Overestimation of Skills Affects Drivers Adaptation to Task Demands

Similar documents
The Effectiveness of Pre-Driver Training

Cognitive and Psychomotor Correlates of Self- Reported Driving Skills and Behavior

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

Traffic Violations and Errors: The Effects of Sensation Seeking and Attention

Role of Monotonous Attention in Traffic Violations, Errors, and Accidents

PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF THE MANCHESTER DRIVER BEHAVIOUR QUESTIONNAIRE IN ROMANIA: VALIDATION OF A CROSS-CULTURAL VERSION

COMPARISON OF SELF-REPORTED AND COMPUTER-BASED HAZARD PERCEPTION SKILLS AMONG NOVICE AND EXPERIENCED DRIVERS

THE INFLUENCE OF RISK PERCEPTION AND SELF-ASSESSED DRIVING ABILITIES ON THE BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS

Social forgivingness and vulnerable road users

The effect of training approaches and feedback for young and novice drivers on the. relationship between the actual and perceived speed

Development of a Driving Attitude Scale

Social desirability and self-reported driving behaviours: Should we be worried? Mark J. M. Sullman. School of Psychology, Hertfordshire University, UK

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

On the Fast Lane to Road Rage

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics

Alcohol-free on the road (Alcohol-vrij op weg)

Chapter 5 Car driving

Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition

SAFE COMMUNITIES SURVEY RESULTS 2000

Perceptions of Risk Factors for Road Traffic Accidents

Measuring Road Safety Culture in Relation to Speed Susan Cambridge and Tony Francis Francis and Cambridge Ltd, Christchurch

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

How to change young drivers attitudes and behaviour with respect to drivingwhile-intoxicated

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

The Worldwide Decline in Drinking and Driving

A COACHING PROGRAM FOR RECENTLY LICENSED YOUNG DRIVERS IN THE NETHERLANDS: WHICH DRIVERS ARE ATTRACTED?

Predicting Older Drivers' Difficulties Using the Roadwise Review

Adjusted Crash Odds Ratio Estimates of Driver Behavior Errors: A Re-Analysis of the SHRP2 Naturalistic Driving Study Data

Modifications to Traffic Signal Operation to Improve Safety for Alcoholaffected

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

PSYCHOMETRIC AND RASCH ANALYSIS OF THE DRIVER BEHAVIOUR QUESTIONNAIRE (DBQ): IMPLICATIONS FOR ITS USE AS AN EVALUATION TOOL WITH NOVICE DRIVERS

COVER SHEET. Accessed from Australasian College of Road Safety. This is the author-version of article published as:

ROAD SAFETY (OR LACK THEREOF) IN THE PUBLIC MIND

Attitudes towards vehicle driving behaviour: Categorising and contextualising risk

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 137 (2016 ) GITSS2015

THE CROSS CULTURAL STUDY ON DRIVING BEHAVIOUR OF MALAYSIAN AGEING AUTOMOBILE DRIVERS

DRIVER S SITUATION AWARENESS DURING SUPERVISION OF AUTOMATED CONTROL Comparison between SART and SAGAT measurement techniques

COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

Effects of Visual and Cognitive Distraction on Lane Change Test Performance

Do Drowsy Driver Drugs Differ?

Assessing Moral Reasoning, Cognitive Distortions and Driving Style in the Context of Post-License Young Driver Coaching

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

Southeast Minnesota TZD Region Crash Data. May 1, 2014

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator

Effectiveness of Training Interventions on the Hazard Anticipation for Young Drivers Differing in Sensation Seeking Behavior

That s close enough - A threshold effect of time headway on the experience of risk, task difficulty, effort, and comfort

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study.

Subject: The Case for a Provincial 0.00% BAC Limit for All Drivers Under the Age of 21

Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES AND USING SIGNAL LIGHTS

SOCIAL-ECOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS IN THE EVALUATION AND IMPLEMENTATION OF ALCOHOL AND HIGHWAY SAFETY PROGRAMS

Cultural Differences and Traffic Safety.

Worldwide Trends in Drinking and Driving: Has the Progress Continued?

Analysing the inability to focus on the driving task in young males, inexperienced and offender drivers. Castro, C., Gugliotta, A. & Padilla, J.L.

