version received 26 November 2003