Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain

Similar documents
Supplementary Information. Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain

Effective Intentions: The Power of Conscious Will

Report Reading Hidden Intentions in the Human Brain

Manipulating the Experienced Onset of Intention after Action Execution

John Ostrowick, School of Computer Science, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

Can Conscious Experience Affect Brain Activity?

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Cognitive Neuroscience and Addiction

MIND BODY Problem. distinction: body soul. Mind-Body Problem 1. Psychology of Religous. Beliefs

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background:

fmri: What Does It Measure?

The Representation of Abstract Task Rules in the Human Prefrontal Cortex

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplemental information online for

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

Myers Psychology for AP*

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplemental Material

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification

Neuroimaging and Assessment Methods

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Supporting Online Material for

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Importance of Deficits

On the Time of Peripheral Sensations and Voluntary Motor Actions. Text

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN)

fmri (functional MRI)

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Behavioral Task Performance

Report. Decoding Successive Computational Stages of Saliency Processing

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

Supplementary Information

Internally Generated Preactivation of Single Neurons in Human Medial Frontal Cortex Predicts Volition

There are, in total, four free parameters. The learning rate a controls how sharply the model

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Neuroscience Tutorial

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Title of file for HTML: Peer Review File Description:

The Central Nervous System

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Mirror neurons. Romana Umrianova

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

5/2/2013. Real-time fmri: Methods and applications

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Am I free? Free will vs. neuroscience

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Overlap between default mode network (DMN) and movie/recall maps.

Statistical parametric mapping

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems

TITLE:&The"organisation"of"working"memory"networks"is"shaped"by"early"sensory"experience"

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Enhancing functional magnetic resonance imaging with supervised learning

Supplementary Materials for

Mirror neurons in the infant brain are formed by the interactions between self and other. M. Iacoboni SELF AND OTHER. J. A. Pineda COGS171 UCSD

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Prediction of Successful Memory Encoding from fmri Data

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Men fear other men most: Gender specific brain activations in. perceiving threat from dynamic faces and bodies. An fmri. study.

The association of children s mathematic abilities with both adults cognitive abilities

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

performance of verbal working memory tasks

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Bottom-up and top-down effects of unconscious

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons

Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the. attentional network

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Summary of my talk. Cerebellum means little brain but a huge neural resource. Studying the cerebellum in. Chris Miall

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers

Transcription:

Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain Chun Siong Soon 1,2, Marcel Brass 1,3, Hans-Jochen Heinze 4 & John-Dylan Haynes* 1,2 1 Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstrasse 1A, 04103 Leipzig, Germany 2 Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Haus 6, Philippstrasse 13, 10115 Berlin, Germany 3 Department of Experimental Psychology and Ghent Institute for Functional and Metabolic Imaging, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent, Belgium 4 Department of Neurology II, Otto-von-Guericke University, Leipziger Strasse 44, 39120 Magdeburg, Germany Abbreviated title: Determinants of free decisions Key words: intention, decision, free will, self-paced, Libet * Corresponding author: John-Dylan Haynes Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Haus 6, Philippstrasse 13 10115 Berlin Phone: +49 (0)30 341 6762 Email: haynes@bccn-berlin.de

