Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

Similar documents
How difficult is it? How well Adults with Aphasia Perceive Task Demands

Effort Invested in Cognitive Tasks by Adults with Aphasia: A Pilot Study

The effect of word familiarity and treatment approach on word retrieval skills in aphasia

Measuring Changes in Quality of Life in Persons with Aphasia: Is Communication Confidence a Good Measure?

The Relation between Language, Nonverbal Cognition, and Quality of Life in People with Aphasia

Background. Can APT-3 Improve Reading Impairment in Mild Aphasia?

Long-Term Recovery Outcomes in Aphasia

RECOVERY OF LINGUISTIC DEFICITS IN STROKE PATIENTS; A THREE- YEAR-FOLLOW UP STUDY.

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

The Functional Outcome Questionnaire- Aphasia (FOQ-A) is a conceptually-driven

INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION PURPOSE METHOD

Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia

Methods. Participants

APHASIA IN THE COGNITIVE DISORDERS CONTEXT PRELIMINARY STUDY

Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key

TITLE: Acquisition and generalization responses in aphasia treatment: Evidence from sentence-production treatment

Clinical Aphasiology Submission 2006

CLINICAL AND PROGNOSTIC PROPERTIES OF STANDARDIZED AND FUNCTIONAL APHASIA ASSESSMENTS

Comparing the outcomes of intensive and non-intensive context-based aphasia treatment

Validation of ipad based treatment 1

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations

Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g.

PERSEVERATION IN RIGHT HEMISPHERE BRAIN DAMAGED INDIVIDUALS

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Contextual bias and inferencing in adults with right hemisphere brain damage It is widely accepted that damage to the right cerebral hemisphere (RHD)

Maximizing Generalization Effects of Semantic Feature Analysis

Critical Review: Effectiveness of EMA in improving articulatory accuracy in adults with AOS

Critical Review: Inner and overt speech post-stroke: Is there a dissociation?

Utilizing Principles of Neuroplasticity to Guide Language Rehabilitation. Peter Meulenbroek, MA CCC-SLP University of Wisconsin-Madison

Pamela S. Klonoff, PhD Clinical Director Center for Transitional Neuro-Rehabilitation Barrow Neurological Institute, Phoenix, Arizona

LONGITUDINAL EVALUATION OF COGNITION AFTER STROKE A SCOPING REVIEW

A temporally dynamic context effect that disrupts voice onset time discrimination of rapidly successive stimuli

College of Health Sciences. Communication Sciences and Disorders

Method. Procedures and Measures.

Short-term memory refers to the ability to temporarily

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

CENTRAL EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH APHASIA. Artemis Alexandra Basilakos. Under the direction of Rebecca J. Shisler Marshall ABSTRACT

Carmen Inoa Vazquez, Ph.D., ABPP Clinical Professor NYU School of Medicine Lead Litigation Conference Philadelphia May 19, 2009 Presentation

METHODS. Participants

Feedback and feedforward control in apraxia of speech: Noise masking effects on fricative production.

Innovative Aphasia Intervention: Optimize Treatment Outcomes through Principles of Neuroplasticity, Caregiver Support, and Telepractice

fmri scans were compared pre- and post-treatment and perilesional activated volumes on the left hemisphere were compared.

Characteristics of Cognitive Impairment in Patients With Post-stroke Aphasia Boram Lee, MD, Sung-Bom Pyun, MD, PhD

Critical Review: What are the objective and subjective outcomes of fitting a conventional hearing aid to children with unilateral hearing impairment?

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA

Psy /16 Human Communication. By Joseline

Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet?

10/5/15. Should you conduct a speech, language, and cognitive screening on either or both of these patients?

