Supplementary Material. Participants completed the 90-item Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ;

Similar documents
Prefrontal mediation of age differences in cognitive reappraisal

Levels of γ-aminobutyric acid in the ventral lateral prefrontal cortex as a predictor of underdetermined selection ability

Journal of Anxiety Disorders

Author's personal copy

ANSWERS TO EXERCISES AND REVIEW QUESTIONS

Comorbid Anxiety and Depression: Do they Cluster as Distinct Groups in Youth?

Affective Control Scale

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Task timeline for Solo and Info trials.

Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Relations of the factors of the tripartite model of anxiety and depression to types of social anxiety

Attentional modulation of the amygdala varies with personality

Supplementary Results: Age Differences in Participants Matched on Performance

Supplementary Materials for

Master's Thesis. Present to

Attentional Deployment Is Not Necessary for Successful Emotion Regulation via Cognitive Reappraisal or Expressive Suppression

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

BRIEF REPORT. Links between neuroticism, emotional distress, and disengaging attention: Evidence from a single-target RSVP task

Vulnerabilities to depression, anxiety and interpersonal problems: the role of parental styles, schemas and coping styles

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Supplementary Materials Are taboo words simply more arousing?

The interaction of reinforcement sensitivity and life events in the prediction of anhedonic depression and mixed anxiety-depression symptoms

Measuring Positive Emotion With the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire: Psychometric Properties of the Anhedonic Depression Scale

Subjective Responses to Emotional Stimuli During Labeling, Reappraisal, and Distraction

See no evil: Directing visual attention within unpleasant images modulates the electrocortical response

Emotion Regulation During Relationship Conflict. Shiri Ben-Naim Gilad Hirschberger Tsachi Ein-Dor Mario Mikulincer

The interaction of reinforcement sensitivity and life events in the prediction of anhedonic depression and mixed anxiety-depression symptoms

Love, Emotions, and the Highly Sensitive Brain

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

HTS Report EPS. Emotional Processing Scale. Technical Report. Anna Patient ID Date 29/09/2016. Hogrefe Ltd, Oxford

Cognitive and neural basis of emotion regulation in bipolar disorder

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Overlap between default mode network (DMN) and movie/recall maps.

Reliability and Validity checks S-005

Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

Gaze fixations predict brain activation during the voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect

Supplementary Information

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011

The primate amygdala combines information about space and value

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Motivational biases in memory for emotions Heather C. Lench a ; Linda J. Levine b a

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Understanding the relationship between subjective wellbeing and gambling behavior

Pain Catastrophizing and Social Support in Married Individuals with Chronic Pain: The Moderating Role of Pain Duration

The effects of mindfulness versus thought suppression instruction on the appraisal of emotional and neutral pictures

Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals

ORIGINAL RESEARCH PSYCHOLOGY

David M. Fresco, Ph.D.

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Perceived Stress, Anhedonia and Illusion of Control: Evidence for Two Mediational Models

The Validation of the Career Decision- Making Difficulties Scale in a Chinese Culture

Neural Processing of Emotional Musical and Nonmusical Stimuli in Depression

Advanced Data Modelling & Inference

One-Way ANOVAs t-test two statistically significant Type I error alpha null hypothesis dependant variable Independent variable three levels;

SOCIAL ANXIETY AND ATTENTIONAL BREADTH 1. publication. Copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proofs will be

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Relationships of Distinct Affective Dimensions to Performance on an Emotional Stroop Task

SENTENCE COMPLETION TEST FOR DEPRESSION. LONG FORM Version 3.1 SCD-48

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

Spousal Congruence on Disability, Pain, and Spouse Responses to Pain

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS)

The Function and Organization of Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Test of Competing Hypotheses

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged

Depressed individuals who have anhedonia, a reduced ability

Memory suppression can help people unlearn behavioral responses but only for nonemotional memories

Shaping the Relative Importance of Needs from Recent to Early Autobiographical Experiences

Supporting Information

Autism Spectrum Disorders. A general overview

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Nature of emotion: Six perennial questions

This material should not be used for any other purpose without the permission of the author. Contact details:

This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Sex differences in interpretation bias in adolescents, Gluck, R., Lynn, D. A.

