Temperamental Effortful Control (Self- Regulation)

Similar documents
Children s Effortful Selfregulation: Relations to Adjustment and Maladjustment

Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process

Emotion-Related Self- Regulation: Conceptualualization and Relations to Socioemotional Functioning. Nancy Eisenberg Duke University March, 2014

Empathy-Related Responding and Its Relations to Socioemotional Development 1)

Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood

Min Deng. Chapel Hill Approved by: Jean-Louis Gariépy (Chair) Martha J. Cox. W. Roger Mills-Koonce. Abigail T. Panter. J.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Dev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 27.

Antecedents, Correlates, and Sequelae of Children s Empathy-related and Prosocial Responding. Nancy Eisenberg Arizona State University

COMMENTARIES: Relationships as Inputs and Outputs of Emotion Regulation

Commentary: Differentiated Measures of Temperament and Multiple Pathways to Childhood Disorders

Emotional Development in Childhood

A twin study of the relationship between inhibitory control and behavior problems

Effortful Control, Social Competence, and Adjustment Problems in Children at Risk for Psychopathology

Emotional Development

Socio-Cognitive Correlates of Prosocial Behaviour in Young Children

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Cogn Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 October 1.

Section C. Intrinsic and Extrinsic Influences on Infant and Early Childhood Regulatory Development

Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 December 1.

What explains the remarkable variation between children

The Moral Foundations of Prosocial Behaviour

The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood*

Predicting Empathy-Related Responding and Prosocial Behavior. from Dispositional Sadness and Effortful Control. Alison Edwards

As adults, we are good at planning for the day s activities,

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Health and Human Development

Emotion Regulation 1. Foundations of Emotion Regulation. Neuroscience and Education (HDAP 1238)

Maria M. Wong Idaho State University

Emotions and Psychopathology in the First 5 Years of Life

Sources of variability in working memory in early childhood: A consideration of age, temperament, language, and brain electrical activity

Parent-child interactions and children with autism: A novel perspective

Anger and Positive Reactivity in Infancy: Effects on Maternal Report of Surgency and Attention Focusing in Early Childhood

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Soc Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 February 1.

The Developmental Course of Potential Precursors in the First Year of Life and the Emergence of ADHD Symptoms in Early Childhood.

Reproductive Technology and Its Impact on Psychosocial Child Development

Supporting Families to Build Secure Attachment Relationships : Comments on Benoit, Dozier, and Egeland

The Effect of Early Intervention on the Social and Emotional Development of Young Children (0-5) with Autism

Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development: 2 nd edition. Revised chapter on Temperament and social development

DIFFERENTIAL SUSCEPTIBILITY TO MATERNAL SENSITIVITY

CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES

If-Then Planning Helps School-Aged Children to Ignore Attractive Distractions

The Influences of Early Temperament and Emotion Regulation on Social Competence in Middle Childhood

Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: The role of effortful control

The expression and regulation of negative emotions: Risk factors for young children s peer victimization

Kindergarten children s attachment security, inhibitory control, and the internalization of rules of conduct

Developmental Stability and Change in Self- Regulation From Childhood to Adolescence

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Introduction CHAPTER 1

The Impact of Developing Social Perspective-taking Skills on Emotionality in Middle and Late Childhood

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF

The integration of cognition and emotion during infancy and early childhood: Regulatory processes associated with the development of working memory

The Interaction between Negative Emotionality and Effortful Control in Early Social-emotional Development

The Brain is Still a Mystery. National Institutes of Health 7/11/2012

Early Childhood Self Regulation and Mental Health What Can You Do? A KidsMatter Early Childhood webinar

Recognition and Assessment of Anxiety & Depression in Early Childhood

Infant Behavior and Development

Influences on Development: Causal Factors

Section B. Self-Regulation, Attention, and Problem Solving

Inhibitory Control and Classroom Behavior in Kindergarten Children

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

Attentional control and psychopathological symptoms in children

Development and Aging

Jeffry A. Simpson, W. Andrew Collins, and Jessica E. Salvatore University of Minnesota. Introduction. An Organizational Perspective on Development

JAMIE L. ABAIED DISSERTATION

The Importance of Attachment in Adolescence. Lesley Bell Therapeutic Social Worker

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Behavioral Stability and Change in Children 6 36 months of Age Using Louisville Twin Study Data

Treatment of Postpartum Depression

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

references moral baby

FARMINGDALE STATE COLLEGE

0-3 DEVELOPMENT. By Drina Madden. Pediatric Neuropsychology 1

Welcome to the Early Learning Webcast Self-Regulation...What is it and why is it important for learning?

