Aggression at School: Belief in a Personal Just World and Well-Being of Victims and. Aggressors. Jozef Dzuka 1, and Claudia Dalbert 2

Similar documents
The belief in a just world and distress at school

Belief in a just world, justice judgments, and their functions for prisoners. Claudia Dalbert and Eva Filke

Belief in a Just World and Self-Efficacy to Promote Justice in the World Predict Helping Attitudes, but only among Volunteers

M.A. Alhad & S.S. Turnip Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia

INFLUENCE OF PERSONALITY ON CONDUCT DISORDER AMONG SECONDARY SCHOOL ADOLESCENTS

Introduction. The current project is derived from a study being conducted as my Honors Thesis in

ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN SELF-RATED HEALTH AND PERSONALITY

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Personality. Chapter 13

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

BODY IMAGE IN DANCERS

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Multiple Act criterion:

Big five personality traits and coping styles predict subjective wellbeing: A study with a Turkish Sample

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

Perfectionism and the Big Five: Joachim Stoeber. University of Kent. Kathleen Otto. University of Leipzig. Claudia Dalbert

Running head: ANGER AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1. The Effect of Third-party Anger on Prosocial Behavior. Bachelor s Thesis. Tilburg University

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE

Client Personality and Preference for Counseling Approach: Does Match Matter?

Mediating Effects of Growth Mindset in the Relationship between Employment Stress and Happiness of University Students in Korea

University of Warwick institutional repository:

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Abstract. Asia-pacific Journal of Convergent Research Interchange Vol.4, No.2 June 30 (2018), pp

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Perfectionistic concerns suppress associations between perfectionistic strivings and positive life outcomes

All Types of Mortality Salience Are Not Equal: The Effect of Contemplating Natural versus Unnatural Death on. Materialism Behavior

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs

Thriving in College: The Role of Spirituality. Laurie A. Schreiner, Ph.D. Azusa Pacific University

Relationship between Teachers' Personality Traits and Self Efficacy: An Empirical Analysis of School Teachers in Karaikal Region (Puducherry)

Personality: Definitions

Running head: EXAMINATION OF THE BIG FIVE 1

Exam 2 Problem Items

T H E J U R Y E X P E R T. Just World Jurors. By Alison K. Bennett

SUMMARY chapter 1 chapter 2

Hardiness and optimism as moderators of cognitive emotion regulation. strategies in coping negative life events by adolescents.

Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions

1/23/2012 PERSONALITY. Personality. THE JOURNEY OF ADULTHOOD Barbara R. Bjorklund

Study of Meditational Role of Self-Esteem in the Relationship Between Perfectionism and Competitive Anxiety Elite Athletes

Association Between Distributive and Procedural Justice and Life Satisfaction Among Correctional Staff: Research Note

Commentary: The role of intrapersonal psychological variables in academic school learning

Master Thesis: Belief in a Just World & Language Abstraction: Victim Derogation through Language Abstraction.

Abstract. In this paper, I will analyze three articles that review the impact on conflict on

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

International Conference on Economy, Management and Education Technology (ICEMET 2015)

Athanasia Papathanasiou & Frosso Motti-Stefanidi University of Athens

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision

The Doctrine of Traits. Lecture 29

The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees

Adaptation of immigrant middle school students: The role of perceived discrimination against the self and against the group, and of acculturation

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Where I found hope. Young Male, Recent HIV+ diagnosis, Rejected by Family, Homeless and living under bridge. Hopeless right?

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology

Encyclopedia of Counseling Personality Theories, Traits

ADHD Explanation 2: Oppositional defiant disorder and MERIM

GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PREDICTING UNSAFE DRIVING BEHAVIORS IN YOUNG ADULTS

Violence and Mental Illness Resources Wednesday, August 16, :30 PM 5:00 PM

Chapter 8: Regression

Focus of Today s Presentation. Partners in Healing Model. Partners in Healing: Background. Data Collection Tools. Research Design

Relationship between assertiveness and social anxiety with happiness 1

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

Overview. Classification, Assessment, and Treatment of Childhood Disorders. Criteria for a Good Classification System

IJPSS Volume 2, Issue 7 ISSN:

1. procrastination 2. Lay, C. H. 3 Ferrari, J. R. 4. Steel, P. 5 Brothen, T.