Driver Alertness Detection Research Using Capacitive Sensor Array

Deciphering Psychological-Physiological Mappings While Driving and Performing a Secondary Memory Task

Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications

On-the-road driving tests and neurocognitive tests for measuring the effects of cannabis on driving

Impact of mailed feedback on speeding behaviours of convicted male drivers: A brief intervention.

DRIVER PSYCHOLOGICAL TYPES AND CAR FOLLOWING: IS THERE A CORRELATION? RESULTS OF A PILOT STUDY

Youth Involvement In Traffic Accidents In Japan-New Trends

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus

Pund, B. 1, Jänsch, M. 2, Otte, D. 2 1 TÜV Technische Überwachung Hessen GmbH, Life Service. Introduction

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Manufacturing 3 (2015 )

Estimation of the severity of traffic conflicts in naturalistic driving studies. Omar Bagdadi

Sight Distance AMRC 2012 MODULE 7 CONTENTS

Exploring the Factors that Impact Injury Severity using Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM)

Sakineh Varmazyar 1, Seyed Bagher Mortazavi 1, *, Ebrahim Hajizadeh 2, Shirazeh Arghami Background

Recent European Projects on Driver Impairment

Risk Factors Associated with Speeding Offences Among Young Western Australian Drivers

EVALUATING CHANGES IN THE DRIVING BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS A FEW YEARS AFTER LICENSURE USING IN-VEHICLE DATA RECORDERS

The role of attitudes in transportation studies

II: ALCOHOL - RELATED CRASHES

Protecting Workers with Smart E-Vests

TIME-TO-CONTACT AND COLLISION DETECTION ESTIMATIONS AS MEASURES OF DRIVING SAFETY IN OLD AND DEMENTIA DRIVERS

National Sleep Foundation. State of the States Report on Drowsy Driving: Summary of Findings. November 2008

The Effectiveness of Drinking-and-Driving Policies in the American States: A Cross-Sectional Time Series Analysis for

Driving and Dementia Practical Tips for the Family Physician

Reliability of the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire in a sample of novice drivers. Warren Harrison Eastern Professional Services Pty Ltd

Road safety. Tool 1 COMMUNITY TOOLS

THE MELLANBY EFFECT. Why Impaired Individuals Should Not Be Allowed to Be Behind the Wheel By Miriam Norman

Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios

driving in children? Max J. Cantor A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the Requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts

8/26/2013 DMV UCSD CHP. Presenters. Law Enforcement s Identification and Referral of Medically Impaired Older Drivers. NHTSA Priority.

DRINKING AND DRIVING. Alcohol consumption, even in relatively small quantities, increases the risk of road crashes.

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

The Misjudgment of Risk due to Inattention

ROAD SAFETY MONITOR. ALCOHOL-IMPAIRED DRIVING IN THE UNITED STATES Results from the 2018 TIRF USA Road Safety Monitor

J. O. Thomas, Chief Inspector, Q.P.M.* and G.W. Trinca, Dr., O.B.E., MBBS, F.R.A.C.S., National Chairman**

Transportation Research Part F

A Study Predicting Self-Reported Crashes Among Fleet Drivers. Davey, J., Freeman, J., & Wishart, D.

Policy Research CENTER

SOCIAL NORMS, ATTITUDES AND INTENTIONS CONCERNING DRINKING AND DRIVING. A CROSS-NATIONAL COMPARISON

The Severity of Pedestrian Injuries in Alcohol-Related Collisions

Reducing Driving Violations by Receiving Feedback from Other Drivers

briefing notes - road safety issues Auckland Motorways

Transcription:

University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 10th, 12:00 AM Overestimation of Skills Affects Drivers Adaptation to Task Demands Saskia de Craen SWOV, Institute for Road Safety Research, Netherlands Divera A.M. Twisk SWOV, Institute for Road Safety Research, Netherlands Marjan P. Hagenzieker SWOV, Institute for Road Safety Research, Netherlands Henk Elffers NSCR, Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement Leiden, the Netherlands Karel A. Brookhuis Delft University of Technology, Netherlands Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment de Craen, Saskia; Twisk, Divera A.M.; Hagenzieker, Marjan P.; Elffers, Henk; and Brookhuis, Karel A.. Overestimation of Skills Affects Drivers Adaptation to Task Demands. In: Proceedings of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, July 9-12, 2007, Stevenson, Washington. Iowa City, IA: Public Policy Center, University of Iowa, 2007: 39-45. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1212 This Event is brought to you for free and open access by the Public Policy Center at Iowa Research Online. It has been accepted for inclusion in Driving Assessment Conference by an authorized administrator of Iowa Research Online. For more information, please contact lib-ir@uiowa.edu.