The development and socialization of aggression in the first five years of life. Kate Keenan Department of Psychiatry University of Chicago

SPECIAL ISSUE TITLE: Developmental and Clinical Approaches to. Coping and Emotion Regulation

KATHRYN LEMERY-CHALFANT, PH.D.

BEHAVIORAL INHIBITION: Linking Biology and Behavior within a Developmental Framework

Maura Sabatos-DeVito. Chapel Hill Approved By: J. Steven Reznick. Grace T. Baranek. Martha Cox. Barbara D. Goldman.

Biological and Experimental Psychology School of Biological and Chemical Sciences Individual Differences in Environmental Sensitivity

Title: The involvement of attentional mechanisms in children' self-regulation abilites. (1) University of Murcia, Spain

Temperament and Preschool Children s Peer Interactions

Chapter 3: Biological foundations Genes, temperament, and more

G E interaction in the organization of attachment: mothers responsiveness as a moderator of children s genotypes

Developmental Changes in Baseline Cortisol Activity in Early Childhood: Relations with Napping and Effortful Control

Infant Behavior and Development

Chapter 1: The People and The Field

Childhood Stuttering and Temperament. Children Who Stutter: Easy, Difficult, or Slow to Warm Up?

Year 12 Psychology (Paper 1)

AREAS IN WHICH MORE RESEARCH ON THE EFFECTS OF COCAINE IS NEEDED

Parental ADHD Symptomology and Ineffective Parenting: The Connecting Link of Home Chaos

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Child Dev Perspect. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.

Social and Emotional Foundations of Transitional Kindergarten

Consumption of Tobacco During Pregnancy and Its Impact on Child Development

Susan VanDerhei. Bachelor of Arts, University of Maryland, Master of Arts, George Mason University, 2012

Consistency and Development of Prosocial Dispositions: A Longitudinal Study

Jeffrey Liew a, Audrea Y. Johnson a, Tracy R. Smith b & Felix Thoemmes a. University b Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University

TEMPERAMENT AND EMOTION REGULATION: PREDICTING SOCIAL COMPETENCE, INTERNALIZING, AND EXTERNALIZING BEHAVIORAL OUTCOMES

The Resilient Revolution takes on

AN EVALUATION AND REVISION OF THE CHILDREN S BEHAVIOR QUESTIONNAIRE EFFORTFUL CONTROL SCALES

Brazelton Touchpoints Center

Resilience in Individuals and Communities

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality

Parenting a Child with Mental Health Concerns

Transcription:

TEMPERAMENT Temperamental Effortful Control (Self- Regulation) Nancy Eisenberg, PhD Arizona State University, USA April 2012, Rev. ed. Introduction An important dimension of temperament is effortful control, which has been defined by Rothbart as "the ability to inhibit a dominant response to perform a subdominant response" (p. 137) 1 or the efficiency of executive attention, including the ability to inhibit a dominant response and/or to activate a subdominant response, to plan, and to detect errors. 1,2 Effortful control includes the abilities to voluntarily manage attention (attentional regulation) and inhibit (inhibitory control) or activate (activational control) behaviour as needed to adapt, especially when the child does not particularly want to do so. For example, the abilities to focus attention when there are distractions, to not interrupt others and sit still in church or class, and to force oneself to do an unpleasant task are aspects of effortful control. These abilities underlie the emergence of self-regulation, a major milestone in children s development. 2,3 Although nearly all children improve greatly in their effortful control (and hence self-regulation) across the first five years of life, there are large individual differences in effortful control. As is true for other aspects of temperament, individual differences in effortful control are believed to be due to both biological factors (hereditary and constitutional factors, such as the prenatal environment) and environmental influences (e.g., on prenatal care), and to be affected over time by environmental influences during early childhood. Effortful control is believed to involve executive attention abilities and to be linked to activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus (part of the brain) and prefrontal cortex. 3 Effortful control, as part of executive attention, has been shown to be involved in the voluntary control of thoughts and feelings, in resolving conflict in regard to discrepant information, correcting errors and planning new actions. 1,3,4 Subject The emergence of temperamentally based self-regulation and individual differences therein is important for 2012-2017 CEECD / SKC-ECD TEMPERAMENT 1

