Erratum to Age-related change in neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features [Cognit. Brain Res. 25 (2005) ]

Similar documents
Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Gamma and beta neural activity evoked during a sensory gating paradigm: Effects of auditory, somatosensory and cross-modal stimulation

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate

Attention and prepulse inhibition: the effects of task-relevant, irrelevant, and no-task conditions

Title change detection system in the visu

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Processing Interaural Cues in Sound Segregation by Young and Middle-Aged Brains DOI: /jaaa

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome

Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns

The Verification of ABR Response by Using the Chirp Stimulus in Moderate Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Design and ERP data: Basic concepts. Caitlin M. Hudac Graduate student, Developmental Brain Laboratory

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

International Journal of Neurology Research

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain

THRESHOLD PREDICTION USING THE ASSR AND THE TONE BURST CONFIGURATIONS

Differentiation of conversive sensory loss and malingering by P300 in a modified oddball task

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

C ritical Review: Do we see auditory system acclimatization with hearing instrument use, using electrophysiological measures?


The development of P50 suppression in the auditory event-related potential

EVOKED RESPONSE CORRELATES OF LETTER RECOGNITION

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Comparison of sensory gating to mismatch negativity and self-reported perceptual phenomena in healthy adults

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter?

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The impact of verbal framing on brain activity evoked by emotional images

Title of Thesis. Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm

1. Department of clinical neurology, Tbilisi State Medical University, Tbilisi, Georgia.

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Supporting Information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony Vroomen, Jean; Keetels, Mirjam; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P.

Expectations Modulate the Magnitude of Attentional Capture by Auditory Events

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Neural correlates of perceptual separation-induced enhancement of prepulse inhibition of startle in humans

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

Shirley Y. Hill, Stuart Steinhauer, Lisa Lowers, and Jeannette Locke. ERP Procedure

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

The relationship between epilepsy and schizophrenia

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Neurocognitive and Behavioral Indexes for Identifying the Amnestic Subtypes of Mild Cognitive Impairment

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Visual Selection and Attention

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

AUDITORY ERPS IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL COORDINATION DISORDER

The effects of covert attention and stimulus complexity on the P3 response during an auditory continuous performance task

Supplemental Material

Transcription:

BRAIN RESEARCH 1082 (2006) 205 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Erratum Erratum to Age-related change in neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features [Cognit. Brain Res. 25 (2005) 913 925] Michael A. Kisley a,, Deana B. Davalos b, Laura L. Engleman a, Paul M. Guinther a, Hasker P. Davis a a Department of Psychology, University of Colorado at Colorado Springs, 1420 Austin Bluffs Parkway, Colorado Springs, CO 80933-7150, USA b Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver, CO 80262, USA Due to a typesetting error at a late stage of production, all in-text references after Reference 43 should be decreased by one number to correspond with the references listed in the References section. For example, Ref. 45 in the text corresponds to Ref. 44 in the References section. This erratum applies to the print and online PDF versions only. All reference citations in the full-text version of this article on ScienceDirect are correct. The publisher apologizes for the error. DOI of original article: 10.1016/j.cogbra inres.2005.09.01 4. Corresponding author. Fax: +1 719 262 4166. E-mail address: mki sley@uccs.edu (M.A. Kisley). 0006-8993/$ see front matter 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/ j.brainres.2006.01.09 3

Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 Research Report Age-related change in neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features Michael A. Kisley a, *, Deana B. Davalos b, Laura L. Engleman a, Paul M. Guinther a, Hasker P. Davis a a Department of Psychology, University of Colorado at Colorado Springs, 1420 Austin Bluffs Parkway, Colorado Springs, CO 80933-7150, USA b Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver, CO 80262, USA Accepted 26 September 2005 Available online 27 October 2005 www.elsevier.com/locate/cogbrainres Abstract Aging is associated with changes in automatic processing of task-irrelevant stimuli, and this may lead to functional disturbances including repeated orienting to nonnovel events and distraction from task. The effect of age on automatic processing of time-dependent stimulus features was investigated by measurement of the auditory mismatch negativity (MMN) in younger (18 23) and older (55 85) adults. Amplitude of MMN recorded during a paradigm involving low-probability deviation in interstimulus interval (from 500 ms to 250 ms) was found to be reduced in the older group at fronto-central sites. This effect was paralleled by, and correlated to, decreased sensory gating efficiency for component N1 recorded during a separate paradigm involving alternate presentation of auditory stimuli at long (9 s) and short (0.5 s) interstimulus intervals. Further, MMN amplitude was correlated to behavioral performance on a small subset of neuropsychological tests, including the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, within a group of healthy older adults. The results support the hypothesis that aging is associated with declines in automatic processing of time-dependent stimulus features, and this is related to cognitive function. These conclusions are considered in the context of age-related declines in prefrontal cortex function and associated increases in susceptibility to task-irrelevant stimuli. D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Theme: Neural basis of behavior Topic: Aging Keywords: Mismatch negativity; Sensory gating; Auditory perception; Time perception; Aging; Neuropsychology 1. Introduction Aging is associated with changes in automatic processing of task-irrelevant stimuli [5,43], and this may lead to functional disturbances including repeated orienting to nonnovel events [45,85] and distraction from task [22,48]. Whereas studies employing behavioral measures have largely investigated age-related changes in top-down regulation of attention [58,107] (reviewed in [116]), eventrelated potential (ERP) methodology, because of fast temporal resolution and independence from overt behavioral * Corresponding author. Fax: +1 719 262 4166. E-mail address: mkisley@uccs.edu (M.A. Kisley). responses, has allowed for assessment of bottom-up, stimulus-driven processing. For example, age-related reductions in amplitude of the mismatch negativity (MMN: [48,104,134]) and novelty-p300 (i.e., P3a: [46,69]) waveforms provide evidence for changes in automatic identification and exogenous attention switching to novel stimuli (reviewed by [72]). It has recently been argued that the full extent of changes in automatic stimulus-driven processing might be difficult to document with behavioral methods, as top-down systems may provide some amount of compensation in older adults [5,31,43] (but see [74]). Automatic processing of the temporal (i.e., timing) properties of stimuli may be particularly compromised by age. For example, amplitude of the auditory MMN, elicited 0926-6410/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.014