OVERESTIMATION OF SKILLS AFFECTS DRIVERS ADAPTATION TO TASK DEMANDS Saskia de Craen, 1 Divera A.M. Twisk, 1 Marjan P. Hagenzieker, 1 Henk Elffers, 2 and Karel A. Brookhuis 3 1 SWOV, Institute for Road Safety Research Leidschendam, the Netherlands E-mail: Saskia.de.Craen@SWOV.nl 2 NSCR, Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement Leiden, the Netherlands 3 Delft University of Technology Delft, the Netherlands Summary: Inadequate self-assessment, and specifically, overestimation of skill, results in insufficient adaptation to task demands, which can manifest itself on different levels of the driving task. A total of 130 drivers (83 novice and 47 experienced drivers) participated in an on-road driving assessment. Their performance in this assessment (i.e., fail or pass) was compared to the participants reported confidence in their driving skills (i.e., high or low confidence), resulting in three calibration groups: a) well-calibrated drivers (reported confidence matched performance on assessment), b) overconfident drivers (high confidence but failed assessment) and c) insecure drivers (low confidence but passed assessment). Furthermore, participants completed a questionnaire which focused on choices made on the strategic and manoeuvring level of the driving task. No significant difference was found between the calibration groups for the strategic level. Overconfident drivers reported significantly more violating behaviour than the well-calibrated and the insecure drivers. At the manoeuvring level, overconfident drivers showed significantly less instances of adaptation to traffic complexity. In conclusion, the current study suggests that overconfidence is related to inadequate adaptation to task demands. INTRODUCTION For safe driving, a driver s capabilities have to match (or exceed) the task demands of a traffic situation (Fuller, 2005). A driver can influence the tasks demands by, for example, adjusting speed or headway. It is assumed that the decision to increase or decrease task demands is influenced by the driver s assessment of the complexity of the traffic situation on the one hand, and of his driver ability on the other hand. Balancing task demands and capabilities has been named calibration. It is postulated that novice drivers are particularly poor at calibration and that improvement in calibration explains the steady decrease in crash rates in the first months after licensing (Brown & Groeger, 1988; Gregersen, 1995; Kuiken & Twisk, 2001). The present study mainly deals with the influence of self-assessment of skills on the adaptation to task demands. Inadequate self-assessment of driving skills can go either way, underestimation or overestimation of skills. Most research has been focused on overestimation of driving skills, 39

as that is considered the most dangerous form of inadequate self-assessment (Gregersen, 1995; Mathews & Moran, 1986). The current study, however, also addresses underestimation of skill. The hypothesis of this study is that inadequate self-assessment of skills results in an insufficient adaptation to the task demands, which can manifest itself on different levels of the driving task (e.g., Fuller & Santos, 2002; Rasmussen, 1986). In the current study, we distinguish adaptation on the two highest levels of Michon s (1984) driving task hierarchy. First, on the strategic level, the driver might avoid a difficult junction or choose (not) to drive in the dark. Second, on the manoeuvring level, a driver might not sufficiently reduce speed or increase headway when encountering more complex situations. The influence of inadequate self-assessment on reported violating behaviour is also studied, which can be viewed as both strategic, as it involves a strategic choice (not) to violate traffic regulations, and manoeuvring, as the specific situation has a large influence on driver behaviour (Reason, 1990). The lowest level of the driving task hierarchy (i.e., the control or vehicle handling level) was not considered here, as adaptation to task demands encompasses a conscious assessment to be made. METHOD Participants The total sample consisted of novice (n = 83, 52% male) and experienced drivers (n = 47, 57% male). The novice group had 6 months driving experience (Mean age = 20; SD = 1.7) and the experienced group at least 10 years (Mean age = 41; SD = 5.1). Instruments Questionnaire. To provide an indication of reported driving behaviour at the strategic and manoeuvring level of the driving task and driver confidence, an online questionnaire was administered. In addition to some background information (e.g., age, occupation), the questionnaire included a five-point scale concerning driver confidence (very confident to not at all) and questions regarding the adaptation to task demands. Relating to the strategic level, drivers were asked: a) whether, in the last 4 months, they had cancelled an intended driving trip due to adverse conditions (e.g., darkness or weather conditions); and b) whether, in the previous 4 months, they had avoided a difficult intersection. To measure adaptation to task demands on the manoeuvring level, the Adaptation Test was applied (De Craen, Twisk, Hagenzieker, Elffers, & Brookhuis, 2007). This test consists of several traffic situations presented in two almost identical photographs. The photographs differ in one single detail relevant to the driving task, increasing the complexity of the situation (e.g., the presence or absence of pedestrians on the pavement, see Figure 1). The photographs are presented in random order, and the complex and simple photographs of one situation never directly succeed one another. Respondents assess at what speed they would drive in the depicted situation. A response is considered correct when the reported speed is lower in the complex situation than in the corresponding simple situation. The adaptation score is the sum of all correct responses (1 credit point per correct response; no negative points are assigned for incorrect 40

