Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Similar documents
DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

The spatio temporal dynamics of deviance and target detection in the passive and active auditory oddball paradigm: a sloreta study

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute.

Processing Interaural Cues in Sound Segregation by Young and Middle-Aged Brains DOI: /jaaa

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Competing Streams at the Cocktail Party

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Supporting Information

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

INHIBITION AND SET-SHIFTING TASKS IN CENTRAL EXECUTIVE FUNCTION OF WORKING MEMORY: AN EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERP) STUDY

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

P revious research has suggested that aging. Effect of Age on Interaural Asymmetry of Event-Related Potentials in a Dichotic Listening Task

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Altered Dynamic of EEG Oscillations in Fibromyalgia Patients at Rest

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Outline. Biological Psychology: Research Methods. Dr. Katherine Mickley Steinmetz

Northeast Center for Special Care Grant Avenue Lake Katrine, NY

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA

WAVELET ENERGY DISTRIBUTIONS OF P300 EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS FOR WORKING MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN CHILDREN

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

EEG reveals divergent paths for speech envelopes during selective attention

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

1- Cochlear Impedance Telemetry

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials in Children with Developmental Dysphasia

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Viewing a Map Versus Reading a Description of a Map: Modality-Specific Encoding of Spatial Information

Neural Networks: Tracing Cellular Pathways. Lauren Berryman Sunfest 2000

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

ERP Components and the Application in Brain-Computer Interface

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Event Related Potentials: Significant Lobe Areas and Wave Forms for Picture Visual Stimulus


In the first section, Introduction, we present our experimental design.

Distributed Auditory Cortical Representations Are Modified When Nonmusicians. Discrimination with 40 Hz Amplitude Modulated Tones

Mental Representation of Number in Different Numerical Forms

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Supplementary Information on TMS/hd-EEG recordings: acquisition and preprocessing

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

The Sonification of Human EEG and other Biomedical Data. Part 3

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Cognitive Science 279: Electrophysiology of Cognition Spring Quarter 2013 Tu/Th 11-12:20 CSB 180

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain

Report. Direct Recordings of Pitch Responses from Human Auditory Cortex

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy

Amy Kruse, Ph.D. Strategic Analysis, Inc. LCDR Dylan Schmorrow USN Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency

Brain Computer Interface. Mina Mikhail

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Source localisation in the clinical practice: spontaneous EEG examinations with LORETA. Ph.D. thesis. Márton Tamás Tóth M.D.

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse

Evaluation and Validity of a LORETA Normative EEG Database

Electroencephalography

EEG/ERP Control Process Separation

Practicalities of EEG Measurement and Experiment Design YATING LIU

Supporting Information

Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks

Transcription:

International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities Volume 5 Article 3 June 2013 Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry Whitney R. Arthur Central Washington University, kwamay27@gmail.com Ralf Greenwald Central Washington University, greenwar@cwu.edu Follow this and additional works at: http://commons.pacificu.edu/ijurca Recommended Citation Arthur, Whitney R. and Greenwald, Ralf (2013) "Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry," International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities: Vol. 5, Article 3. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7710/2168-0620.1000

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry Peer Review This work has undergone a double-blind review by a minimum of two faculty members from institutions of higher learning from around the world. The faculty reviewers have expertise in disciplines closely related to those represented by this work. If possible, the work was also reviewed by undergraduates in collaboration with the faculty reviewers. Abstract In an effort to further explore the role of the right hemisphere in auditory processing, this study utilized brain event-related potentials (ERPs) and standardized low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sloreta) in order to localize electrical brain activity of complex tone processing. Participants engaged in a diotic pitch discrimination task while ERP components were recorded from 28 electrodes on the scalp. ERPs were analyzed via individual/group average area measurements and were submitted to sloreta for neural source localization. Results showed stronger right vs. left hemisphere activation to complex tone processing that was localized to regions of the auditory cortex. Findings support the notion that auditory cortical regions in the right hemisphere may be involved in music processing. Moreover, these findings indicate that source localization methods may be a good approach to study the complex nature of brain electrical activity during cognitive processing. Keywords hemispheric asymmetry, spectral processing, sloreta, ERP, P300 Acknowledgements Editor's Note: Dr. Ralf Greenwald, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, Central Washington University, served as faculty mentor for this work. This research article is available in International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities: http://commons.pacificu.edu/ijurca/vol5/iss1/3