Biological Risk Factors

Authors: Paul A. Frewen, PhD 1,2,*, David J. A. Dozois, PhD 1,2, Richard W. J. Neufeld 1,2,3, &Ruth A. Lanius, MD, PhD 2,3 Departments of Psychology

LEARNERS EMOTIONS IN SIMULATIONBASED MEDICAL EDUCATION

Personality and Individual Differences

The revised short-form of the Eating Beliefs Questionnaire: Measuring positive, negative, and permissive beliefs about binge eating

Differences in Social 1. Running head: DIFFERENCES IN SOCIAL SUPPORT AS A RISK FACTOR

Nature of emotion: Six perennial questions

Lateral and medial prefrontal contributions to emotion generation by semantic elaboration during episodic encoding

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

the body and the front interior of a sedan. It features a projected LCD instrument cluster controlled

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

EXAMINING DIFFERENTIATION OF SELF AS A MEDIATOR IN THE BIOBEHAVIORAL FAMILY MODEL

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

Personality Affects Aspects of Health-Related Quality of Life in Parkinson s Disease via Psychological Coping Strategies

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

Gender Differences Associated With Memory Recall. By Lee Morgan Gunn. Oxford May 2014

Career Decision-Making Difficulties Perceived by College Students in Taiwan *

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16)

Bruno D. Zumbo, Ph.D. University of Northern British Columbia

A Developmental Perspective on the Role of Genes on Substance Use Disorder. Elisa M. Trucco, Ph.D., Florida International University

DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW PATIENT REPORTED OUTCOME MEASURE FOR MENTAL HEALTH SERVICES

Threat or Challenge? Self-Focused Attention and Evaluation on Perceived Performance in Social Anxiety. Megan A. Viar. Distinguished Majors Thesis

Chapter 16: Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

Transcription:

Prefrontal mediation 1 Supplementary Material 1. Questionnaires Participants completed the 90-item Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ; Watson and Clark, 1991, Watson, Clark, Weber, Smith Assenheimer, Strauss and McCormick, 1995) and the 20-item Attentional Control Scale (ACS; Derryberry & Reed, 2002). The MASQ-90 assesses anxious, depressed, and mixed symptoms, general distress, anxious arousal, and anhedonic depression (Watson et al., 1991). On a scale from one to five, where one equals not at all, and five equals extremely, participants rate how much they have experienced each item in the week prior to testing. Items include felt confused (general distress), hands were shaky (anxious arousal), and felt like nothing was enjoyable (anhedonic depression). Across samples of undergraduate students, adults, and clinical samples, all MASQ sub-scales have shown good reliability (Cronbach s alphas ranging from.78 to.93) (Watson et al., 1995). The ACS (Derryberry & Reed, 2002) assesses voluntary control over attention and has subscales cataloguing attentional shifting and attentional focusing. Participants rate each item on a scale from one to four, where one equals almost never and four equals almost always. Items include It s very hard for me to concentrate on a difficult task when noises are around, and It takes me a while to get really involved in a new task. The ACS has shown good reliability (Cronbach s alpha =.88). 2. Additional Analyses 2.1 Effects of picture valence on valence ratings Mean ratings of valence for view unpleasant and view neutral trials (averaged across gaze direction conditions) were submitted to a multivariate GLM to assess the main and interactive

Prefrontal mediation 2 effects of picture valence (unpleasant, neutral; within-subjects) and age group (younger, older; between-subjects). The GLM revealed a significant main effect of picture valence, F(2,28) = 296.30, p <.001, ηp 2 =.91. The highest rating of 3 indicates negative valence. Thus, unpleasant pictures, M = 2.73, SD =.27, were rated as more unpleasant than neutral pictures, M = 1.72, SD =.29. There was no significant effect of age group, F(1,29) = 1.87, p =.187, ηp 2 =.06, and no significant interaction between valence and age group, F(1,29) = 2.08, p =.160, ηp 2 =.07. 2.2 Effects of reappraisal goal, age group, and gaze direction on valence ratings The main effect of reappraisal goal on valence ratings for the unpleasant picture trials was significant, F(2,28) = 8.78, p<.001, ηp 2 =.39. Judgments of valence were more negative during the increase, M = 2.87, SD =.20, compared to view, M = 2.73, SD =.27, p <.001, condition. The difference between the view and decrease, M = 2.75, SD =.24, conditions was not significant, p =.355. Gaze direction did not interact with reappraisal goal, F(2,28) =.56, p =.58, ηp 2 =.04. 2.3 Are the age differences in neural activation above driven by age differences in spatial extent? Samanez-Larkin and D Esposito (2008) noted that older adults may exhibit smaller spatial extents of activation within an ROI compared to younger adults. As a result, mean activation calculated across the entire ROI may be artificially lower for older than younger adults due to inclusion of very low signal change in many voxels for older adults. Thus, we submitted estimates of peak signal change to the same analyses reported above. The pattern of findings remained unchanged, which suggests that age differences in spatial extent of activation within the two ROI do not explain the age differences in mean percent signal change 2.4 What did participants actually do when asked to reappraise? To determine whether participants used cognitive reappraisal as instructed, we examined