responses). An evaluation of the Adaptation Test indicated that it is a valid instrument to measure adaptation of reported driving speed to a specific situation (De Craen et al., 2007). Figure 1. Example of a simple (left) and complex (right) traffic situation The questionnaire also contained an abbreviated Dutch version (Verschuur, 2003) of the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire (DBQ, see Parker, Reason, Manstead, & Stradling, 1995), which relates to the strategic level, as well as the manoeuvring level. The 8 items of the DBQ indicating violating behaviour were used to measure the effect of inadequate self-assessment on violating behaviour. On-road driving assessment. A driving assessment was conducted, consisting of half an hour driving on different types of road. Examiners rated drivers on their ability to drive safely on a scale from 0 to 10, 5.5 being the pass-fail criterion in a real driving test. As in a regular driving test, this general mark for safe driving is a combination of many different skills on different levels of the driving task (e.g., to manoeuvre safely through traffic, a driver must possess a certain level of vehicle control). Data analysis Self-assessment was computed by comparing the examiner's rating for safe driving with the participants confidence-rating. Chi-square analysis was used to test for significant differences in frequencies. Analysis of variance was used to test for significant effects (α =.05) on the DBQ items and the adaptation score. Besides significance of the results, the effect size (Partial èta squared, η 2 ) is also reported, with η 2.01 as a small, η 2.06 as a medium, and η 2.14 as a large effect size (Cohen, 1988). RESULTS The variable driver confidence was used to distinguish two driver groups: a) high-confidence drivers (drivers who said they were (very) confident); and b) low-confidence drivers (drivers whose responses were neutral or not (at all) confident). Next, the examiner s safe driving score was used to divide the drivers into two groups: a pass group, consisting of those who would have passed the driving test (scoring 5.5 or higher) and a fail group consisting of those who would 41

have failed (scoring less than 5.5). Table 1 shows a cross tabulation of driver confidence and the examiner s safe driving score. This results in three groups: a) 88 (82 + 6) well-calibrated drivers (share the same opinion about their driving skills), b) 20 insecure drivers (are not confident about their driving skills, but passed the driving assessment) and c) 22 overconfident drivers (are confident about their driving skills, but failed the driving assessment). The reported results in this paper are fairly robust for the pass-fail criterion for safe driving (that is, the results are similar when a pass-fail criterion of, for example, 6 is used). Table 1. Calibration groups Safe driving Total Confidence Passed Failed Confident 82 a 22 c 104 Insecure 20 b 6 a 26 Total 102 28 130 Table 2 shows some characteristics of the three calibration groups. Chi-square analysis indicated that the group of experienced drivers consisted of significantly more well-calibrated drivers (85%) than the group of novice drivers (58%); (χ 2 (1, N = 130) = 10.21; p <.01). The difference between males and females in the three calibration groups was also significant (X 2 (2, N = 130) = 8.83; p <.05); 77% of the males were well-calibrated drivers; of the female drivers only 57% were well calibrated. Female drivers were significantly overrepresented in the insecure group (25% of the female drivers compared to 7% of the male drivers; χ 2 (1, N = 130) = 7.91; p <.01). There was no gender effect in the overconfident group. Table 2. Characteristics of calibration groups n Well calibrated Insecure Overconfident Group Experienced 47 85% 11% 4% Novice 83 58% 18% 24% Gender Male 70 77% 7% 16% Female 60 57% 25% 18% Adaptation Strategic level. The results show that overall, 25% of the drivers reported not driving because of adverse conditions ; no difference was found between the well-calibrated group and the other two groups. A larger proportion of drivers (44%) reported having avoided a difficult intersection. However, further analyses showed that drivers avoided such intersections because of traffic congestion not because of traffic complexity. Consequently, there was no difference between calibration groups. There was also no individual effect of driver confidence or performance on the driving assessment ( safe driving ), on these variables. 42