Replications and Refinements. Effects of Own Versus Other s Fair Treatment on Positive Emotions: A Field Study

Psych 305A: Lecture 22. Wrap Up Emotions and Personality. Begin Relationships. Exam 5 on Friday. Wrap of evolutionary approach Cognitive Approach

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

International Workshop on Military Recruitment and Retention in the 21 st century

TRAITS APPROACH. Haslinda Sutan Ahmad Nawi. Faculty of Computer Science and Information Technology Universiti Selangor, Selangor, Malaysia

Rosenthal, Montoya, Ridings, Rieck, and Hooley (2011) Appendix A. Supplementary material

Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs -- A Pilot Study

Results and Discussion Section

ATTITUDE OF B.Ed. PUPIL-TEACHERS OF SCIENCE AND ARTS STREAMS TOWARDS CREATIVE TEACHING: A STUDY

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

Kent Academic Repository

The Humanistic Perspective

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI

IN PRESS PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

IMPROVING INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS. Facilitator: Ms. Vu Viet Hang (M.Ed)

10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals

Define the following term Criminal Describe a general profile of an offender with regards to culture, ethnic diversity, gender and age.

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Interpretive Report. Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D.

The Relation Between Happiness and Number of Siblings. Michael W. Passer. University of Washington

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

The Difference Analysis between Demographic Variables and Personal Attributes The Case of Internal Auditors in Taiwan

PYSC 333 Psychology of Personality

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior

(CORRELATIONAL DESIGN AND COMPARATIVE DESIGN)

THE IMPACT OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN THE CONTEXT OF LANGUAGE LEARNING AND TEACHING

Chinese Personality Traits and Mental Health: Mediating Effect of Coping Style

GCSE Psychology PAPER 1 REVISION BOOKLET. Name: Target grade: Predicted grade: Exam dates:

DOES SELF-EMPLOYED WORK MAKE 15-YEAR LONGITUDINAL PERSONALITY- BASED ANALYSIS

AN EMPIRICAL RESEARCH ON ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS: STUDENTS PERCEPTIONS

The Relationship between Personality Styles and Academic Self- Efficacy among Jordanian EFL Learners

JUST-WORLD BELIEFS AND MODELS OF PERSONALITY 1. Differential Associations of Personal and General Just-World Beliefs with the Five-

Transcription:

Aggression at School: Belief in a Personal Just World and Well-Being of Victims and Aggressors. Jozef Dzuka 1, and Claudia Dalbert 2 1 University of Presov, Slovakia 2 Martin Luther University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany Dzuka, J. & Dalbert, C. (2007). Aggression at School: Belief in a Personal Just World and Well-Being of Victims and Aggressors. Studia Psychologica, 49, 313-320. * Corresponding author: Jozef Dzuka, Inštitút psychológie FF PU, Ulica 17. novembra 1, 08078 Prešov, dzukaj@unipo.sk

2 Abstract This paper presents the study that apply just world research to the analysis of aggression at school. Based on previous findings that characterize the belief in a just world (BJW) as a valuable resource for maintaining positive well-being and assimilating injustice, the present studies test the hypotheses that: (1) personal BJW is positively correlated with subjective well-being; (2) This relationship holds for victims and aggressors alike. Overall, findings were in line with our hypotheses. The stronger the adolescents endorsements of the BJW, the better their well-being. The pattern of results persists when controlled for neuroticism and extraversion. The implications of these results for further studies on aggression, victimization, and well-being are discussed. Key words: aggression; just world beliefs; adolescence; well-being; victimization