Prefrontal mediation 3 the strategies participants reported using when instructed to increase or decrease their response to the unpleasant pictures. For increase reappraisals, seven participants (22.58%; 4 younger, 3 older) indicated imagining themselves or loved ones in the depicted situations, 18 participants (58.06%; 8 younger, 9 older) reported imagining the depicted situation to be very negative, or getting worse in the near future, and five participants (16.13%; 2 younger, 3 older) reported nonspecific reappraisals such as tried to increase how I felt. Three participants (8.33%; all younger adults) reported focusing on specific parts of the scene to increase their emotions. For decrease reappraisals, 17 participants (54.84%; 6 younger, 11 older) reported imagining the situation as being not as severe as it appeared, or improving in the near future. Three participants (9.68%, all younger adults) indicated imagining suffering coming to an end. Five participants (16.12%; 4 younger, 1 older) reported imagining the situation being fake or pretend, two participants (6.25%; both older adults) reported nonspecific reappraisals, and one participant (3.22%; younger adult) described distancing myself from the scene internally. Three participants (6.45%; 1 younger, 2 older) reported focusing on specific parts of the scene to decrease their emotions. These data collectively suggest that participants largely used cognitive reappraisal to increase and decrease their unpleasant emotion. A few participants relied on attention-based regulation strategies, thus underscoring the importance of analyzing the dot detection data to determine if there were differences in attentional deployment across reappraisal conditions and/or age groups. 2.5 Do age differences in a third variable account for age differences in CR? There were age differences in dot detection performance (increase > view for older

Prefrontal mediation 4 adults). In addition, as shown in Supplementary Table 1, there were age differences in measures of mood/anxiety symptoms and attentional control (older < younger for MASQ; older > younger for ACS). Thus, to evaluate whether the age differences we observed in reappraisal-related differences in intensity ratings and neural activation were explained by these variables, we recalculated Cohen s d effect sizes for the age difference with the influence of these covariates partialled out and examined the extent to which the age difference effect sizes changed. The effect size for the age difference in the increase view contrast score for intensity ratings was reduced from Cohen s d =.53 to d =.276, a difference of 48%. Similarly, the effect size for the age difference in the view decrease contrast score for intensity ratings was reduced from Cohen s d =.343 to d =.174, a difference of 49%. In left ventrolateral PFC, the effect size for the age difference in reappraisal-related activation was reduced from Cohen s d =.515 to d =.282, a difference of 45%. In dorsal medial PFC, the effect size remained essentially unchanged, going from Cohen s d =.307 to d =.309, a difference of less than 1%. This pattern collectively indicates that the size of the age difference was reduced when controlling for dot detection performance, and MASQ and ACS scores. However, these variables do not fully explain the age differences we observed. 2.6 Is the age difference in prefrontal activation driven by increase or decrease reappraisals? Independent samples t-tests (one-tailed) to evaluate the age difference for (increase view) and (decrease view) separately indicated that, in left VLPFC, the age difference was significant for both increase reappraisals, t(29) = 3.06, p =.005, and decrease reappraisals, t(29) = 2.19, p =.04. In DMPFC, the age difference was significant for increase reappraisals, t(29) = 2.05, p =.049, but not for decrease reappraisals, t(29) = 1.02, p =.32. See Supplementary Table 2 for effect sizes.

Prefrontal mediation 5 2.7 Does directing gaze impact subjective emotion experience or prefrontal activation? Consistent with Urry (2010), participants rated trials as more intense when gaze was directed to an arousing area, M = 2.67, SD =.59, compared to when gaze was directed to a nonarousing area, M = 2.59, SD =.57, of the picture, F (1,29)= 5.69, p =.02, P =.16. In left ventrolateral PFC, participants showed marginally higher mean estimates of % BOLD signal change when gaze was directed to an arousing, M =.60, SD =.13, compared to when gaze was directed to a non-arousing, M =.14, SD =.13, area of the picture, F (1,29) = 3.27, p =.08, P =.1. Similarly, in dorsal medial PFC, participants showed marginally higher estimates of % BOLD signal change when gaze was directed to an arousing area of the picture, M =.11, SD =.14, compared to when gaze was directed to a non-arousing area of the picture, M =.09, SD =.13, F (1,29) = 3.41, p =.075, P =.11. These results indicate that gaze direction by itself manipulates emotion experience and, to some degree, activation in these two regions of PFC, although lack of statistical power meant only marginally significant neural effects. These results also provide validation of our a priori arousing and non-arousing AOI, which were originally validated by Urry (2010) in a separate sample of younger adults. Supplementary Material References Derryberry, D. & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225 236. Samanez-Larkin, G., & D Esposito, M. (2008). Group comparisons: Imaging the aging brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3 (3), 290-297. Urry, H. L. (2010). Seeing, thinking, and feeling: Emotion-regulating effects of gaze-directed cognitive reappraisal. Emotion, 10, 125-135.