Arthur and Greenwald: Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing INTRODUCTION It has been well established that functional hemispheric asymmetries exist regarding speech processing. Specifically, speech has been shown to be dominantly processed in the left hemisphere (Belin, Zatorre, Lafaille, Ahad, & Pike, 2000; Rosen et al., 2011). In contrast, music is speculated to be dominantly processed in the right hemisphere (Zatorre, Belin, & Penhune, 2002). However, there has been a wide debate about the nature of the right hemisphere involvement. Several lines of research suggest that the right auditory cortex may play a key role in music processing. Specifically, spectral and temporal features of sounds may be crucial factors for auditory encoding and perception (Zatorre et al., 2002). For example, behavioral studies have shown spectral processing to be important for music perception, while speech perception tends to rely more on temporal processing (Warrier & Zatorre, 2002). These differences in auditory processing may be the cause of asymmetric neural activities between hemispheres during speech and music processing. In addition, Zatorre and Belin (2001) used PET imaging to investigate auditory cortical responses to spectral and temporal variation. Findings showed that responses to spectral features were weighted toward the right hemisphere, providing evidence that right hemispheric auditory cortical areas may be specialized for spectral, and thus music perception. The role of the right hemisphere in auditory processing has also been studied using brain event-related potentials (ERPs). For example, Greenwald and Jerger (2003) showed a right hemisphere processing bias for complex spectral tones and that the P3 brain wave may be a good indicator of hemispheric specialization. However, the specific auditory cortical regions involved in spectral processing remains an open question. One interesting avenue for investigating these types of issues may be found in cortical source localization methods. These methods have been developed to directly link scalp-recorded ERP potentials with cortical activation. One of the more promising source localization approaches is found in Standardized Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (sloreta) developed by Pascual-Marqui (2002). This method is a non-invasive functional brain imaging technique that estimates the electrical activity within the brain related to ERPs (i.e., current source density). For instance, Lewald & Getzmann (2011) recently published a study using sloreta to localize electrical brain activity of auditory spatial processing, highlighting the capability of this kind of an approach. The aim of the present study was to investigate the possible neural sources of complex tone processing (i.e., spectral processing) via the sloreta source localization method and to further explore the right hemisphere bias for spectral discrimination. This study expands the Greenwald and Jerger (2003) study in that it seeks to detect the possible cortical sources of ERPs elicited by complex tones. The authors hypothesize that ERPs elicited by spectrally complex tones (musical notes) will be related to auditory cortex activation. Specifically, it is expected that the right auditory hemisphere will show greater activation than the left hemisphere in young adults. MATERIALS AND METHODS Participants Fifteen neurologically healthy undergraduate psychology students (8 1

International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities, Vol. 5 [2013], Art. 3 women and 7 men; mean age = 21.2; SD =1.5 years) participated in the study. All the participants were right-handed and nonmusicians (less then two years of formal musical training). All procedures were approved by the CWU Institutional Review Board (IRB). Experimental Stimuli Participants completed a diotic (same tones presented to each ear), complex-tone discrimination task that was based on the Complex Tone Test (CTT) of Sidtis (1980). The spectrally complex tones (CT) were square waveforms with fundamental frequencies corresponding to six notes in the octave between C4 and C5 on the major scale. This includes C4 (264 Hz), D (297 Hz), E (330 Hz), F (352 Hz), G (396 Hz), and A (440 Hz). Tones were 250 msec in duration, with rise-decay times of 20 msec. Experimental Procedures This study used the same procedures as outlined in Greenwald and Jerger (2003). The participants were seated comfortably in a chair. Target stimuli were delivered from two phase-matched loudspeakers at ear height to either side of participant s ears. Each participant performed a diotic pitch discrimination task presented in a basic S1- S2-R design. In this paradigm a reference CT is followed two seconds later by a probe CT. The participant s task was to decide whether the pitch of the probe CT matches the pitch of the reference CT. Trials were arranged in six 25-trial blocks. Using random ordering, each block, half of the reference CT matched the probe CT. Electrophysiological Recording Electroencephalographic (EEG) activity was recorded from 28 scalp locations, using silver/silver-chloride (Ag/AgCl) electrodes attached to an elastic cap (Neuromedical Supplies Inc.) and a Neuroscan amplifier/stimulator with the Neuroscan recording software. Eye movements and eye blinks were monitored via two electrodes, one positioned medially above the left eye and one at the outer canthus of the left eye. All 28 channels of EEG were referenced to linked mastoid electrodes. Individual sweeps of EEG activity, time-locked to the stimuli, were stored for off-line analysis. Each sweep was initiated by a trigger placed at the onset of each probe tone. The stored epoch encompassed 1200 msec (including a 200 msec prestimulus baseline) relative to stimulus onset. All electrodes impedances were below 5 kω. Ongoing EEG activity was amplified, analog filtered from.15 to 70 Hz (1 to 100 Hz for the VEOG channel), and digitized through the Neuroscan acquisition interface system. Continuous analog-todigital conversion of the EEG and stimulus trigger codes was performed on-line by the Neuroscan acquisition interface system. Signal averaging was conducted after offline artifact rejection and baseline correction. Individual epochs were examined and rejected whenever electrical activity in either VEOG channel or the frontal channels (FP1, FP2) exceeded ±50µV. Successfully averaged evoked potential waveforms were then digitally lowpass-filtered with zero phase-shift at 20Hz with a filter slope of -48 db per octave in order to remove ambient electrical noise and muscle artifact. Data Analysis Since the latency of the P3 peak can vary widely across subjects, which in turn can attenuate the P3 peak and introduce latency jitter (Kok, 2001), individual averaged waveforms were subjected to measurement of area (amplitude integrated http://commons.pacificu.edu/ijurca/vol5/iss1/3 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7710/2168-0620.1000 2