3 Aggression at School: Belief in a Personal Just World and Well-Being of Victims and Aggressors. Aggression has been shown to have marked detrimental consequences for both victims and aggressors. Effects on victims include low self-esteem, depression, and school failure (for a review, see Parker & Ascher, 1987); implications for aggressors include delinquent behavior (Rigby & Cox, 1996) and low levels of happiness (Rigby & Slee, 1993). We classified the following five acts as aggression: verbal aggression, physical aggression, stealing, manipulation of belongings, and forcing the victim to engage in adverse behavior aimed at his or her social isolation. We defined victims of aggression as students who had experienced at least one of these acts recently, and perpetrators of aggression as students who had performed one of these acts recently. We do not consider repetition to be a necessary condition for aggression (O Moore & McGuire, 2001), because even a single threat can cause emotional damage and concern about the future. The belief that events in one s life are just, as reflected in the personal belief in a just world (BJW), seems to be of particular relevance to emotional development at school. It serves as a resource that bolsters subjective well-being in general (Dalbert, 2001), and in adolescents (e.g., Dzuka & Dalbert, 2002) and at school in particular (e.g., Dalbert & Stoeber, 2005). Against this background, the study presented in this paper test the association between personal BJW and well-being in the context of aggression. More specifically, we examine whether this association holds for victims and aggressors, and whether strength of BJW is associated with victim and aggressor status. According to the just world hypothesis, "people want to and have to believe they live in a just world so that they can go about their daily lives with a sense of trust, hope, and confidence in their future" (Lerner, 1980, p. 14). Three functions of this BJW have been identified (Dalbert, 2001). The first is to compel individuals to behave justly, for example,

4 high believers in a just world will be motivated to achieve their personal goals by just means (e.g., Hafer, 2000). By acting justly, they respect the terms of their personal contract (Lerner, 1980), which gives them the prospect of being justly rewarded. Consequently, as its second function, BJW endows people with trust in others and in the justice of their fate. This trust has several adaptive consequences - it gives individuals the confidence to invest in long-term goals (e.g. Dalbert & Sallay, 2004; Otto & Dalbert, 2005), to trust others to treat them fairly, and to be rewarded justly. As a third function, BJW provides a framework that helps individuals to interpret the events of their life in a meaningful way (e.g., Hafer & Correy, 1999), thus protecting the BJW, by restoring justice either psychologically (e.g., minimizing the injustice) or behaviorally (e.g., compensating the injustice). These three functions of the BJW can be assumed to mediate the positive relationship between BJW and well-being in the victims of aggression at school and its perpetrators. Aggression is clearly a threatening and unjust situation that may lead to decreases in the well-being and mental health of its victims. Based on the third function of BJW, we argue that BJW can be expected to buffer the well-being of aggression victims because strong believers are better able to cope with such unjust experiences. When individuals high in BJW are confronted with injustice, they try to compensate or justify it; for example, by blaming themselves or by playing down the unfairness (Bulman & Wortman, 1977). Consequently, BJW is seen as a buffer helping victims of injustice to cope with their experiences. Accordingly, we expect aggression victims high in BJW to report more positive affect and higher life satisfaction than victims low in BJW. Because just behavior is mandatory for students high in BJW, aggression may also threaten the aggressor s BJW. This threat can, however, be diminished by finding justifications for the unjust behavior. Aggressors can either reason that they are better than the other students and are therefore authorized to impose the appropriate behavior on their fellow students (Kolář, 2001), or they may justify aggression by blaming the victim,

5 reasoning that he or her deserved to be punished or provoked the aggressor in the first place (e.g., Říčan, 1995). Hence, a strong personal BJW is also expected to be positively associated with the well-being of aggressors. Recent investigations have shown that it is necessary to distinguish the belief in a personal just world from the belief in a general just world (Dalbert, 1999; Lipkus, Dalbert, & Siegler, 1996). The personal BJW reflects the belief that, overall, events in one s life are just, whereas the general BJW reflects the belief that, basically, the world is a just place. It has been shown that individuals tend to endorse the belief in a personal just world more strongly than the belief in a general just world and that the personal BJW is a better predictor of wellbeing than the general BJW (e.g., Dzuka & Dalbert, 2002; Lipkus, et al., 1996). We therefore focus on personal BJW in the present study. Research into the relationship between broad personality dimensions and well-being has revealed that extraversion correlates positively with the frequency of positive affect, and neuroticism with the frequency of negative affect (Costa & McCrae, 1980). Additionally, there is some evidence to suggest that personal BJW as a personality trait correlates positively with extraversion (e.g., Lipkus, et al., 1996) and, in particular, negatively with neuroticism (e.g., Dzuka & Dalbert, 2002). Thus, a significant association between personal BJW and well-being might be attributable to the fact that both constructs are related to global personality dimensions, namely neuroticism and extraversion. To test this possibility, neuroticism and extraversion were assessed as control variables. In sum, this Study tested the following hypotheses: (a) The more strongly students endorse the personal BJW, the less likely they are to report negative affect, and the more likely they are to report positive affect and high life satisfaction. (b) These associations hold for both victims and aggressors. (c) These associations hold when controlled for the global personality dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism. Method