Manoeuvring level. ANOVA shows that there was a moderate (η 2 =.09) and significant difference between calibration groups (F 2,124 = 6.15; p <.01) in the responses on the Adaptation Test. Post Hoc Bonferroni tests indicated that overconfident drivers adapted their speed to the complex situation significantly less (26% correct responses; p <.01) than well-calibrated drivers (42% correct responses) and insecure drivers (36% correct responses). No significant interaction effect was found between calibration groups and gender. Safe drivers, as determined in the driving assessment, reported reduced speed more often in the complex situations (41% correct responses) compared to unsafe drivers (31% correct responses; F 1,126 = 4.56; p <.05). No effect of driver confidence was found on the Adaptation Test. Violations. The 8 items of the DBQ indicating violating behaviour (Table 3) were used to see whether well-calibrated drivers reported less dangerous decisions. MANOVA indicated that there was a large (η 2 =.13) and significant difference between calibration groups (F 16,236 = 2.11; p <.01) in their reported violating behaviour. Tests of between-subject effects indicate that, except for being angered by another driver (item 4), and having an aversion to a class of road users (item 7), all items differentiated significantly between calibration groups. The overconfident group generally reported more violating behaviour than the well-calibrated and insecure group. Table 3 shows the mean score for each group on the 8 DBQ items indicating how often they would indulge in that behaviour (0 = Never; 5 = Nearly all the time). Significant deviations (α =.05) from the well-calibrated group, based on Bonferroni Post Hoc tests, are indicated by an asterisk. Table 3 Mean score on DBQ items 1. Become impatient with a slow driver in the outer lane and overtake on the inside 2. Drive especially close to the car in front as a signal to its driver to go faster or get out of the way 3. Cross a junction knowing that the traffic lights have already turned against you 4. Angered by another driver s behaviour, you give chase with the intention of giving him/her a piece of your mind Well calibrated Mean score Insecure Overconfident 2.0 2.1 2.8* 1.9 2.0 2.6* 1.7 1.8 2.3* 1.2 1.1 1.1 5. Disregard the speed limits late at night or early in the morning 2.8 3.6 3.5 6. Drive even though you realise that you may be over the Legal blood-alcohol limit 7. Have an aversion to a particular class of road user, and indicate your hostility by whatever means you can 1.2 1.1 1.6* 1.5 1.6 2.0 8. Get involved in unofficial races with other drivers 1.4 1.2 1.9 Note: * indicates a significant difference (α =.05) with well-calibrated drivers 43