There has been a long controversy whether subjectively free decisions are determined by brain activity ahead of time. Here we show that the outcome of a decision is encoded in brain activity of prefrontal and parietal cortex even up to ten seconds before it enters awareness. This delay presumably reflects the operation of a network of high-level control areas that begin to prepare an upcoming decision long before it enters awareness. The impression that we are able to freely choose between different possible courses of action is fundamental to our mental life. However, it has been suggested that this subjective experience of freedom is no more than an illusion, and that our actions are initiated by unconscious mental processes long before we become aware of our intention to act 1-3. In a previous experiment 1, electrical brain activity was recorded while subjects were asked to press a button as soon as they felt the urge to do so. Interestingly, their conscious decision to press the button was preceded by a few hundred milliseconds by a negative brain potential, the so-called readiness potential (RP) that originates from a brain region involved in motor preparation, the supplementary motor area (SMA). Because brain activity in the SMA consistently preceded the conscious decision it has been argued that the brain had already unconsciously made a decision to move even before the subject became aware of it. However, these intriguing experiments have left a number of important controversial questions open 4-6 : (1) The readiness potential is generated by the SMA, and hence only provides information about late stages of motor planning. Thus, it is unclear whether the SMA is indeed the cortical site where the decision for a movement originates 7 or whether high-level planning stages might be involved in unconsciously preparing the decision 8, as shown in studies on conscious action planning 9-12. (2) The time delay between the onset of the readiness potential and the decision is only a few hundred milliseconds 1. It has been repeatedly argued that at such short delays potential inaccuracies in the behavioural measurement of the decision time could lead one to misjudge the relative timing of brain activity and intention 3-6. (3) An important question is whether any leading brain activity indeed selectively predicts the specific outcome of a choice ahead of time. In order to rule 2

out that any leading activity merely reflects unspecific preparatory activation 13, it is important to study free decisions between more than one behavioural option 11,14. Here we investigated directly which regions of the brain pre-determine conscious intentions and how early they start shaping a motor decision. Subjects who gave informed written consent performed a freely paced motor decision task while their brain activity was measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri; see Fig. 1 and Supplementary Methods). They were asked to relax while fixating on the center of the screen where a stream of letters was presented. At some point, when they felt the urge to do so, they should freely decide upon one of two buttons, operated by the left and right index fingers, and press it immediately. In parallel they should remember the letter presented when their motor decision was consciously made. After subjects pressed their freely chosen response button a response mapping screen with four choices appeared. By selecting the corresponding letter with a second button press subjects indicated when they had made their motor decision. After a delay the letter stream started again and a new trial began. The freely paced button presses occurred on average 21.6 s after trial onset, thus leaving sufficient time to estimate any potential buildup of a cortical decision without contamination by previous trials. Both left and right response buttons were pressed equally often and most of the intentions (88.6 %) were reported to be consciously formed within 1,000 ms prior to the movement (see Supplementary Material for details). Next we directly assessed how much predictive information each brain region contained about the specific outcome of a motor decision at various time points before and after it reached awareness. For each time point we measured how much information could be decoded from local patterns of fmri signals in various brain regions using statistical pattern recognition techniques 15. These pattern-based decoders were trained to predict the specific outcome of a subject s motor decision by recognizing characteristic local brain patterns associated with each choice. This highly sensitive approach had several advantages over previous studies. First, it allowed us to investigate any potential long-term determinants of human intentions that preceded the conscious intention much beyond the few hundred milliseconds observed over the SMA 1,14. Second, it allowed us to investigate for each brain 3

region separately how much information it had about the outcome of a motor decision. Finally, it allowed us to identify whether any leading brain activity indeed selectively predicted the outcome of the subject s choice, rather than reflecting potentially unspecific preparatory processes. In order to validate our method we first investigated from which brain regions this decision could be decoded after it had been made and the subject was executing the motor response. As expected, two brain regions encoded the outcome of the subject s motor decision during the execution phase: primary motor cortex and SMA (Fig. 2, top). Next, we addressed the key question of this study, whether any brain region encoded the subject s motor decision ahead of time. Indeed, we found that two brain regions encoded with high accuracy prior to the conscious decision whether the subject was about to choose the left or right response, even though the decision had not yet been made (Fig. 2, bottom; threshold p=0.05 fwe-corrected for multiple spatial and temporal comparisons). The first region was in frontopolar cortex, BA10. The predictive information in the fmri signals from this brain region was already present 7 seconds prior to the subject s motor decision. Taking into account the sluggishness of BOLD responses the predictive neural information will have preceded the conscious motor decision by up to 10 seconds. There was a second predictive region located in parietal cortex stretching from the precuneus into posterior cingulate cortex. Importantly, there was no overall signal increase in the frontopolar and precuneus/posterior cingulate during the preparation period (Supplementary Fig. 3). Rather, the predictive information was encoded in the local spatial pattern of fmri responses, which is presumably why it has not been noticed before. When the statistical threshold was relaxed several other regions of frontal cortex exhibited predictive information, albeit less pronounced (Supplementary Table 1). Importantly, we also ensured that there was no carryover of information between trials, so that the high decoding performance preceding the motor decision by up to 10 seconds cannot reflect decoding related to the previous trial (Supplementary Material). Finally, we also assessed to which degree the timing of the decision could be predicted ahead of time. We found that decoding of the time decision was possible as early as 5 seconds preceding the motor decision, but mainly from pre-sma and SMA, whereas in the frontopolar and parietal cortex this was only possible just before the motor decision 4