914 M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 in the absence of selective attention to the stimulus, has exhibited the most consistent aging effects when variation of stimuli involved temporal parameters [104] (stimulus duration: [61,65,134], silent gap duration [5,18]) as opposed to parameters such as pitch or intensity, where a number of negative results have been reported [8,47,54]. The present study involved an investigation of age-related change in automatic processing of a different temporal parameter: the time between auditory stimuli. Further, we tested whether this function is correlated to decreased efficiency in the suppression of neural processing of low-salience stimuli (i.e., sensory gating ), and examined possible cognitive correlates of observed changes. The specific MMN paradigm employed in this study [66] involves variation in the time between serially-presented auditory stimuli, or the interstimulus interval (ISI) [41,97]. Like similar paradigms involving change in parameters such as stimulus loudness or duration, the MMN waveform is elicited by sudden deviation in a stimulus parameter, and is presumed to reflect a neural signal regarding the potential importance of the novel stimulus (reviewed by [88,106]). This idea is supported by demonstrations that larger (i.e., more salient) deviations, including deviations of ISI [4,66,76,92], lead to larger MMN amplitude [64,91, 128,130] and can also cause distraction as assessed both behaviorally and by the presence of a subsequent P3a waveform [115]. Since the auditory stimulus (50 ms, 1000 Hz tone) is physically identical for both standards (repeatedly presented stimulus) and deviants (occasional novel stimulus) in the present paradigm, the MMN waveform in this case can be assumed to result predominantly from automatic neural processing of the temporal interval between stimuli, as opposed to automatic processing of the stimulus itself [92]. Nevertheless, an observed age-related reduction in MMN amplitude elicited in this paradigm could arise from disrupted suppression of responding to the standard interval, and/or disrupted detection of the deviant interval [90]. Although present study design does not allow an explicit test between these two alternatives, it does provide for a preliminary investigation into this possibility by comparing, through correlational analysis, MMN waveform-generation to ERP-measures of sensory gating. Sensory gating studies typically involve documenting reduced neural responding as a function of reduced stimulus-driven salience (i.e., gating out stimuli of low importance). Amplitude of specific ERP components elicited by low salience stimuli presented at short-isis such as 0.5 s is typically compared to amplitude in response to higher salience stimuli physically identical but presented at longer ISIs such as 9 s [1]. This manipulation is based on the premise that, in addition to stimulus properties including semantic content (e.g., your name) and stimulus intensity, stimulus salience is proportional to novelty [16,63,88,99,124,125]. In this context, novelty is inversely proportional to the total number of exposures to a given stimulus, and directly proportional to the time since last exposure (i.e., the ISI). For the present study, sensory gating was assessed by measuring amplitude of auditory N1 as a function of ISI manipulation. Sensory gating of component N1 in this paradigm has recently been shown to correlate with behavioral observations concerning the tendency to be distracted by task-irrelevant stimuli [67]. Also, reductions in N1 amplitude with decreased stimulus salience appear to involve active suppression as opposed to simple habituation [119]. Thus, measures of N1 sensory gating are taken here to reflect reduced neural processing in response to stimuli of low stimulus-driven salience. For the present study, after demonstrating an age-related change in this neural function, we tested the hypothesis that sensory gating efficiency is correlated to MMN amplitude in older adults. We expected that stronger suppression of response to low salience stimuli in the sensory gating paradigm would be associated with more robust MMN waveform-generation. Possible cognitive correlates of age-related changes in MMN amplitude elicited by time-dependent variation of the stimulus train were also investigated here. Specific neuropsychological tests were selected based on previouslydocumented sensitivity to age (e.g., Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task, Reaction-Time Speed of Processing Task) and on posited relationships to prefrontal cortex (e.g., Stroop Color Word task, Tower of London Task, Wisconsin Card-Sorting Task) which is known to be involved in generation of the MMN waveform [3,7,87,100,113]. 2. Material and methods Procedures were approved by the University of Colorado at Colorado Springs Institute Review Board. Subjects gave written informed consent before participating, and received monetary compensation or academic extra credit upon completion of the study. 2.1. Participants and general procedures A total of 60 healthy adults participated: 20 younger adults (13 female) aged 18 to 23 (mean = 19.9), and 40 older adults (22 female) aged 55 to 85 (mean = 68.4). Exclusion criteria for all subjects included current diagnosis of psychiatric or neurological illness, current psychoactive drug use (prescriptive or illicit), and past traumatic brain injury (including loss of consciousness lasting at least 5 min after head injury). Older adults were also administered the Mini Mental State Exam [40], and all scored 27 or higher (mean = 29.4) out of 30. Hearing was tested at 1000 Hz (binaural, method of limits), and all subjects included exhibited less than 10 db hearing loss. Auditory stimuli were individually adjusted for hearing loss. Electrophysiological signals were recorded during two separate, randomly-counterbalanced paradigms. Both paradigms were passive, as participants did not respond to stimuli and watched silent, closed-captioned movies (all