6 Procedure and Sample The study was conducted in Slovakian vocational schools, which are mainly attended by male students preparing for blue-collar jobs. Vocational training lasts three years and culminates in an exam. The assessment was conducted in the classroom during lesson time. Participants were guaranteed anonymity. Participants were 195 male students aged between 15 and 19 years (M = 16.6; SD = 0.86). Based on their self-reports, 61 students (31% of the sample) were identified as victims of aggression, having experienced at least one act of aggression recently, and 49 students (25%) were identified as aggressors, having performed at least one act of aggression recently. The analyses that follow are based on these two groups. Additionally, 59 students reported no experience of aggression, and 26 reported being both victims and perpetrators of aggression. These 85 students were excluded from the following analyses. Measures BJW. BJW was measured using the Personal Belief in a Just World Scale (Dalbert, 1999). The scale comprises 7 items designed to capture the belief that, overall, events in one s life are just (α =.77; sample items: I am usually treated fairly, Overall, events in my life are just ) with a 6-point answer scale ranging from 1 ( totally disagree ) to 6 ( totally agree ). Global personality dimensions. To assess extraversion and neuroticism, we selected the 7 items with the highest item-total correlations from the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales of the NEO FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992; Czech version: Hřebíčková, 1997; extraversion: α =.72; sample item: I like to have a lot of people around me ; neuroticism: α =.70; sample item: I often feel inferior to others ). The 5-point answer scales ranged from 1 ( strongly disagree ) to 5 ( strongly agree ). Well-Being. Life satisfaction was assessed using the 5-item General Life Satisfaction Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; α =.68) with a 7-point answer scale ranging from 1 ( totally disagree ) to 7 ( strongly agree ) (sample item: I am satisfied with my life ). The emotional component of well-being was

7 assessed using the Affectometer 2 (Kammann & Flett, 1983), which consists of two scales-- Frequency of Positive Affect (24 items; sample item: joy ; α =.91); Frequency of Negative Affect (24 items; sample item: anxiety ; α =.91) tapping the emotions experienced over the past weeks with 5-point answer scales ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( always ). Scale scores were computed by averaging across items, with higher scores indicating stronger endorsement of the construct. Status. Each participant was asked two questions: first, whether they had experienced any of the five acts of aggression recently (verbal aggression, physical aggression, stealing, manipulation of belongings, and forcing the victim to engage in adverse behavior aimed at his or her social isolation) and, second, whether they had performed any of these acts recently. Students chose between the response alternatives not at all, once or twice, several times, and frequently. Those who endorsed at least one of the acts were categorized as victims or aggressors. Results First, to inspect mean differences between the two groups, a MANOVA was conducted on all measures, comparing the 61 victims of aggression with the 49 aggressors. Wilk s lambda did not identify any substantial differences between the groups (F 7/102 = 0.63; p =.73). Second, zero-order correlations between the key variables were inspected (see Table 1). Results show that personal BJW displayed significant correlations in the expected direction with both dimensions of well-being. The more strongly students endorsed the personal BJW, the more satisfied they were with life (significant in both groups), and the less negative affect they experienced (significant in the aggressors group). The negative correlation between BJW and negative affect was particularly strong in aggressors. Extraversion correlated with positive affect, and neuroticism with negative affect, in the expected direction in both groups, but the two global personality dimensions did not correlate with BJW. -- insert Table 1 about here --