(Supplementary Fig. 3). Thus, in very early stages there appears to be a double dissociation between brain regions shaping the specific outcome of the motor decision and brain regions determining the timing of a motor decision. At later stages right before the conscious decision both of these regions begin to encode timing and handedness information. Finally in order to further investigate the involvement of frontopolar cortex and precuneus in selecting intentions we investigated voluntary decisions where subjects have to decide between left and right responses at an externally determined point in time. In this case the time when a decision is selected is under experiment control. This revealed that frontopolar cortex was already predictive during the selection of the response whereas the predictive information in precuneus began after the selection during the delay. This is in agreement with a trend in the main experiment that showed that the information in lateral frontopolar cortex peaked already at the earliest time point. One interpretation of this finding is that frontopolar cortex was the first cortical stage at which the actual decision was made, whereas precuneus was involved in storage of the decision until it reached awareness. Importantly, in this control experiment the intention was selected consciously, suggesting that similar networks might be involved in conscious and unconscious preparation of decisions (see Supplementary Material and Supplementary Fig. 9 for full details). Taken together, two specific regions in the frontal and parietal cortex of the human brain had considerable information that predicted the outcome of a motor decision the subject had not yet even consciously made. This suggests that when the subject s decision reached awareness it had been influenced by unconscious brain activity up to even 10 seconds before, thus also providing a potential cortical origin for unconscious changes in skin conductance preceding risky decisions 8. Our results go significantly beyond previous studies 1-15 by showing that the earliest predictive information is encoded in specific regions of frontopolar and parietal cortex, not in SMA. This preparatory time period in high-level control regions is considerably longer than reported previously for motor-related brain regions 1,14, and is considerably longer than the predictive time exhibited by the SMA in the current study (Supplementary Fig. 3). Also, in contrast to most previous studies 1,13, it reveals that this prior activity is not an unspecific preparation of a response. Instead, it specifically encodes how a subject is going to decide. Thus, the SMA is 5

presumably not the ultimate cortical decision stage where the conscious intention is initiated as has been previously suggested 7. Importantly, the lead times are too long to be explained by any timing inaccuracies in reporting the onset of awareness, a major criticism of previous studies 4-6. The temporal ordering of information suggests a tentative causal model of information flow, where the earliest unconscious precursors of the motor decision originated in frontopolar cortex, from where they influenced the build-up of decision-related information in the precuneus and later in SMA where it remained unconscious for up to a few seconds. This substantially extends previous work that has shown that BA10 is involved in storage of conscious action plans 9-11 and shifts in strategy following negative feedback 12. Thus, a network of high-level control areas begins to shape an upcoming decision long before it enters awareness. 6