M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 915 movies satisfied a subjective criterion of being appropriate for a general audience). For the temporal deviation paradigm, the MMN waveform was measured in response to deviation in the interstimulus interval (ISI) of an ongoing stimulus train. In particular, interruption of a standard ISI by a deviant ISI was found to elicit this waveform, as expected from previous studies [25,66,92]. In the sensory gating paradigm, suppression of component N1 amplitude was measured as a function of short- compared to long-isi: previous studies have shown that N1 amplitude in response to an auditory stimulus is proportional to the preceding ISI [1,29]. Both paradigms are illustrated schematically in Fig. 1 and described in more detail below. 2.2. Electrophysiology Electrophysiological activity was recorded with a NuAmps multi-channel amplifier system and Scan 4.2 software (Neuroscan; Sterling, VA). Sounds, adjusted for individual hearing threshold, were presented binaurally through headphones while participants sat in a reclining chair watching a silent, closed-captioned movie. ERP paradigms were programmed and presented with Eprime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.; Pittsburgh, PA). Before recording began, disposable Ag/Ag Cl electrodes (Vermed; Bellows Falls, VT) were affixed at Cz, Fz, right and left mastoids (RM and LM), directly superior and lateral to the left eye, and the tip of the nose. Ground was Fig. 1. Schematic diagrams of electrophysiological paradigms. Top: In the MMN paradigm, standard stimuli preceded by a 500 ms standard ISI were randomly interrupted by stimuli preceded by a 250 ms deviant ISI. Auditory stimuli consisted of 50 ms tones. Bottom: In the sensory gating paradigm, auditory click stimuli were presented alternately at long (9 s) and short (0.5 s) ISIs. Within each paradigm, physical stimulus properties were held constant; only ISI was manipulated. affixed to the forehead. During recording, signals were amplified 5000 times, filtered between 0.05 and 100 Hz, and sampled at 1000 Hz. All electrodes were referenced to nose, and impedances were maintained below 10 kv. Individuals who fell asleep during any portion of a recording were excluded from further analysis. 2.2.1. Mismatch negativity (MMN) paradigm and analysis Tones (1000 Hz, 50 ms duration, 5 ms rise/fall, 45 db hearing level) were presented continuously in a train at a standard offset-to-onset ISI of 500 ms. Standard tones were randomly replaced, on average every 20th trial, by a deviant tone at an ISI of 250 ms. Thus, this particular paradigm involves a temporal deviation, or deviation in a timing variable, with otherwise physically-identical stimuli [41,66,92,97]. A total of 2850 standard and 150 deviant tones were presented during a 30 min recording. Single-trial evoked responses, epoched from 100 ms before to 300 ms after tone onset, were baseline corrected and trials for which any channel exceeded T75 AV were discarded. Individuals that had fewer than 75 deviant trials remaining after artifact rejection were excluded from further analysis. Average waveforms were computed separately for standard and deviant tones, and band-pass filtered from 1 to 30 Hz (48 and 96 db/octave slope at these corner frequencies). This filter was applied both forward and reverse to eliminate phase distortion. Because the ISI preceding standard and deviant stimuli were different (500 ms and 250 ms, respectively), an analytical approach was employed to address the possible differences in ongoing activity present at stimulus-onset. Specifically, before filtering, a time-shifted average of the standard evoked waveform was subtracted from the deviant waveform to eliminate the overlap of these responses that results from a sudden decrease of ISI from 500 ms to 250 ms: a standard response waveform shifted forward in time by 250 ms was subtracted, point-by-point, from the deviant response (eliminating this subtraction step from the analysis was found to produce substantively similar results: not shown). Difference waveforms were computed by subtracting standard from deviant waveforms, point-by-point. For each participant, MMN was identified on electrodes Fz and Cz as the most negative trough between 100 and 200 ms poststimulus, and on electrodes RM and LM as the most positive peak between 100 and 200 ms poststimulus (MMN inverts polarity at the mastoids in a temporal deviation paradigm: [92]). Peak waveform amplitudes were measured relative to a 100 ms prestimulus baseline. Although not shown here, MMN amplitude was also measured as the mean voltage of the difference wave between 100 and 200 ms poststimulus. The correlation between MMN amplitudes determined in the two different manners was 0.96 (N = 36, P < 0.001), and the basic pattern of results presented below was essentially the same between methods.

916 M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 2.2.2. Sensory gating paradigm and analysis Acoustic clicks were alternately presented at 9 s and 0.5 s ISIs throughout the recording session at 60 db hearing level (HL). The evoked response at electrodes FZ and Cz to the clicks, termed long-isi and short-isi responses, respectively, were analyzed separately as follows: single trial ERPs, epoched from 100 ms before to 300 ms after each click, were baseline corrected and subject to artifact rejection (T75 AV). Individuals that had fewer than 75 long- ISI or 75 short-isi trials remaining after artifact rejection were excluded from further analysis. Average long- and short-isi waveforms were band-pass filtered, forward and reverse, between 1 and 30 Hz (48 and 96 db/octave slope, respectively). N1 was defined for the long-isi click as the largest negative trough between 75 and 125 ms. Short-ISI N1 was similarly identified, with the added restriction that the trough must be within T20 ms of the long-isi latency. N1 amplitude was measured relative to a 100 ms prestimulus baseline. Sensory gating was then quantified for component N1 by computinglong-i a suppression ratio: ðlong ISI amplitudeþ ðshort ISI amplitudeþ long ISI amplitude A suppression ratio close to 1 indicates robust suppression (very small short-isi response compared with long-isi response) and a ratio of 0 indicates essentially no suppression (short- and long-isi responses comparable in magnitude). A negative value indicates augmentation, where the evoked response to the short-isi stimulus is actually larger than that to the long-isi stimulus. 2.3. Neuropsychological tests Several domains of cognitive function were assessed with select neuropsychological tests. 25 of the older participants completed all tests described below. All tests, except Rey Auditory Verbal Learning and Stroop Color Word tests, were conducted through a computerized battery (Colorado Assessment Test v. 1.0, Colorado Springs, CO) [28,30,36] administered by a trained research assistant. 2.3.1. Tests of memory function The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) was administered by a trained research assistant. The RAVLT was used to assess declarative memory at different delays [111,117,126] and performance on this task declines with advancing age [30]. During 5 acquisition or immediate recall trials, participants were read a list of 15 words and asked to recall as many as possible. Dependent measures were number of words recalled on the first trial, and the total number of words recalled during trials 1 5 ( total acquisition ). After a 20 min delay, participants were asked to recall as many of the 15 words as possible. Dependent variables assessed were number of words recalled after the delay period and the forgetting score, which is the difference between the recall trial and trial 5 of the acquisition phase. A subset of these participants was later administered a separate word list, and the delay trial occurred either 1 h or 1 week after acquisition. Nonverbal memory was assessed with a visuospatial memory task similar to the card game of Concentration or Memory [15,27]. The participant matched 12 card pairs by turning over two cards per match attempt. The dependent variable was the number of repeat errors. 2.3.2. Frontal lobe tasks The Tower of London (TOL) task was developed by Shallice [122] to study problem solving in patients with frontal lobe damage, and has since been shown by regional cerebral blood-flow studies to engage the frontal cortex [86,95]. Participants moved 3 to 5 beads to and from 3 to 5 pegs, according to specific rules, with the goal of achieving a particular configuration shown on the right half of the computer screen. The dependent measures were the number of unnecessary moves and total time elapsed for 21 increasingly-difficult problems. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST: [59]) involves classifying cards based on a sorting rule (shape, color or number of symbols) that is not explicitly stated to the participant. The rule changes, again without explicit notification to the participant, after 10 correct matches. After a rule change, repeated perseveration on incorrect response strategies ( perseveration errors ) during the WCST is associated with an inability to inhibit prepotent responses [10,35], a function commonly attributed to the frontal lobe [34,112,121]. An abbreviated Golden Color Word Test was administered by a trained research assistant to measure susceptibility to Stroop interference [52]. The primary measure taken from this test is the increase in reaction time required by an individual to name an ink color (e.g., blue) that is used to print an incongruent word (e.g., red ) compared to a condition in which ink colors are named in isolation from words (e.g., blue symbols). Clinical patients who, after sustaining frontal lobe damage, perform poorly on the Stroop test tend to have difficulty staying focused on task [77,82]. West and Alain [132] have attributed an increased Stroop effect among older adults to a decline in inhibitory control. 2.3.3. Speed of processing task Speed of processing, known to decrease with advancing age [120], was assessed with three separate reaction-time (RT) tasks: simple, choice, and conditional. The three tasks require the same manual responses, but different amounts of cognitive processing [127]. The simple task involves pressing a key when a symbol appears on the screen. The choice task involves pressing one of two keys, depending on the symbol that appears (i.e., press left arrow if left arrow appears, press right arrow if right arrow appears). The conditional RT task requires participants to consider a