Methods. Preliminary Results

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

3/23/2017 ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT NEEDS OF THE INDIVIDUAL WITH A TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY: A SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGIST S PERSPECTIVE

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles

Everyday cognition in temporal lobe and frontal lobe epilepsy

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Changes in maps of language activity activation following melodic intonation therapy using magnetoencephalography: Two case studies

Key Words: aphasia, working memory, memory measures

Satiation in name and face recognition

Word priming in schizophrenia: Associational and semantic influences

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

The neural basis of sign language processing

Language After Traumatic Brain Injury

A Modified Semantic Feature Analysis Approach With Two Individuals With Chronic Aphasia

Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown

Aphasia And Associated Disorders: Taxonomy, Localization And Recovery By Andrew Kertesz

What is aphasia? Katrina Clarkson Principal Speech and Language Therapist, Regional Rehabilitation Unit, Northwick Park Hospital

Impact of Personalization on Acquisition and Generalization of Script Training: A Preliminary Analysis. Abstract

Enhanced Visual Search for a Conjunctive Target in Autism: A Research Note

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Global aphasia without hemiparesis: language profiles and lesion distribution

Infant Behavior and Development

APPLIED NEUROLINGUISTICS /A 3 credits. September 11 November 27, 2012

Online Courses for Parents and Professionals Who Want to Know More About Children who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing

Speech Modifications for Supporting Auditory Comprehension in Aphasia

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Neuropsychological Test Development and Normative Data on Hispanics

Effects of Systematic Neurocognitive Rehabilitation after Moderate to Severe TBI- Results from a Randomized Controlled Trial

Repetition-priming effect: a cognitive task for the definition of a clinical assessment

Visual Selection and Attention

Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments

Severity of Post-stroke Aphasia According to Aphasia Type and Lesion Location in Koreans

Critical Review: Do mind-body therapies improve language outcomes in persons with aphasia?*

Evaluating technology for rehabilitation: A model for promoting programmatic research Ruth B. Fink, MA & Myrna F. Schwartz, PhD

The impact of dose on naming accuracy with persons with aphasia

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment

Empire BlueCross BlueShield Professional Commercial Reimbursement Policy

THE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF POST-POLIO FATIGUE. Richard L. Bruno, Thomas Galski, John DeLuca.

Course Outline. Course Directions. Cognition and Its Primary Domains: A Course for Clinicians. For cognition and each of its domains

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

Domain Group Mean SD CI (95%) t- value. Lower Upper. Clinical Attention & Orientation (18) <.05. Control

Psychological Tests that Examine Brain Functioning

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

Review Evaluation of Residuals of Traumatic Brain Injury (R-TBI) Disability Benefits Questionnaire * Internal VA or DoD Use Only*

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Adapting Patient Provider. with Communication Disorders

Mitigation of Proper Name Retrieval Impairments in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy

Transcription:

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis INTRODUCTION Since stroke survivors with high level anomia often score within normal limits on traditional assessments of language function, people with mild anomia are often underdiagnosed and underserved. Given the lack of diagnostic specificity and sensitivity for this population, the underlying cause of these deficits may be cognitive rather than linguistic (Moore, 2003). In the last two decades, a growing literature has emerged on cognitive approaches to aphasia and anomia. About 65 percent of stroke survivors exhibit impairments in cognitive processing (Donovan, Kendall, Heaton, Kwon, Velozo and Duncan, 2008), and executive function deficits are particularly noted in people with aphasia (Fridriksson, Nettles, Davis, Morrow and Montgomery, 2006; Helm-Estabrooks, 2002). Alexander (2005) noted the increasing inadequacy of the standard terminology of aphasia as recovery progresses, and suggested using the vocabulary of executive function to describe residual deficits more appropriately. The present study is a retrospective data analysis which examines mild anomia from a cognitive neuropsychological perspective, focusing specifically on selective attention and automatic vs. controlled processing. To examine these distinct yet related cognitive functions and their roles in anomia, the following research questions were posed: 1. Do individuals with mild language deficits have impaired performance on tests of selective attention relative to neurologically typical controls? 2. Do individuals with mild language deficits have impaired performance on tasks which require automatic vs. controlled processing relative to neurologically typical controls? METHODS Participants Participants included fourteen individuals who had experienced a left hemisphere stroke and self-reported mild anomia, and twelve neurologically typical, age- and education-matched controls. Inclusion criteria included native English-speakers, right-handed, with a Western Aphasia Battery AQ 90/100 and Boston Naming Test score of 50/60. Participants with anomia were at least six-months post onset of a single left hemisphere stroke. Exclusion criteria included a history learning disability, developmental language delay or attention deficit disorder, or evidence of diffuse brain injury or disease. Additionally, all participants completed a standard screen, which included the Wechsler Memory Scale (III) and Adult Intelligence Scale (III), Brief Visual Memory Test, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Copy, Ravens Progressive Matrices, and the Self-Rating Depression Scale. Instrument and procedure Participants completed two forms of the Covert Orienting of Visuospatial Attention Task (COVAT and COVAT+Read; Posner and Cohen, 1980). For both forms of the COVAT, participants sat in front of a computer, where two horizontally centered boxes appeared on the monitor. They were instructed to focus on a fixation cross between the boxes and press a button (hand unspecified) as soon as a target (large asterisk) appeared in one of the boxes. The target was presented in one of three conditions: cued/valid (target follows a prompt; i.e., brief highlight (wider, brighter) of the box s border), uncued (target appears with no box highlighted) and