Arthur and Greenwald: Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing over a specified time interval) between waveform and baseline. Area computation uses the extended trapezoidal rule to estimate area under the curve. That is, it sums all points except the two endpoints (which are given half-weight), and multiplies them by the sampling interval. Area measures were made over the 300 to 900 msec latency range for the P3 component, and over the 50 to 150 msec latency range for the N1 component for all conditions. These latency ranges were chosen to ensure capture of the maximal component peaks for each individual and for group averages. In the current study, data analysis was restricted to match judgments. Source Localization The sources of bioelectrical activity were estimated using Standardized Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (sloreta). All computations were made using the template from Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) with the threedimensional solution space restricted to cortical gray matter and hippocampus, as determined by the probabilistic Talairach atlas (Lancaster, Kochunov, Nickerson, Mikiten, & Fox, 2000). The sloreta images represent the standardized electric activity at each voxel in neuroanatomic MNI space as the exact magnitude of the estimated current density. Anatomical labels as Brodmann areas are reported using MNI space, with correction to Talairach space (Brett, Johnsrude, & Owen, 2002). The full description of the method can be found in Pascual-Marqui (2002). sloreta images corresponding to the ERP waveforms were defined as the mean current density values for the interval of the N1-P3 brain wave complex. RESULTS Behavioral Accuracy score data were collected during ERP testing to ensure that the participants understood and were able to perform the tasks. Since the main focus of this study was not to evaluate possible differences between subject s responses to match and mismatch stimuli, only responses to the match experimental conditions were evaluated. In order to assure high accuracy, subjects were instructed to be as accurate as possible when responding to the stimulus. Thus, no response latency was recorded. The mean accuracy score for correct responses of the participants for the diotic CTT was 98.2% (SD: 1.3). Event-Related Potentials The N1-P3 waveform complex obtained in response to matching complex tones is shown in Figure 1. This waveform complex was maximal at the CPZ electrode site. The mean area latency of averaged individual responses for the N1 was -111.2 (SD: 112.2) and -2663.4 (SD: 1190.7) for the P3 component. A negative difference indicates greater waveform on the right hemisphere, thus the P3 component showed a maximal right hemisphere asymmetry at the CPZ electrode. Figure 2 shows the topographic distribution of the N1-P3 brain waveform. Note that the topographic distribution of the response is clearly in the direction of the right hemisphere in the region of the centro-parietal electrodes. 3

International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities, Vol. 5 [2013], Art. 3 Moreover, the cortical activation was more pronounced in the right hemisphere. Cortical activation for the N1-P3 waveform was maximal in the middle temporal gyrus and temporal-parietal lobe (Brodmann s area 21) of the right hemisphere. For clarity, Figure 4 shows a cortical model created by sloreta of the maximal current source density in the right hemisphere. Figure 1. Grand Average ERP Waveform at the CPZ electrode showing the N1-P3 waveform complex. Figure 2. Topographic scalp distribution of the P3 waveform. Red color indicates maximal ERP signal. sloreta Figure 3 shows the results of the sloreta analysis of the N1-P3 complex. sloreta estimates current source density across the brain volume by computing the set of voxel dipole strengths and orientations that account for the obtained scalp topography. The maps shown in Figure 3 are essentially statistical maps of current density at each voxel summarized across time. Processing CT s produced widespread activation in the auditory cortex and temporo-parietal cortical regions. Figure 4. MNI cortical model of the N1-P3 ERP complex neural sources in the right cerebral hemisphere. DISCUSSION The results of the current study are in concordance with other neuroimaging methods, and indicate a right hemisphere asymmetry for spectral processing. Our results are consistent with the notion that the cerebral hemispheres are differentially specialized for processing different aspects of auditory information (i.e., temporal vs. spectral). Figure 3. sloreta source location of the N1-P3 ERP complex superimposed on horizontal, saggital, and coronal slices of the MNI brain. Colors represent the electric activity at each voxel in neuroanatomic Talairach space. Yellow color indicates maximal activation. http://commons.pacificu.edu/ijurca/vol5/iss1/3 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7710/2168-0620.1000 4