M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 917 conditional modifier ( + or ) when responding to a symbol flashed on the screen: a + presented with an arrow requires a response in the same direction as the arrow, whereas requires the opposite response direction. Dependent variables analyzed were reaction times for each task, increase in reaction time from the simple to choice task, increase in reaction time from choice to conditional task, increase in reaction time from simple to conditional task, and number of correct responses on choice and conditional tasks. 2.4. Statistical analysis Repeated measures ANOVA were conducted to assess age-related changes in electrophysiological variables between 20 younger adults and 20 randomly-selected older adults. Planned one-way ANOVA comparisons were also conducted between these groups for selected variables. Hearing loss, though corrected for by adjusting stimulus intensities for each individual, was originally included as a covariate, but found to be nonsignificant and subsequently dropped from all analyses. Within the full older adult group (N = 40), partial Pearson s correlation coefficients, controlled for age, were computed to examine relationships between selected ERP and behavioral measures. All significance tests were two-tailed at the 0.05 level. To protect against Type I errors, the degrees of freedom for all repeated measures ANOVAs were adjusted by the method of Greenhouse and Geisser [53]. All waveform amplitudes, whether from positive- or negative-going waves, are reported here as absolute values. 3. Results A significant age-related reduction in MMN amplitude elicited by temporal deviation of the stimulus train was detected. Specifically, after excluding for recording problems (N = 1) and artifact rejection (N = 3), MMN variables (summarized in Table 1) were compared between 18 younger and 18 older adults. Repeated-measures ANOVA with electrode as within-subjects and age group as between-subjects factors revealed a main effect of Table 1 Summary of MMN measures for younger (N = 18) and older (N = 18) adults Electrode MMN amplitude (AV) MMN latency (ms) Younger Older Younger Older M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Fz 5.87 (2.98) 3.72** (1.06) 142.2 (18.3) 147.6 (18.0) Cz 5.77 (3.05) 3.19** (1.57) 143.1 (17.0) 145.7 (22.1) LM 3.28 (1.49) 3.07 (1.75) 132.7 (16.5) 137.3 (18.4) RM 3.16 (1.18) 2.55 (1.63) 136.4 (13.3) 144.8 (15.0) Bold entries represent a significant difference between groups, one-way ANOVA, **P < 0.01. electrode, F = 10.37, df = 1.19,40.60, P < 0.01, a main effect of group, F = 11.42, df = 1,34, P < 0.01, and an electrode-by-group interaction, F = 3.94, df = 1.19, 40.60, P < 0.05. Planned comparisons showed that MMN amplitude was significantly larger in younger than older adults at central electrodes Fz, F = 41.67, df = 35, P < 0.01, and Cz, F = 60.22, df = 35, P < 0.01 but not at mastoid electrodes (RM, LM). These general findings are also reflected in the grand-averaged difference waveforms (bottom panel of Fig. 2). Although waveform latencies were generally longer for the older adults, no significant difference was detected at any electrode. Because the MMN waveform is computed as a difference between two waves, a separate analysis was conducted to determine whether the observed age-related difference at central sites related primarily to amplitude of standard (500 ms ISI) or deviant (250 ms ISI) evoked responses. These separable components can be seen for younger and older adults in the top two panels of Fig. 2. A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the voltage of the standard and deviant evoked responses at the latency of the corresponding MMN peak for each individual, with stimulus-type (standard or deviant) and electrode (Fz or Cz) as withinsubjects and age group as between-subjects factors. A significant main effect was found for stimulus-type, F = 157.76, df = 1,34, P < 0.001, with a type-by-group interaction, F = 11.15, df = 1,34, P < 0.01. The latter effect reflects a significantly more positive voltage in response to the standard tone for the younger group (marginal M = 2.13 AV above zero) compared to the older group (M = 1.03 AV), in contrast to a more negative voltage in response to the deviant tone for the younger group (M = 3.81 AV below zero) compared to the older group (M = 2.42 AV). In subsequent planned comparisons, the mean waveform voltage at the MMN peak latency was significantly different between age groups for both standard and deviant responses and at both Fz and Cz (a = 0.05). Thus, the larger negativegoing MMN difference wave in the younger group seems to depend on both a more positive standard response and a more negative deviant response. Examination of the standard response waveforms (Fig. 2) also suggests that N1, a negative deflection preceding the MMN latency window, was different between the two age groups. To test this, N1 amplitude was measured as the most negative trough between 75 and 125 ms poststimulus, and a repeated-measures ANOVA with electrode (Fz or Cz) as within-subjects and age group as between-subjects factors was conducted on this variable. Main effects were found for electrode, F = 23.98, df = 1,34, P < 0.001, and age group, F = 6.64, df = 1,34, P < 0.05. The latter effect arose because the mean N1 amplitude evoked by the standard tone was actually larger (i.e., more negative) in the older group (marginal M = 1.16 AV below zero) compared to the younger group (M = 0.12 AV). This general effect can be seen by examining the grand-averaged waveforms in Fig. 2, and suggests that the younger adults exhibit stronger N1