multiple reasons. As children age, they are increasingly held responsible for their own behaviour by their socializers. 2,3 Children who are not well regulated are likely to elicit negative reactions from both peers and adults. In addition, the attentional skills involved in effortful control are likely quite important for learning. 5,6 Finally, the skills involved in effortful control have an obvious relevance to children s emerging adjustment and social competence. 7 Problem For the aforementioned reasons, it is important to identify both the normative pattern for the emergence of effortful control and the antecedents of individual differences in effortful control. Researchers in the developmental sciences have examined both of these issues. Research Context Effortful control has been studied using a variety of methods. Investigators examining effortful control typically have used parents or other caregivers reports of children s effortful control and behavioural measures. These behavioural measures typically include tasks that assess children s focused attention and persistence on tasks, attentional control on Stroop tests or other measures of executive attention, the ability to delay gratification (e.g., hold an M & M on their tongue), and the abilities to inhibit or activate behaviour (e.g., follow an instruction in response to one cue but not another or move faster and slower in accordance with instructions). 8 Such research has been conducted in both laboratory settings (sometimes in preschools) and in the home environment. Key Research Questions Important research questions are the age at which attentional and behavioural control (i.e., inhibitory and activational control) emerge in the early years of life and when they become relatively well developed. Investigators have also been interested in aspects of children s social interactions especially parent-child interactions that are associated with individual differences in effortful control. Behavioural and molecular geneticists have also attempted to identify the degree to which heredity contributes to effortful control and interacts with environmental influences when predicting self-regulation. Finally, researchers have assessed the relationships between effortful control and young children s adjustment and moral development. Recent Research Results Young infants exhibit very little effortful control. Attention becomes somewhat more voluntary (but still quite limited) between nine and 18 months of age 9 as infants learn to resolve conflicts (e.g. when processing information), correct errors and plan new actions. 4 Using a Stroop-like task that requires toddlers to switch attention and inhibit behaviour accordingly, Posner and Rothbart 4 reported that children showed significant improvement in performance by 30 months of age and performed with high accuracy by 36 to 38 months of age. 10,11 Infants are very limited in the behavioural component of voluntary behavioural control (e.g., the ability to inhibit behaviour upon command), but these skills improve considerably in the third year of life. 4,8 The ability to effortfully inhibit behaviour on tasks such as "Simon Says" emerges at approximately 44 months of age and is 2012-2017 CEECD / SKC-ECD TEMPERAMENT 2

fairly good by four years of age, 4,12 although improvements in effortful control continue into childhood. 13 Twin studies confirm that there is a genetic basis to effortful control. 14 However, parenting has also been associated with individual differences in effortful control. In general, young children s self-regulation (including behaviours that reflect effortful control) has been positively associated with maternal support and sensitivity, and negatively related to a directive and controlling caregiving style. 15,16,17 Moreover, children s heredity and quality of their attachment interact to predict their self-regulation; children with certain serotonin-related polymorphisms are more susceptible to the low self-regulation if they have an insecure (but not secure) attachment. 18 Finally, it is clear that effortful control is linked to optimal development, even in the first five years of life. For example, laboratory or parent-report measures of toddlers, preschoolers and children s effortful control have been associated with lower levels of problem behaviours, concurrently and at older ages. 19,20,21 In addition, young children s effortful control has been found to correlate with, and predict over time, low levels of negative emotion, 7,19,20,22,23 conscience, 19,24,25 high levels of social competence, 5,7,26 andhighly committed compliance. 26,27 Conclusions Although effortful control has a hereditary basis, it develops rapidly in the first four years of life, with marked improvements occurring in the third year. Individual differences in effortful control, although due partly to heredity, are also associated with the quality of mother-child interactions. Warm, supportive parenting, rather than cold, directive parenting, appears to predict higher levels of effortful control. Individual differences in effortful control that emerge during the first five years of life have been linked to higher levels of adjustment, social competence, committed compliance and conscience, concurrently and in the future. Implications The toddler and preschool years are a time in which temperamentally based effortful control emerges rapidly and provides the basis for the emergence of self-regulation. Self-regulation is critical because it affects the quality of children s social interactions and their capacity for learning. Because adults increasingly expect children to self-regulate as they mature, adults are likely to respond negatively to children who do not develop at least normative levels of self-regulation. Although individual differences are due partly to heredity, it is likely that socializers influence the emergence of children s effortful control; moreover, genes and the social environment interact in their effects on regulation. Because the quality of parenting is associated with higher levels of effortful control, it is important that parents and other caregivers be encouraged to interact with children in ways that foster the development of effortful control. Indeed, the relation between parenting style and a range of developmental outcomes is likely due in part to the effects of parenting on children s self-regulation. 28 Because of the relation between effortful control and healthy psychological and socio-emotional development, service-providers and policy-makers are well advised to implement procedures that promote supportive parenting and teacher-child interactions. 2012-2017 CEECD / SKC-ECD TEMPERAMENT 3