REFERENCES 1. Libet, B. et al. Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 529-566 (1985). 2. Wegner, D. The mind's best trick: how we experience conscious will. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 65-69 (2003). 3. Haggard, P. Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 290-295 (2005). 4. Van de Grind, W. Physical, neural, and mental timing. Consc. Cogn. 11, 241-264 (2002). 5. Glynn, I.M. Consciousness and time. Nature 348, 477-479 (1990). 6. Joordens, S., van Duijn, M. & Spalek, T.M. When timing the mind one should also mind the timing: biases in the measurement of voluntary actions. Conscious. Cogn. 11, 231-240 (2002). 7. Eccles, J.C. The initiation of voluntary movements by the supplementary motor area. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 231, 423-441 (1982). 8. Bechera, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275, 1293-1295 (1997). 9. Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S. & Grafman, J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature 399, 148-151 (1999). 10. Burgess, P.W., Quayle, A. & Frith, C.D. Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia 39, 545-555 (2001). 11. Haynes, J.D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S., Frith, C. & Passingham, R.E. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Curr. Biol. 20, 323-328 (2007). 12. Hampton, A.N., O'Doherty, J.P. Decoding the neural substrates of rewardrelated decision making with functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 1377-1382 (2007). 13. Lau, H.C., Rogers, R.D., Haggard, P. & Passingham, R.E. Attention to intention. Science 303, 1208-1210 (2004). 14. Haggard, P. & Eimer, M. On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 128-133 (1999). 15. Haynes, J.D. & Rees, G. Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 523-534 (2006). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was funded by the Max Planck Society and the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). The authors would like to thank Dick Passingham and Hakwan Lau for valuable comments and Toralf Mildner and Stefan Zysset for help with scanning. 7

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS J.D.H, C.S.S, M.B. and H.J.H. conceived the experiment. C.S.S. and J.D.H. carried out the experiment. C.S.S. analyzed the data. J.D.H. and C.S.S. co-wrote the paper. 8

FIGURE CAPTIONS Figure 1: Measuring the onset time of conscious motor intentions. Subjects viewed a letter stream that updated every 0 ms (shown here only for a few frames). At some point they spontaneously made the decision for either their left or right index finger and then pressed the corresponding button ( free response ). Subsequently, they were presented with a response mapping screen that instructed subjects which second button to press to report the time when they consciously made the motor decision (see Supplementary Material for details). Figure 2: Decoding the outcome of decisions before and after they reached awareness. Colour-coded brain areas show regions where the specific outcome of a motor decision could be decoded before (bottom, green) and after (top, red) it had been made. The graphs depict separately for each timepoint the accuracy with which the subject s free choice to press the left or right button could be decoded from the spatial pattern of brain activity in that region (solid line, left axis; filled symbols: significant at p<0.05; open symbols: not significant; error bars = s.e.m.; chance level is %). As might be expected, decoding accuracy is higher in cortical areas involved in the motor execution of the response, than in areas shaping the upcoming decision before it reaches awareness (note difference in scale). The vertical red line shows the earliest time when the subjects became aware of their choices. The dashed (right) vertical line in each graph shows the onset of the next trial. The inset in the bottom left shows the representative spatial pattern of preference of the most discriminative searchlight position in frontopolar cortex for one subject. 9

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION GUIDE Combined File with Supplementary Figures, Tables, Discussion and Methods. Contents: Item Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 2 Supplementary Figure 3 Supplementary Figure 4 Supplementary Figure 5 Supplementary Figure 6 Supplementary Figure 7 Supplementary Figure 8 Supplementary Figure 9 Supplementary Table 1 Supplementary Methods Supplementary Discussion Title or caption Behavioural control experiment on decision timing. Multivariate decoding from local spatial patterns using a moving searchlight. Full decoding results of decision outcome and decision timing. Example of voxel selectivity for a representative searchlight. Decoding across multiple brain regions. Decoding the outcome of a decision from motion parameters. Histogram of sequence lengths. The distribution timing judgements in the main experiment. Decoding for cued decision timing. Brain areas encoding intention prior to conscious decision. 10

k 0 ms t d 1 fmri image = 2,000 ms q v Free response L R q v # d Intention onset judgment

Pre-SMA ( 9, 6, 57) 75 Left motor cortex ( 42, 18, 57) Right motor cortex (39, 18, 57) 75 75 Decoding accuracy (%) 75 SMA (3, 6, 54) Lateral frontopolar cortex (33, 69, 12) Time (s) Medial frontopolar cortex (0, 60, 3) 60 60 Decision choice L sup ant Precuneus / posterior cingulate cortex ( 12, 60, 21) 60 R R