918 M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 Fig. 2. Grand-averaged event-related potentials recorded during the MMN paradigm. Top: Average standard (500 ms ISI, dashed line) and deviant (250 ms ISI, solid line) evoked responses recorded from younger adults (N = 18). Middle: Same waveforms for older adults (N = 18). Bottom: Difference waveforms, computed by subtracting standard from deviant responses, superimposed for younger and older adults. Note larger MMN difference waveform for younger compared to older adults, particularly at fronto-central sites (Fz, Cz). Also note polarity reversal at the mastoids (LM, RM) consistent with previous report [93]. For all plots, positivity plotted upwards. suppression, in response to the repeating standard stimuli, when compared to the older adults. 3.1. Age-related differences in suppression of component N1 In order to test more directly for an age-related difference in the suppression of N1 amplitude at short interstimulus intervals (ISIs), a sensory gating paradigm was employed with the same participants (2 further participants were excluded due to recording problems during this paradigm). Stimuli were presented alternately at long (9 s) and short (0.5 s) ISIs. Grand-averaged waveforms for this paradigm are shown in Fig. 3, and summary statistics presented in Table 2. A repeated-measures ANOVA for N1 amplitude on electrode Cz, with ISI as within-subjects and age group as between-subjects factors, revealed a main effect for ISI, F = 24.30, df = 1,32, P < 0.001, and an ISI-by-group interaction, F = 7.16, df = 1,32, P < 0.05. To explore these effects in a manner more typical of sensory gating studies, the suppression ratios were computed for component N1. For the younger adults, N1 amplitude evoked by the short-isi stimulus was generally smaller than the long-isi stimulus, as evidenced by the mean suppression ratio of 0.63 (i.e., mean response amplitude to the short-isi stimulus was reduced 63% from the long-isi response). N1 suppression ratio for the older adults, M = 0.34, was significantly different, F = 9.02, df = 33, P < 0.01, indicating less robust suppression as a function of ISI manipulation. As seen in Table 2 and Fig. 3, and in contrast to the MMN paradigm, the younger adults actually exhibited larger N1 amplitude in response to the long-isi stimulus compared to the older adults, F = 7.22, df = 33, P < 0.05. N1 amplitude evoked by the short-isi stimulus did not differ between groups, F = 2.09, df = 33, P = 0.16. The overall pattern of results at electrode Fz was similar (e.g., see Fig. 3). No effects for age or ISI were found for N1 latency at either electrode. 3.2. Comparison of MMN generation and N1 suppression in older adults To test whether MMN generation is correlated to N1 suppression in older adults, measures of N1 suppression in the sensory gating paradigm were compared to MMN amplitudes within a larger group (N = 35 after exclusions for recording problems and artifact rejection in both

M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 919 Fig. 3. Grand-averaged waveforms recorded during the sensory gating paradigm for 17 younger and 17 older adults. For each group, solid line represents evoked response to long-isi (9 s) stimulus, and dashed line represents evoked response to short-isi (0.5 s) stimulus. Note larger evoked response to the long-isi stimulus for the younger compared to older adults, as well as stronger response suppression when comparing the long- to short-isi evoked responses. paradigms). MMN amplitude measured from the temporal deviation paradigm was significantly correlated (Pearson s, controlled for age) to N1 suppression in the sensory gating paradigm at both Cz, r = 0.42, df = 34, P < 0.05, and Fz, r = 0.41, df = 34, P < 0.05. This correlation is illustrated for electrode Cz in Fig. 4, and is consistent with the idea that those individuals who exhibit stronger N1 suppression at shorter ISI in the sensory gating paradigm also exhibit larger MMN amplitude in response to temporal deviation in the MMN paradigm. 3.3. Comparison of MMN generation and neuropsychological function in older adults In order to test whether integrity of the neural system underlying the scalp-recorded MMN waveform is related to cognitive function in older adults, MMN amplitude and latency recorded during the temporal-deviation paradigm were compared to summary measures of 6 neuropsychological tests (Pearson s, controlled for age). No significant correlations were found with MMN latency, so those tests are not reported here. Although electrophysiological data were available for 38 older adults, only 25 of these individuals completed all neuropsychological tests. To reduce the likelihood of Type I errors, only one electrode (Fz) was considered. MMN amplitude was significantly correlated to the total acquisition score on the RAVLT, r = 0.45, df = 22, P < 0.05. This relationship, which reflects better immediate recall in those individuals that expressed larger MMN waveforms, is illustrated in the top panel of Fig. 5. MMN amplitude was also correlated to the total time required to perform the TOL task, r = 0.42, df = 22, P < Table 2 Summary of N1 sensory gating measures at electrode Cz for younger (N = 17) and older (N = 17) adults Variable N1 amplitude N1 latency Younger Older Younger Older M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Long-ISI 8.16 (5.30) MV 4.96* (2.12) MV 102.8 (13.1) ms 96.7 (12.2) ms Short-ISI 2.46 (2.02) AV 3.27 (1.13) AV 101.6 (11.8) ms 95.7 (11.2) ms Suppression ratio 0.63 (0.34) 0.33** (0.21) n/a n/a Bold entries represent a significant difference between groups, one-way ANOVA, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

920 M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 Fig. 4. N1 suppression in the sensory gating paradigm was correlated to MMN amplitude in the ISI-deviation paradigm in older adults (N = 35). Values taken from electrode Cz. Across these individuals, stronger N1 suppression was associated with larger MMN amplitude, even when age was controlled for, r = 0.42, df = 34, P < 0.05. 0.05. The direction of this correlation implies that individuals with relatively larger MMN amplitudes completed the TOL task more quickly. MMN generation was also found to be related to performance on a conditional RT task. Specifically, MMN amplitude was significantly correlated to the total number of correct trials during the task, r = 0.57, df = 22, P < 0.01, even with an apparent performance ceiling (see bottom panel of Fig. 5). In other words, individuals that generated larger MMN waveforms in the temporal deviation paradigm also performed better on the conditional task. MMN amplitude was not, however, correlated to reaction times in simple, choice, or conditional reaction time tasks. Also, no correlation was found when comparisons were made to the differences in reaction times between the separate tasks. No significant correlations were detected between MMN amplitude and behavioral performance on the WCST, Stroop, and Memory Card tasks, df = 22, a = 0.05. Interpretation of the present results depends upon the assumption that the MMN waveform was elicited automatically, in the absence of selective attention to the deviant stimuli (reviewed by [88,106]), in both age groups. Although the automatic nature of the neural process underlying the scalp-recorded MMN is especially supported by the demonstration that it can be elicited during sleep [14,79,94,96,102], it has been shown previously that MMN amplitude can be modulated by selective attention to the stimulus [33,129,133]. It is unlikely that a systematic difference in attention to the auditory stimuli existed between age groups, as both were instructed to watch the silent movie and ignore the tones. Selective attention directed towards the tones would have led to the elicitation of component N2b, a second negative component evoked by the deviant stimulus [98,114]. The peak latency of component N2b is later than that of component MMN for both younger and older adults [8]; however, no significant latency differences were found between groups in the present study. Finally, attention-related component N2b does not exhibit polarity reversal between central and 4. Discussion The basic finding of this study is consistent with the view that neural processing of stimulus-driven salience, specifically salience as determined by temporal parameters, changes with aging. This was demonstrated as an agerelated reduction in amplitude of the scalp-recorded MMN waveform elicited by deviation in the ISI of an ongoing acoustic stimulus train. The direction of change is interpreted to reflect less robust automatic identification of stimulus salience based on time-dependent stimulus features in the older adults. This argument, previously made based on results from MMN studies employing deviation of the temporal parameters of the auditory stimulus itself [5,18,61,65,104,134], is strengthened by the present study for which the stimuli employed were physically identical and only the time between stimuli was varied. Fig. 5. MMN amplitude (at Fz), recorded from temporal deviation paradigm, was correlated to behavioral performance on selected neuropsychological tests in older adults. Top: Individuals with relatively larger MMN waveforms acquired more words during immediate-recall (total performance over trials 1 5) in the RAVLT, r = 0.45, df = 22, P < 0.05. Bottom: MMN amplitude was also proportional to the number of correct trials (of 32 possible) during a conditional RT task, r = 0.57, df = 22, P < 0.01.