8 To clarify the relationship between well-being and BJW, hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed for life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect, respectively. Following Aiken and West (1991) we centred the BJW before analyzing the data. We first tested the interaction: Status and personal BJW (centered) were entered in the first block, and the product term depicting the interaction of personal BJW (centered) with status was entered stepwise in the final block. If the interaction became significant, it was tested with personality included. Thus, status was entered stepwise in the first block; the two personality variables were entered stepwise in the second block; personal BJW (centered) was entered in the third block; to conclude, the product terms depicting the interaction of personal BJW (centered) with status was entered in the final block. The accepted models (p <.05) are presented in Table 2. -- insert Table 2 about here -- Overall, 19 percent of the variance in life satisfaction was predicted by the main effect of personal BJW and its interaction with status. The more strongly the students endorsed the personal BJW, the more satisfied they were with life. This association was much stronger for victims (b =.92) than for aggressors (b =.39). Twenty percent of the variance in positive affect was predicted by extraversion and personal BJW. The interaction of personal BJW and status was not significant (p =.49). The more extraverted the students were and the stronger their belief in a personal just world, the more often they experienced positive affect. Finally, the main effects of neuroticism and personal BJW explained 25 percent of the variance in negative affect. The interaction of personal BJW and status was not significant (p =.97). The more neurotic the students were and the weaker their personal BJW, the more often they experienced negative affect. Discussion In sum, results were in line with expectations. Personal BJW was associated with higher life satisfaction, more positive affect, and less negative affect, and these relationships

9 persisted when controlled for global personality dimensions and, particularly, victim or aggressor status. Only the association between personal BJW and life satisfaction differed in the two groups, with BJW proving to be a better buffer for victims than for aggressors. Finally, the groups did not differ in terms of their global personality dimensions, their wellbeing, or their personal BJW. Findings provided unambiguous support for the adaptive relationship hypothesized between personal BJW and dimensions of subjective well-being. The more strongly the students believed in a personal just world, the more satisfied they were with life, the less negative affect, and the more positive affect they experienced. Furthermore, these associations held for victims and aggressors alike. A differential pattern was only observed for life satisfaction, where personal BJW and life satisfaction were more strongly related in victims than in aggressors. Finally, the association between BJW and well-being persisted when other personality dimensions were controlled (extraversion, neuroticism). One shortcoming of our research is however the cross-sectional nature of our study, meaning that causal conclusions cannot be drawn for at least two reasons. We interpret BJW as a relatively stable personal disposition which influences subjective well-being and that it is not the other way round. There is some evidence supporting this notion (Dalbert, 2002; Dalbert & Stoeber, 2006). We cannot rule out however that our participants reported on more BJW the better their well-being was. Furthermore, other variables, such as social desirability, may also have contributed to the associations between BJW and well-being. We at least controlled for neuroticism and extraversion to exclude this alternative interpretation for two well known correlates of BJW and well-being. Longitudinal studies which take into account additional alternative explanations are needed for further corroborate our findings. Taken collectively, our data support the notion that the personal BJW acts as a buffer protecting well-being in various situations. In all of our students whether they were aggressors or victims a strong just world belief was associated with positive well-being. This pattern of

10 results is in line with the reasoning that BJW can trigger different coping patterns in different situations, but that all of these coping mechanisms seem to mediate the adaptive relationship between BJW and well-being. Victims may blame themselves. Aggressors may justify their behavior. The results seem to be the same across all groups: the stronger their BJW, the better their well-being. Future studies should examine the coping patterns mediating this relationship in more detail and to seek to elucidate the cognitions that are evoked in students high in personal BJW when aggression others, or experiencing aggression themselves.

11 Acknowledgements Study was completed during a research fellowship awarded by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation to the first author at the Department of Educational Psychology, Martin Luther University of Halle, Germany.

12 References AIKEN, L. S., WEST, S. G., 1991, Multiple Regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage, London. BULMAN, R. J., WORTMAN, C. B., 1977, Attributions of blame and coping in the "real world": Severe accident victims react to their lot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 351-363. COSTA, P. T., Jr., McCRAE, R. R., 1980, Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668-678. COSTA, P. T., Jr., McCRAE, R. R., 1992, Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. DALBERT, C., 1999, The world is more just for me than generally: About the Personal Belief in a Just World Scale's validity. Social Justice Research, 12, 79-98. DALBERT, C., 2001, The justice motive as a personal resource: Dealing with challenges and critical life events. New York: Plenum Press. DALBERT, C. (2002). Beliefs in a just world as a buffer against anger. Social Justice Research, 15, 123-145. DALBERT, C., SALLAY, H., 2004, The justice motive in adolescence and young adulthood: Origins and consequences. London, UK: Routledge. DALBERT, C., STOEBER, J., 2005, Belief in a just world and distress at school. Social Psychology of Education, 8,123-135. DALBERT, C. & STOEBER, J. (2006). The personal belief in a just world and domainspecific beliefs about justice at school and in the family: A longitudinal study with adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 200-207.