This research was supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and the National Institute of Health. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nancy Eisenberg, Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104. nancy.eisenberg@asu.edu. References 1. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Eisenberg N, ed. Social, emotional, and personality development. New York, NY: Wiley. Damon W, ed. Handbook of Child Psychology. 2006: 99-166. 6th ed; vol 3. 2. Kopp CB, Neufeld SJ. Emotional development during infancy. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, eds. Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2003:347-374. 3. Posner, MI, Rothbart, MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology 2007; 58; 1-23. 4. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Attention, self-regulation and consciousness. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B- Biological Sciences 1998;353(1377):1915-1927. 5. Eisenberg, N, Valiente, C, & Eggum, ND. Self-regulation and school readiness. Early Education and Development. 2010; 21; 681-698. 6. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Do children s attention processes mediate the link between family predictors and school readiness? Developmental Psychology 2003;39(3):581-593. 7. Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N., Vaughan, J., & Edwards, A. Relations of self-regulatory/control capacities to maladjustment, social competence, and emotionality. In:. Hoyle R, ed., Handbook of personality and self-regulation. New York: Wiley; 2010: 21-46. 8. Kochanska G, Murray K, Harlan ET. Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology 2000;36(2):220-232. 9. Ruff HA, Rothbart MK. Attention in early development: Themes and variations. London, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 1996. 10. Gerardi-Caulton G. Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children 24-36 months of age. Developmental Science 2000;3(4):397-404. 11. Rothbart MK, Ellis LK, Rueda MR, Posner MI. Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality 2003;71(6):1113-1143. 12. Reed MA, Pien DL, Rothbart MK. Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 1984;30(2):131-147. 13. Murphy BC, Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Shepard SA, Guthrie IK. Consistency and change in children's emotionality and regulation: A longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 1999;45(3):413-444. 14. Goldsmith, HH, Pollak, SD, Davidson, RJ Developmental neuroscience perspectives on emotion regulation. Child Development Perspective s, 2008; 2, 132-140. 15. Eisenberg, N, Smith, C., Spinrad, TL Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In: Baumeister RF & Vohs KD, eds., Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford; 2011, 2nd edition; pp. 263-283. 16. Eisenberg, N, Zhou, Q, Spinrad, T L, Valiente, C, Fabes, RA., & Liew, J. Relations among positive parenting, children s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 2005; 76: 1055-1071. 17. Belsky, J, Fearon, RMP, & Bell, B Parenting, attention and externalizing problems: Testing mediation longitudinally, repeatedly and reciprocally. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2007; 48; 1233-1242. 18. Kochanska, G., Philibert, R. A., & Barry, R. A. Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2009; 50; 1331-1338. 19. Kochanska G, Knaack A. Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality 2003;71(6):1087-1112. 20. Eisenberg, N, Valiente, C, Spinrad, TL, Cumberland, A, Liew, J, Reiser, M, Zhou, Q, Losoya, SH. Longitudinal relations of children s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology 2009; 45; 988-1008. 21. Eisenberg, N, Spinrad, TL, Eggum, ND Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2010; 6; 495-525. 22. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Nyman M, Bernzweig J, Pinuelas A. The relations of emotionality and regulation to children's anger-related reactions. Child Development 1994;65(1):109-128. 2012-2017 CEECD / SKC-ECD TEMPERAMENT 4

23. Kochanska G, Coy KC, Tjebkes TL, Husarek SJ. Individual differences in emotionality in infancy. Child Development 1998;69(2):375-390. 24. Kochanska G, Murray K, Coy KC. Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development 1997;68(2):263-277. 25. Kochanska, G, Aksan, N. Children s conscience and self-regulation. Journal of Personality 2006; 74(6); 1587-1617. 26. Spinrad, TL, Eisenberg, N, Gaertner, B, Popp, T, Smith, CL, Kupfer, A, Greving, K, Liew, J, Hofer, C. Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers effortful control to children s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology 2007; 43, 1170-1186. 27. Spinrad, TL, Eisenberg, N, Silva, KM, Eggum, ND, Reiser, M, Edwards, A, Iyer, R, Kupfer, AS, Hofter, C, Smith, CL, Hayashi, A, & Gaertner, BM. Longitudinal relations among maternal behaviors, effortful control, and young children s committed compliance. Developmental Psychology. In press. 28. Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL. Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry 1998;9(4):241-273. 2012-2017 CEECD / SKC-ECD TEMPERAMENT 5