M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 921 mastoid sites [98,114], but both age groups exhibited such reversal here. In summary, there is no evidence to support the notion that selective attention to the tones differed between the two age groups. The observed age-related reduction in amplitude of the MMN difference wave was statistically related to the suppression of neural responding to stimuli of low salience. For example, amplitude of component N1 in response to the standard stimuli of the MMN paradigm was actually larger in the older adults, consistent with previous reports which generally attribute such effects to possible inhibition deficits of the neural representation of task-irrelevant stimuli [9,31,42,49,65,134]. By contrast, in the sensory gating paradigm, it was found that N1 amplitude was actually smaller for the older adults at long- ISI, but suppression of N1 amplitude from long- to short- ISI was stronger in the younger adults. Thus, the existence and direction of an N1 amplitude difference between older and younger adults depends upon the interval between stimuli, and this effect arises because N1 amplitude in the older adults is rather insensitive to ISI compared to the younger adults. Further, MMN difference waveform amplitude elicited by ISI-deviation (MMN paradigm) was positively correlated to N1 suppression efficiency (sensory gating paradigm) within a group of older adults. Clearly the latter result does not, by itself, demonstrate a causative relationship between suppression of neural responding to low-salience stimuli and subsequent automatic identification of high-salience stimuli (e.g., [2,90]). Indeed, other processes including those related to memory of ongoing sensory stimulation, for example, appear to be important for MMN generation [101]. At the least, though, the results obtained in the present study provide strong evidence that automatic neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features, even when measured during substantively different ERP paradigms, is modified with advancing age. The specificity of this effect to temporal parameters is supported by a recent failure to demonstrate a correlation between N1 suppression in the sensory gating paradigm and MMN amplitude elicited by deviation of stimulus intensity [67], although that study included only younger adults. Although evidence for age-related change in neural processing of time-dependent features was provided here by two separate ERP paradigms, the putative separability between the MMN and N1 waveform effects is more difficult to document. In fact, recent evidence suggests that these waveforms might represent scalp-recorded manifestations of the same neural system [62,83] (but see [93]), specifically the MMN elicited by stimulus novelty could essentially correspond to a disinhibited N1 response. Regardless of whether they are identical, there appears to be substantial overlap between the neural systems responsible for generating these waveforms, including especially auditory association and frontal cortices (N1: [6,51,80, 81,89]; MMN: [3,7,78,87,113]). Age-related change in frontal lobe function (reviewed by [60,109]) provides a parsimonious explanation for the present pattern of electrophysiological results. Robust, statistically significant age-related differences in MMN amplitude were seen at fronto-central sites, but not at mastoid sites. The mastoid MMN waveform is believed to reflect primarily activity in the superior temporal lobe (i.e., auditory cortex), whereas the frontal lobe contributes to the MMN waveform at Fz and Cz [3,7,87,100,113]. In addition to the role of the frontal lobes in generation of both N1 and MMN, prefrontal cortex appears to be involved in gating out irrelevant stimuli and in coding temporal intervals (reviewed by [70]). Regarding the former, both older adults [21,22,135] and prefrontal patients [71] exhibit enhanced primary auditory cortical processing (i.e., larger Pa amplitudes) of task-irrelevant sounds. In the domain of time processing, both groups exhibit deficits in the perception of intervals on the order of hundreds to thousands of milliseconds (older adults: [38,84,108], prefrontal patients: [56]). Interestingly, another patient population with pronounced hypofrontality exhibits a similar constellation of deficits to healthy older adults, though of greater severity: schizophrenia patients exhibit reduced MMN amplitude in an ISI-deviation paradigm [25], N1 suppression deficits [1,20], and temporal processing impairments [24,26]. Frontal lobe changes in older adults could also be responsible for the observed positive correlation between MMN amplitude and total acquisition score on the RAVLT. The acquisition or immediate recall portion of this task involves short-term memory which depends upon the integrity of prefrontal cortex (for recent reviews see [23,103]). On the other hand, words immediately recalled after trial 1 might be considered to provide a purer estimate of short-term memory no repetition and/or consolidation have occurred yet but this variable was not significantly correlated to MMN amplitude, although the direction of relationship was as expected: r = 0.29, P = 0.13. Nevertheless, the acquisition phase of the RAVLT task has been shown to involve increased metabolism in prefrontal cortex [11]. Another possible explanation for the correlation between MMN amplitude and RAVLT acquisition, particularly in light of the overall paucity of relationships detected here, is the modality of presentation. Of the tests administered, only the RAVLT involved auditory presentation of stimuli. In this context, a somewhat trivial explanation for the observed correlation is that those older adults with poorer auditory acuity exhibited smaller MMN amplitudes due to reduced activation of the auditory system, and worse performance on the RAVLT due to difficulty hearing the verbally-presented words. However, this explanation is unlikely because all auditory stimuli in the MMN paradigm were adjusted for individual hearing loss. Also, the correlation between MMN amplitude and total acquisition was retested with both age and individual hearing loss controlled for, and a significant