2 invalid (target appears in the box opposite the one highlighted) conditions. Targets appeared equally within two interstimulus (cue to target) intervals (ISI): 100 and 800 msec. The 100 msec ISI is thought to assess automatic processing, while the 800 msec ISI is thought to assess controlled processing (Hagoort, 1993; Petry, Crosson, Gonzalez Rothi, Bauer and Schauer, 1994). The COVAT+Read task introduced a language interference component to test selective attention. In this task, participants were instructed to focus on the fixation cross between boxes, and read aloud a word which appeared in its place while continuing with the primary COVAT task. The one- and two-syllable, high-frequency, English words appeared 100 msec after trial initiation (and before a cue, if present). In total, the experimental task consisted of five blocks of 48 trials each (i.e., 240 trials), with one-minute rest between each block. RESULTS To answer Research Question 1 Is there a significant between-group difference on tasks of selective attention? a two-by-two repeated measures ANOVA was used to examine group differences on COVAT type (COVAT, COVAT+Read) by participant group (anomic, typical control) in the 800 msec ISI condition. The results showed no significant difference between groups on COVAT alone (F(1, 24) =.952, n.s.); however, significant between-group differences were found on COVAT+Read (F(1, 24) = 5.336, p <.05) with the anomia group significantly slower than typical controls. In other words, the anomia group s performance was similar to controls during the COVAT task alone, but slowed significantly when the task was performed with linguistic interference. See Table 1 and Figure 1. To answer Research Question 2 Is there a significant between-group difference on tasks which require automatic vs. controlled processing? a two-by two (COVAT type x ISI x group) repeated measures ANOVA was used. No significant difference was seen between groups in either ISI condition (100: F(1,24)=.972, n.s.; 800: f(1,24)=2.778, n.s.). Therefore, participants showed no difference from their typical counterparts on automatic vs. controlled processing when performing the COVAT task alone. See Table 2 and Figure 2. However, the 100 msec ISI during COVAT+Read also shows a significant between-group difference, F(1,24) = 14.547, p <.005, indicating a potential deficit in automatic processing in the presence of linguistic interference. See Table 3 and Figure 3. DISCUSSION When comparing the COVAT and COVAT+Read tasks at 800 msec, participants with mild anomia were significantly slower when linguistic interference was present. These results, similar to Murray, Holland and Beeson (1997), Murray (2002), and aligning with McNeil, Odell and Tseng (1991), provide evidence that individuals with anomia have difficulty attending to priority stimuli in the presence of linguistic distraction, i.e. they demonstrate impaired selective attention. Specifically, these individuals may not be able to appropriately suppress non-priority stimuli, even when the primary task is non-linguistic. These findings support the need for diagnostic protocols which include a detailed assessment of attention for people reporting mild anomia to more fully understand the impairment. Regarding automatic vs. controlled processing, participants with anomia show no difference from typical participants when examining automatic vs. controlled processing on the COVAT task in isolation. Post-hoc analysis, however, revealed a significant difference between

3 these groups when linguistic interference was introduced. The participants with anomia were significantly slower than typical controls at the 100 msec ISI on COVAT+Read. In other words, participants with anomia showed notably slower reaction times compared to typical controls during automatic processing on the task with linguistic interference, indicating possible deficits in automatic processing when linguistic processing is required. Consistent with previous research (Copland, Chenery and Murdoch, 2001; Petry, Crosson, Gonzalez Rothi, Bauer and Schauer,1994), individuals with anomia may not be able to inhibit items activated through the automatic processing mechanism of spreading activation. If inhibition during automatic processing is impaired, lexical selection during discourse may be difficult. It is also possible that automatic processing is slowed overall when processing resources are allocated for multiple tasks, one of which is linguistic. Conclusion This study provides evidence that deficits in selective attention may be a source of impairment for people experiencing mild anomia. Additionally, while automatic and controlled processing appears to be intact during task performance in isolation, automatic processing may also be impaired in the presence of a linguistic distraction. Given these results, it follows that assessment and treatment for mild anomia should consider these deficits as a likely component of the impairment. Furthering this line of research will ultimately improve service provision for those with mild anomia.