13 DIENER, E., EMMONS, R. A., LARSON, R. J., GRIFFIN, S., 1985, The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-76. DZUKA, J., DALBERT, C., 2002, Mental health and personality of Slovak unemployed adolescents: The impact of belief in a just world. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 732-757. HAFER, C. L., 2000, Investment in long-term goals and commitment to just means drive the need to believe in a just world. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1059-1073. HAFER, C. L., CORREY, B. L., 1999, Mediators of the relation of beliefs in a just world and emotional responses to negative outcomes. Social Justice Research, 12, 189-204. HŘEBÍČKOVÁ, M., 1997, Jazyk a osobnost: Pětifaktorová struktura popisu osobnosti. [Sprache und Persönlichkeit: Fünffaktorielle Struktur Beschreibung der Persönlichkeit]. Brno: Vydavatelství Masarykovy univerzity a Psychologický ústav AV ČR. KAMMANN, R., FLETT, R., 1983, Affectometer 2. A scale to measure current level of general happiness. Australian Journal of Psychology, 35, 259-265. KOLÁŘ, M., 2001, Bolest šikanování [Suffering from bullying]. Praha: Portál. LERNER, M. J., 1977, The justice motive: Some hypotheses as to its origins and forms. Journal of Personality, 45, 1-52. LERNER, M. J., 1980, The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press. LIPKUS, I. M., DALBERT, C., SIEGLER, I. C., 1996, The importance of distinguishing the belief in a just world for self versus for others: Implications for psychological wellbeing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 666-677. O MOORE, A. M., McGUIRE, L., 2001, Bullying at school. Dublin: A.B.C. OTTO, K., DALBERT, C., 2005, Belief in a just world and its functions for young prisoners. Journal of Research in Personality, 6, 559-573.

14 PARKER, J.G., ASCHER, S.R.,1987, Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are lowaccepted children at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357-389 ŘÍČAN, P., 1995, Agresivita a šikana mezi dětmi [Aggression and bullying in children]. Praha: Portál. RIGBY, K., COX, I., 1996, The contribution of bullying at school and low self-esteem to acts of delinquency among Australian teenagers. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 609-612. RIGBY, K., SLEE, P. T., 1993, Dimensions of interpersonal relating among Australian school children and their implications for psychological well-being. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 33-42.

15 Table 1 Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Victims and Aggressors Scale M SD Positive Negative Personal Extraversion Neuroticism affect affect BJW Victims (n = 61) Life satisfaction 4.12 1.27.45** -.25*.49**.11 -.07 Positive affect 3.01 0.67 -.15.23.40** -.15 Negative affect 2.39 0.71 -.21 -.04.40** Personal BJW 4.10 0.68 -.01 -.23 Extraversion 3.80 0.56 -.03 Neuroticism 3.07 0.61 Aggressors (n = 49) Life satisfaction 4.26 1.01.19 -.18. 34* -.05 -.11 Positive affect 3.17 0.53 -.08.20.41** -.04 Negative affect 2.17 0.47 -.42**.22.57** Personal BJW 4.12 0.88.01 -.11 Extraversion 3.80 0.57.36** Neuroticism 2.94 0.62 Note. Ratings of life satisfaction range from 1 to 7; of positive and negative affect, extraversion, and neuroticism from 1 to 5; of personal BJW from 1 to 6. In all cases, a higher value indicates stronger endorsement of the construct. * p <.05; ** p <.01

16 Table 2 Regression of Well-Being on Status, Global Personality, Personal BJW (centered), and the Interaction of BJW with Status (accepted models; p <.05) Predictors R 2 -change B β t p Life satisfaction (F total (3,106) = 8.669; p <.001) Status.00 0.14 Personal BJW.16*** 0.92 Personal BJW X status.03* -0.53-1.981.05 (Constant) 4.12 Positive affect (F total (2,107) = 13.811; p <.001) Extraversion.16*** 0.44.40 4.676 <.001 Personal BJW.04* 0.16.21 2.402. 02 (Constant) 1.46 Negative affect (F total (2,107) = 18.174; p <.001) Neuroticism.21*** 0.43.43 5.013 <.001 Personal BJW.04* -0.17 -.21-2.467. 02 (Constant) 1.00 Note. Status: 0 = victim, 1 = aggressor * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001