922 M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 correlation remained (r = 0.41, df = 22, P < 0.05). Another possible explanation involves age-related changes in temporal (i.e., time) processing. First, temporal perception is generally more closely related to auditory than to visual stimulation [37,55,110,123]; the opposite is true for spatial perception [17,68,131]. Second, age-related declines in temporal processing have been shown to be related to reduced auditory verbal comprehension [105]. Thus, it remains possible that the neural system studied here (i.e., the population of neurons underlying the scalp-recorded MMN elicited by deviation of time-dependent stimulus parameters) is involved in, or at least related to, temporal processing of auditory stimuli including some types of verbal material. In this very speculative scenario, older adults with relatively smaller MMN amplitudes would process time-dependent stimulus features with degraded fidelity, and as a consequence process auditory information less robustly. MMN amplitude was not found to be significantly related to performance on several other tasks including Stroop, Memory Cards, and WCST which are commonly associated with frontal lobe function. On the other hand, MMN was significantly correlated to performance on TOL; this task requires a number of psychological processes attributed to prefrontal cortex including planning and working memory [86,122]. Also, correct performance on the conditioning RT task, which was positively correlated to MMN amplitude, involves responding to a stimulus (e.g., a right arrow) with an opposing response (e.g., a left arrow). This behavior could be considered to require inhibition of the default response (e.g., press right arrow in response to right arrow), another function typically attributed to the prefrontal cortex [13,50,73,118]. In past studies of agerelated changes in automatic neural processing of taskirrelevant stimuli, significant correlations have been found between performance on the WCST and component Pa (primary auditory cortex response, approximately 30 ms poststimulus: [75,136]) within a group of older adults [22], and between Stroop performance and component P3a elicited by unexpected stimuli in an adult lifespan sample [39]. In conclusion, neural processing of temporal stimulus features changes with age. In particular, the robustness with which low-probability deviant ISIs are identified automatically, as assessed by MMN amplitude appears to decline. This is consistent with the hypothesis that aging is particularly associated with changes in the identification of (and subsequent exogenous attention switching to) stimuli based on their temporal features [18,104]. Further, in agreement with previous aging studies (see especially [22,48]), age-related impairments in the gating in [19] of high salience stimuli appear to be related to impairments in gating out of low salience stimuli, demonstrated here by a correlation between MMN amplitude in the ISIdeviation paradigm and N1 suppression in the sensory gating paradigm. The potential behavioral significance of age-related changes in the neural system that underlies the scalp-recorded MMN waveform is suggested by observed correlations between MMN amplitude and cognitive function. However, the basis for this correlation whether related to select aspects of prefrontal cortex function, auditory temporal processing, or otherwise remains to be determined. But the present results are at least consistent with broader theories linking age-related prefrontal functional declines with impaired filtering of task-irrelevant information [12,32,57,69]. Acknowledgments Technical assistance was provided by Janice Trew, Tara Noecker, and Christina Burrows. This work was supported by funds from the Wallace Research Foundation and the Coleman Institute for Cognitive Disabilities. References [1] L.E. Adler, E. Pachtman, R.D. Franks, M. Pecevich, M.C. Waldo, R. Freedman, Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia, Biol. Psychiatry 17 (1982) 639 654. [2] C. Alain, D.L. Woods, Age-related changes in processing auditory stimuli during visual attention: evidence for deficits in inhibitory control and sensory memory, Psychol. Aging 14 (1999) 507 519. [3] C. Alain, D.L. Woods, R.T. Knight, A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans, Brain Res. 812 (1998) 23 37. [4] C. Alain, F. Cortese, T.W. Picton, Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing, NeuroReport 10 (1999) 2429 2434. [5] C. Alain, K.L. McDonald, J.M. Ostroff, B. Schneider, Aging: a switch from automatic to controlled processing of sounds? Psychol. Aging 19 (2004) 125 133. [6] M. Alcaini, M.H. Giard, M. Thevenet, J. Pernier, Two separate frontal components in the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked response, Psychophysiology 31 (1994) 611 615. [7] K. Alho, D.L. Woods, A. Algazi, R.T. Knight, R. Naatanen, Lesions of frontal cortex diminish the auditory mismatch negativity, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91 (1994) 353 362. [8] E. Amenedo, F. Diaz, Automatic and effortful processes in auditory memory reflected by event-related potentials. Age-related findings, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108 (1998) 361 369. [9] E. Amenedo, F. Diaz, Ageing-related changes in the processing of attended and unattended standard stimuli, NeuroReport 10 (1999) 2383 2388. [10] A. Amos, A computational model of information processing in the frontal cortex and basal ganglia, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (2000) 505 519. [11] N.C. Andreasen, D.S. O Leary, T. Cizadlo, S. Arndt, K. Rezai, G.L. Watkins, L.L. Ponto, R.D. Hichwa II, PET studies of memory: novel versus practiced free recall of word lists, NeuroImage 2 (1995) 296 305. [12] T.Y. Arbuckle, D.P. Gold, Aging, inhibition, and verbosity, J. Gerontol. 48 (1993) 225 232. [13] A.R. Aron, T.W. Robbins, R.A. Poldrack, Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex, Trends Cogn. Sci. 8 (2004) 170 177. [14] M. Atienza, J.L. Cantero, C.M. Gomez, Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep, Psychophysiology 37 (2000) 485 493.