No main effect of gender was found on the DBQ-items. However, the interaction of gender and calibration group was significant (F 16,236 = 2.16; p <.01). Tests of between subject effects showed that the interaction effect was exclusively caused by driving over the Legal bloodalcohol limit (item 6). Only in the overconfident group, male drivers reported significantly more drink and drive behaviour than females. In the well-calibrated and insecure group, no effect of gender was found. The items of the DBQ were additionally analysed for the effects of driver confidence and performance on the driving assessment ( safe driving ). Confident drivers reported more violations than less confident drivers (F 8,119 = 2.47; p <.05), but no effect of safe driving was found. DISCUSSION As mentioned in the introduction, research has suggested that, compared to experienced drivers, novice drivers are poor at calibration and that inadequate self-assessment, and specifically, overestimation of skills, results in inadequate adaptation of task demands. To measure selfassessment of skills, drivers self-reported confidence was compared with actual driving performance. The cross tabulations resulted in three groups: a) well-calibrated drivers, b) overconfident drivers and c) insecure drivers. Only half of the novice drivers were well calibrated as opposed to 85% of the experienced drivers. As expected, a relatively large proportion of novices (24%) belong to the overconfident group. This proportion is particularly large, when taking into account the modest amount of driving experience in the novice driver group. The main hypothesis of this paper, that inadequate self-assessment of skills may lead to dangerous behaviour (irrespective of experience level), was supported by the finding that overconfident drivers generally reported more violating behaviour. Our results also show that overconfident drivers reported less instances of adaptation of driving speed to the complexity of traffic situations as measured with the Adaptation Test. Although insecure drivers performed somewhat worse on the Adaptation Test, the percentage of correct responses was not significantly different from the well-calibrated drivers. There was no evidence found to suggest that inadequate self-assessment of skills affects adaptation on the strategic level of the driving task. This could have been a result of the choice of questions to measure adaptation on this level. There were only two questions, of which one seemed to have measured something other than it was intended to measure (i.e., drivers considered congestion as a reason to avoid a difficult intersection, instead of complexity of the traffic situation). The construction of the calibration groups was based on a comparison of self-reported confidence ( driver confidence ) and the examiners safe driving scores. It may be argued that reported differences between calibration groups are caused solely by, for example, safe driving, and that driver confidence did not contribute to the reported differences. It is difficult to eliminate this alternative explanation, because the number of participants is too few to have a reasonable power for tests of interaction between confidence and examiners opinion. As a result, no significant interaction effects were found. However, we did not find any evidence that either driver confidence or safe driving could explain all the results. For example, the 44

violation items of the DBQ indicated a significant main effect for driver confidence but there was no significant main effect for safe driving. In contrast, for the Adaptation Test, a significant effect for safe driving was found but not for driver confidence. These results suggest that the combination of safe driving and driver confidence (i.e., self-assessment of skills) explains more variation than both factors separately. Prior research has indicated calibration as a relevant factor for safe driving, and has linked calibration to the high crash rate of young, novice drivers. The current study provides evidence for the behavioural consequences of overconfidence. Whether, how, and when calibration improves in the first months of licensing is subject of further study. REFERENCES Brown, I.D. & Groeger, J.A. (1988). Risk perception and decision taking during the transition between novice and experienced driver status. Ergonomics, 31(4), 585-597. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. De Craen, S., Twisk, D.A., Hagenzieker, M.P., Elffers, H.E., & Brookhuis, K.A. (2007). The development of a method to measure speed adaptation to traffic complexity: Identifying novice, unsafe and overconfident drivers. Manuscript submitted for publication. Fuller, R. & Santos, J.A. (2002). Human Factors for Highway Engineers. Oxford: Elsevier Science Ltd. Fuller, R. (2005). Towards a general theory of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 27(3), 461-472. Gregersen, N.P. (1995). Prevention of road accidents among young novice car drivers. Doctoral dissertation, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden. Kuiken, M.J. & Twisk, D.A.M. (2001). Safe driving and the training of calibration (Report no. R-2001-29). Leidschendam, the Netherlands: SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research. Mathews, M.L. & Moran, A.R. (1986). Age differences in male drivers' perception of accident risk: The role of perceived driving ability. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 18, 299-313. Michon, J.A. (1984). A critical view of driver behavior models: What do we know, what should we do? Human behavior and traffic safety: Proceedings of a general motor symposium on human behavior and traffic safety, Michigan, 516-520. Parker, D., Reason, J.T., Manstead, A.S.R., & Stradling, S.G. (1995). Driving errors, driving violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics, 38(5), 1036-1048. Rasmussen, J. (1986). Information processing and human-machine interaction; an approach to cognitive engineering. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland. Reason, J. (1990). Human Error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Verschuur, W.L.G. (2003). Onderzoek uitgevoerd met de 'Driver Behaviour Questionnaire'; eindrapportage (Werkgroep Veiligheid/R-03/68; in Dutch). Leiden, The Netherlands: Rijksuniversiteit Leiden. 45