Arthur and Greenwald: Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing Specifically, the findings of this study show that complex tone processing has a right hemisphere bias with maximal activation in the temporal cortical regions in non-musicians. Moreover, the findings of this study enhance a previous study of complex tone processing (Greenwald & Jerger, 2003) by providing the possible neural correlates of spectral processing in the brain. To our knowledge, there have been no prior studies of sloreta and complex tone processing, and this study shows the utility of cortical source localization methods in investigating hemispheric asymmetries. A limitation of this study is that it only investigated the responses to correct matches of complex tone pairs and did not view incorrect responses. Future research may want to compare the differences in hemispheric asymmetries to the different types of responses in order to gain a more complete understanding of complex tone processing. In addition, future investigations could also include neural source comparisons between spectral and temporal aspects of sounds processing. For example, an fmri study (Jamison, Watkins, Bicho, & Mathews, 2006) showed that there are some hemispheric differences in processing of temporal versus spectral information. Utilizing a method like sloreta could be another avenue to further probe the various auditory processing regions of the brain. Finally, the results of this study only address complex tone processing in non-musicians, and further studies are warranted to explore the complex tone processing of musicians. Using the well-established complex tones test, the present study clearly shows that the spectral aspect of sound is dominantly processed in the right hemisphere. Since spectral processing is an important factor in music processing (Warrier & Zatorre, 2002), the results of the current study also shed some further insight into the nature of right hemisphere involvement in the processing of music. For instance, results indicate that the middle temporal gyrus and temporal-parietal lobe regions appear to be involved in music note processing (complex tones). In conclusion, the observations of the current study strongly support the hypothesis that the human auditory cortex of the right hemisphere has strong spectral resolution capabilities (Zatorre & Belin, 2001; Warrier & Zatorre, 2002) and contribute to the overall understanding of hemispheric asymmetries of the human auditory cortex function. REFERENCES Belin, P., Zatorre, R.J., Lafaille, P., Ahad, P., & Pike, B. (2000). Voiceselective areas in human audiotry cortex. Nature, 403, 309-312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35002078 Brett, M., Johnsrude, I.S., & Owen, A.M. (2002). The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 243-249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn756 Greenwald, R. R., & Jerger, J.F. (2003). Neuroelectric correlates of hemispheric asymmetry: Spectral discrimination and stimulus competition. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 14(8), 434-443. Jamison, H. L., Watkins, K.E., Bishop, V.M., & Matthews, P.M. (2006). Hemispheric specialization for processing auditory nonspeech stimuli. Cerbral Cortex, 16, 1266-1275. 5

International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities, Vol. 5 [2013], Art. 3 Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 38, 557-577. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s00485772 01990559 Lancaster, J.L., Kochunov, P.V., Nickerson, D., Mikiten, S.A., & Fox, P.T. (2000). Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping, 10, 120-131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::aid- HBM30>3.0.CO;2-8 Lewald, J., & Getzmann, S. (2011). When and where of auditory spatial processing in cortex: A novel approach using electrotomography. Public Library of Science One, 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pon e.0025146 Warrier, C.M., & Zatorre, R.J. (2002). Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch. Perceptual Psychophysiology, 64, 198-207. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf031957 86 Zatorre, R.J., & Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 946-953. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.1 0.946 Zatorre, R.J., Belin, P., & Penhune, V.B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 37-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01816-7 Pascual-Marqui, R.D. (2002). Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomopgraphy (sloreta): Technical details. Method & Findings in Experimental Clinical Pharmacology, 24, 5-12. Rosen, S., Wise, R.J.S., Chadha, S., Conway, E-J., & Scott, S.K. (2011). Hemispheric asymmetries in speech perception: Sense, nonsense and modulation. Public Library of Science One, 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pon e.0024672 Sidtis, J.J. (1980). On the nature of the cortical function underlying right hemisphere auditory perception. Neuropsychologia, 18, 321 330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(80)90127-x http://commons.pacificu.edu/ijurca/vol5/iss1/3 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7710/2168-0620.1000 6