M.A. Kisley et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 25 (2005) 913 925 923 [15] L. Baker-Ward, P.A. Omstein, Age differences in visual spatial memory performance: do children really out-perform adults when playing Concentration, Bull. Psychon. Soc. 26 (1998) 331 332. [16] D.E. Berlyne, M.A. Craw, P.H. Salapatek, J.L. Lewis, Novelty, complexity, incongruity, extrinsic motivation, and the GSR, J. Exp. Psychol. 66 (1963) 560 567. [17] P. Bertelson, G. Aschersleben, Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location, Psychon. Bull. Rev. 5 (1998) 482 489. [18] S. Bertoli, J. Smurzynski, R. Probst, Temporal resolution in young and elderly subjects as measured by mismatch negativity and a psychoacoustic gap detection task, Clin. Neurophysiol. 113 (2002) 396 406. [19] N.N. Boutros, A. Belger, Midlatency evoked potentials attenuation and augmentation reflect different aspects of sensory gating, Biol. Psychiatry 45 (1999) 917 922. [20] N.N. Boutros, A. Belger, D. Campbell, C. D Souza, J. Krystal, Comparison of four components of sensory gating in schizophrenia and normal subjects: a preliminary report, Psychiatry Res. 88 (1999) 119 130. [21] R.D. Chambers, S.K. Griffiths, Effects of age on the adult auditory middle latency response, Hear. Res. 51 (1991) 1 10. [22] L.L. Chao, R.T. Knight, Prefrontal deficits in attention and inhibitory control with aging, Cereb. Cortex 7 (1997) 63 69. [23] C.E. Curtis, M. D Esposito, Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory, Trends Cogn. Sci. 7 (2003) 415 423. [24] D.B. Davalos, M.A. Kisley, R.G. Ross, Deficits in auditory and visual temporal perception in schizophrenia, Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 7 (2002) 273 282. [25] D.B. Davalos, M.A. Kisley, S.D. Polk, R.G. Ross, Mismatch negativity in detection of interval duration deviation in schizophrenia, NeuroReport 14 (2003) 1283 1286. [26] D.B. Davalos, M.A. Kisley, R.G. Ross, Effects of interval duration on temporal processing in schizophrenia, Brain Cogn. 52 (2003) 295 301. [27] H.P. Davis, F.R. Keller, Colorado assessment test manual, Colorado Assessment Tests, Colorado Springs, Colorado, 1998. [28] H.P. Davis, K.J. Klebe, A longitudinal study of the performance of the elderly and young on the Tower of Hanoi puzzle and Rey recall, Brain Cogn. 46 (2001) 95 99. [29] H. Davis, T. Mast, N. Yoshie, S. Zerlin, The slow response of the human cortex to auditory stimuli: recovery process, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 21 (1966) 105 113. [30] H.P. Davis, S.A. Small, Y. Stern, R. Mayeux, S.N. Feldstein, F.R. Keller, Acquisition, recall, and forgetting of verbal information in long-term memory by young, middle-aged, and elderly individuals, Cortex 39 (2003) 1063 1091. [31] A.R. De Chicchis, M. Carpenter, J.L. Cranford, M.R. Hymel, Electrophysiologic correlates of attention versus distraction in young and elderly listeners, J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 13 (2002) 383 391. [32] F.N. Dempster, The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging, Dev. Rev. 12 (1992) 45 75. [33] A. Dittmann-Balcar, R. Thienel, U. Schall, Attention-dependent allocation of auditory processing resources as measured by mismatch negativity, NeuroReport 10 (1999) 3749 3753. [34] T. Egner, J. Hirsch, The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task, NeuroImage 24 (2005) 539 547. [35] J. Everett, K. Lavoie, J.F. Gagnon, N. Gosselin, Performance of patients with schizophrenia on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 26 (2001) 123 130. [36] S.N. Feldstein, F.R. Keller, R.E. Portman, R.L. Durham, K.J. Klebe, H.P. Davis, A comparison of computerized and standard versions of the Wisconsin Card Sorting test, Clin. Neuropsychol. 13 (1999) 303 313. [37] R. Fendrich, P.M. Corballis, The temporal cross-capture of audition and vision, Percept. Psychophys. 63 (2001) 719 725. [38] P.J. Fitzgibbons, S. Gordon-Salant, Aging and temporal discrimination in auditory sequences, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109 (2001) 2955 2963. [39] A.M. Fjell, K.B. Walhovd, P3a and neuropsychological Ffrontal_ tests in aging, Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 10 (2003) 169 181. [40] M.F. Folstein, S.E. Folstein, P.R. McHugh, Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician, J. Psychiatr. Res. 12 (1975) 189 198. [41] J.M. Ford, S.A. Hillyard, Event-related potentials (ERPs) to interruptions of a steady rhythm, Psychophysiology 18 (1981) 322 330. [42] J.M. Ford, A. Pfefferbaum, Event-related potentials and eyeblink responses in automatic and controlled processing: effects of age, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 78 (1991) 361 377. [43] J.M. Ford, W.T. Roth, B.G. Isaacks, J.R. Tinklenberg, J. Yesavage, A. Pfefferbaum, Automatic and effortful processing in aging and dementia: event-related brain potentials, Neurobiol. Aging 18 (1997) 169 180. [44] D. Friedman, G.V. Simpson, ERP amplitude and scalp distribution to target and novel events: effects of temporal order in young, middle-aged and older adults, Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2 (1994) 49 63. [45] D. Friedman, G. Simpson, M. Hamberger, Age-related changes in scalp topography to novel and target stimuli, Psychophysiology 30 (1993) 383 396. [46] H. Gaeta, D. Friedman, W. Ritter, J. Cheng, An event-related potential study of age-related changes in sensitivity to stimulus deviance, Neurobiol. Aging 19 (1998) 447 459. [47] H. Gaeta, D. Friedman, W. Ritter, J. Cheng, An event-related potential evaluation of involuntary attentional shifts in young and older adults, Psychol. Aging 16 (2001) 55 68. [48] H. Gaeta, D. Friedman, W. Ritter, G. Hunt, Age-related changes in neural trace generation of rule-based auditory features, Neurobiol. Aging 23 (2002) 443 455. [49] H. Garavan, T.J. Ross, K. Murphy, R.A. Roche, E.A. Stein, Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection, and correction, NeuroImage 17 (2002) 1820 1829. [50] M.H. Giard, F. Perrin, J.F. Echallier, M. Thevenet, J.C. Froment, J. Pernier, Dissociation of temporal and frontal components in the human auditory N1 wave: a scalp current density and dipole model analysis, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 92 (1994) 238 252. [51] C. Golden, S. Freshwater, A Manual for the Adult Stroop Color and Word Test, Stoelting, Chicago, IL, 2002. [52] S.W. Greenhouse, S. Geisser, On methods in the analysis of profile data, Psychometrika 24 (1959) 95 112. [53] T.C. Gunter, J.L. Jackson, G. Mulder, Focussing on aging: an electrophysiological exploration of spatial and attentional processing during reading, Biol. Psychol. 43 (1996) 103 145. [54] S.E. Guttman, L.A. Gilroy, R. Blake, Hearing what the eyes see, Psychol. Sci. 16 (2005) 228 235. [55] D.L. Harrington, K.Y. Haaland, R.T. Knight, Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception, J. Neurosci. 18 (1998) 1085 1095. [56] A.A. Hartley, Evidence for the selective preservation of spatial selective attention in old age, Psychol. Aging 8 (1993) 371 379. [57] L. Hasher, R.T. Zacks, Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view, in: G.H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Academic Press, Inc., San Diego, 1988, pp. 193 225. [58] R.K. Heaton, G.J. Chelune, J.L. Talley, G.G. Kay, G. Curtiss, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual, Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., Odessa, FL, 1993. [59] T. Hedden, J.D. Gabrieli, Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience, Nat. Rev., Neurosci. 5 (2004) 87 96. [60] I.P. Jaaskelainen, R. Varonen, R. Naatanen, E. Pekkonen, Decay of