4 REFERENCES Alexander, M (2005). Impairments of procedures for implementing complex language are due to disruption of frontal attention processes. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 236-247. Copland, DA, Chenery, HJ and Murdoch, BE (2002). Hemispheric contributions to lexical ambiguity resolution: Evidence from individuals with complex language impairment following left-hemisphere lesions. Brain and Language, 81, 131-143. Donovan, NJ, Kendall, DL, Heaton, SC, Kwon, S, Velozo, CA, Duncan, PW (2008). Conceptualizing functional cognition in stoke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22, 122-135. Fridriksson, J, Neetles, C, Davis, M, Morrow, L, Montgomery, A (2005). Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 20(6), 401-410. Hagoort, P (1993). Impairments of lexical-semantic processing in aphasia: evidence from the processing of lexical ambiguities. Brain and Language, 45, 189-232. Helm-Estabrooks, N (2002). Cognition and aphasia: a discussion and study. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 171-186. Kaplan, E, Goodglass, H & Weintraub, S (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger. Kertesz, A (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune & Stratton. McNeil, MR, Odell, K and Tseng, CH (1991). Toward the integration of resource allocation into a general theory of aphasia. Clinical Aphasiology, 20, 21-39. Moore, AB (2003). VA RR&D Advanced Career Development Grant. 2003-2006. Linguistic and cognitive profiles of patients with mild aphasia. Murray, LL, Holland, AL, Beeson, PM (1997). Auditory processing in individuals with mild aphasia: a study of resource allocation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 792-808. Murray, LL (2002). Attention deficits in aphasia: presence, nature, assessment and treatment. Seminars in Speech and Language, 32, 2, 107-116. Petry, MC, Crosson, B, Gonzalez Rothi, LJ, Bauer, RM, and Schauer, CA. (1994). Selective attention and aphasia in adults: preliminary findings. Neuropsychologia, 32-11, 1397-1408. Posner, MI and Cohen, Y (1980). In Tutorials in Motor Behavior, GG Stelmach and J Requin (Editors), pp. 243-258. North Holland, Amsterdam.

5 APPENDIX Anomia Group Control Group COVAT at 800 msec 515.49 476.18 COVAT+Read at 800 msec 654.61 497.75 Table 1. Group mean reaction times (msec) at 800 msec by COVAT type. Selective Attention 700.00 Reaction time (msec) 600.00 500.00 400.00 300.00 200.00 100.00.334.03 Anomia group Control group 0.00 COVAT COVAT+ COVAT type at 800 msec ISI Figure 1. Group mean reaction times (msec) by COVAT type at 800 msec. Anomia Group Control Group COVAT at 100 msec 592.41 507.59 COVAT at 800 msec 515.49 476.18 Table 2. Group mean reaction times (msec) at 100 and 800 msec for COVAT alone.

6 Autom atic vs. Controlled Processing 700.00 Reaction time (msec) 600.00 500.00 400.00 300.00 200.00 100.00.108.334 Anomia group Control group 0.00 100 800 Interstimulus intervals (msec) for COVAT Figure 2. Group mean reaction times (msec) by ISI for COVAT alone. Anomia Group Control Group COVAT+Read at 100 msec 943.70 603.26 COVAT+Read at 800 msec 654.61 497.75 Table 3. Mean reaction times (msec) for each group at 100 and 800 msec for COVAT+Read.

7 Automatic vs. Controlled Processing in task w ith interference 1000.00 Reaction time (msec) 800.00 600.00 400.00 200.00.001.03 Anomia group Control group 0.00 100 800 Interstim ulus intervals (m sec) for COVAT+Read Figure 3. Group mean reaction times (msec) by ISI for COVAT+Read.