Prefrontal mediation 6 Watson, D., & Clark., L.A. The mood and anxiety symptom questionnaire. (1991). Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, Department of Psychology, Iowa City. Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J., Strauss, M. & McCormick, R. (1995). Testing a Tripartite Model: I. Evaluating the Convergent and Discriminant Validity of Anxiety and Depression Symptom scales. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 3-14.

Prefrontal mediation 7 Supplementary Table 1 Characteristics of the sample Measure Younger Older Significant Age Difference? Mean (SD) age in years 19.25 (1.43) 59.87 (3.14) t(18.548) = -43.6, p <.001 N 16 15 Men 8 7 Women 8 8 (1) =.034, p =.853 (2x2: sex by age group) Highest Level of Education High school diploma 2 (12.5%) 1 (6.6%) Some college 14 (87.5%) 7 (46.6%) College diploma or higher 0 (0%) 7(46.6%) (2) = 13.58, p =.001 (3x2: education by age group) Marital Status Single (never married) 16 (100%) 1 (6.6%) Married 0 7 (40%) Divorced 0 7 (46%) 2 (2) = 27.23, p <.001 (3x2: marital status by age group) Self Report Measures MASQ total 32.28 (5.77) 30.03 (3.54) t(26) = 1.76, p =.09 Anxious 19.82 (4.58) 14.2 (3.08) t(29) = 3.98, p <.001 Depressed 21.69 (8.01) 18.38 (6.07) t(29) = 1.29, p =.207 Mixed Symptoms 31.56 (9.70) 22.25 (3.42) t(19.13) = 3.59, p =.002 Anxious Arousal 22.78 (4.11) 18.733 (1.87) t(28) = 3.472, p =.002 Anhedonic Depression 53.46 (0.5) 53.18 (7.12) t(27) =.08, p =.937 General Distress 24.35 (6.90) 18.479 (3.57) t(23.09) =2.87, p =.008 ACS total 25.34 (4.93) 30.48 (3.62) t(29) = -3.29, p =.003 Attentional Focusing 24.44 (5.49) 30.10 (3.61) t(29) = -3.37, p =.002 Attentional Shifting 26.25 (4.91) 30.87 (3.62) t(29) =-2.66, p =.013 Note. MASQ = Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ); ACS = Attentional Control Scale. Some participants did not provide MASQ or ACS data, and were therefore excluded from those analyses. For some measures, unequal variances between age groups were controlled for by adjusting degrees of freedom.

Prefrontal mediation 8 Supplementary Table 2 Effect sizes for ratings of emotional intensity and neural activation for increase vs. view and decrease vs. view (Cohen s dz), and for the between-subjects age difference in those two paired effects (Cohen s d) across and within gaze directions across gaze directions arousing non-arousing increase - view decrease - view increase - view decrease - view increase - view decrease - view n ratings of emotional intensity younger adults 0.83 0.21 0.60 0.08 0.80 0.26 16 older adults 1.64 0.47 1.10 0.33 1.37 0.44 15 age difference for the contrast 0.53 0.34 0.25 0.20 0.69 0.35 dorsal medial PFC activation younger adults 1.13 0.59 1.03 0.41 0.82 0.50 16 older adults 0.30 0.35 0.15 0.33 0.35 0.07 15 age difference for the contrast 0.37 0.18 0.34 0.07 0.21 0.31 left lateral PFC activation younger adults 1.29 1.04 1.10 0.54 1.16 0.78 16 older adults 0.41 0.45 0.28 0.45 0.27 0.16 15 age difference for the contrast 0.55 0.39 0.39 0.16 0.49 0.38 Notes. Arousing = gaze directed to an emotionally relevant area of the picture, non-arousing = gaze directed away from the emotionally relevant areas of the picture.

Prefrontal mediation 9 Supplemental Figure 1. Dot detection results for unpleasant picture trials. Error bars represent +1 SEM.