Gender composition in teams

Similar documents
DIFFERENCES IN THE ECONOMIC DECISIONS OF MEN AND WOMEN: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE*

Fairness behavior in an economic crisis environment: a Romanian experimental perspective

Veronika Grimm, Friederike Mengel. Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games RM/10/017

Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization

Poverty in dictator games: Awakening solidarity

ULTIMATUM GAME. An Empirical Evidence. Presented By: SHAHID RAZZAQUE

Koji Kotani International University of Japan. Abstract

Of mice and men: Within gender variation in strategic behavior

Women and Men are Different but Equal: Observations of Learning Behavior in Auctions

Gender Differences in Giving in the Dictator Game: The Role of Reluctant Altruism

CAN TEAMS HELP TO CLOSE THE GENDER COMPETITION GAP?

Sequential Decision and Strategy Vector Methods in Ultimatum Bargaining: Evidence on the Strength of Other- Regarding Behavior

A cash effect in ultimatum game experiments

DEPARTMENT OF ECONOMICS AND FINANCE COLLEGE OF BUSINESS AND ECONOMICS UNIVERSITY OF CANTERBURY CHRISTCHURCH, NEW ZEALAND

Altruism, Anticipation, and Gender

How to identify trust and reciprocity

UNIVERSITY OF SIENA DEPARTMENT OF ECONOMICS. Alessandro Innocenti Maria Grazia Pazienza. Altruism and Gender in the Trust Game 5/2006 WORKING PAPER

Deserving Altruism: Type Preferences in the Laboratory

People recognise when they are really anonymous in an economic game

Gender and preferences for redistribution in a dynamic production environment

Institutions and Cooperative Behavior

Beauty, Gender and Stereotypes: Evidence from Laboratory Experiments

Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences

Strong Reciprocity and Human Sociality

Lecture 3. QIAO Zhilin ( 乔志林 ) School of Economics & Finance Xi an Jiaotong University

Gender and Culture: International Experimental Evidence from Trust Games

Social Preferences of Young Adults in Japan: The Roles of Age and Gender

Searching for a better deal On the influence of team decision making, time pressure and gender *

ExpEc I. Preliminaries

GENDER SELECTION DISCRIMINATION: EVIDENCE FROM A TRUST GAME* Robert Slonim Case Western Reserve University. September 7 th, 2004

Positive reciprocity and intentions in trust games

Università degli Studi di Siena DIPARTIMENTO DI ECONOMIA POLITICA

Emanuela Carbonara. 31 January University of Bologna - Department of Economics

Documentos de Trabajo. An experiment on corruption and gender. María Fernanda Rivas

Reciprocity, Cooperation, and Reputation

Today s lecture. A thought experiment. Topic 3: Social preferences and fairness. Overview readings: Fehr and Fischbacher (2002) Sobel (2005)

An investment game with third-party intervention

Let the Dummy Talk! - Unilateral Communication and Discrimination in Three-Person Dictator Experiments -

Kin Selection Theory. Overview. Methods of Kin Recognition. The Trust Game. Hypotheses. The Trust Game

Territoriality and Gender in the Laboratory

Inequity and Risk Aversion. in Sequential Public Good Games

Minimal Social Cues in the Dictator Game

Giving, taking, and gender in dictator games

Topic 3: Social preferences and fairness


HYPOTHETICAL AND REAL INCENTIVES IN THE ULTIMATUM GAME AND ANDREONI S PUBLIC GOODS GAME: AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY

The Game Prisoners Really Play: Preference Elicitation and the Impact of Communication

Supporting Information

Courtesy and Idleness: Gender Di erences in Team Work and Team Competition

econstor Make Your Publications Visible.

Which determines Dictating the Risk, risk preference or social image? Experimental evidence-

Shifting the blame to a powerless intermediary

Jakub Steiner The University of Edinburgh. Abstract

Gender differences in the giving and taking variants of the dictator game by Subhasish M. Chowdhury* Joo Young Jeon* Bibhas Saha**

On the behavioural relevance of optional and mandatory impure public goods: results from a laboratory experiment

Stackelberg in the Lab: The E ect of Group Decision Making and Deliberation Periods

Reciprocity, Cooperation, and Reputation

MOTIVATIONAL CROWDING OUT EFFECTS IN CHARITABLE GIVING: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE

Some Thoughts on the Principle of Revealed Preference 1

Contributions and Beliefs in Liner Public Goods Experiment: Difference between Partners and Strangers Design

Experimental Evidence of Self-Image Concerns as Motivation for Giving

Accepting Zero in the Ultimatum Game Does Not Reflect Selfish. preferences

Fairness and Reciprocity in the Hawk-Dove game

Giving To Ingrates?*

TITLE: MEN ARE FROM MARS, WOMEN FROM VENUS, ONLY WITH INCENTIVES

Conditional behavior affects the level of evolved cooperation in public good games

Article Anticipated Communication in the Ultimatum Game

TESTING GUILT AVERSION WORKING PAPERS IN ECONOMICS. Department of Economics TORE ELLINGSEN, MAGNUS JOHANNESSON, SIGVE TJØTTA

An Experimental Comparison of the Fairness Models by Bolton and Ockenfels and by Fehr and Schmidt

Minimal Social Cues in the Dictator Game in press, Journal of Economic Psychology

Learning and Peer Effects Disadvantageous lies

Norms, Frames and Prosocial Behavior in Games

The inefficiency of splitting the bill: A lesson in institution design

Do Women Shy Away from Competition? Do Men Compete too Much?

Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization

An investment game with third-party intervention

Stackelberg in the Lab: The E ect of Group Decision Making and Cooling-o Periods

Findings from lab experiments in urban and rural Uganda

KRANNERT SCHOOL OF MANAGEMENT

Marriage Matching with Correlated Preferences

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Charitable Giving as a Gift Exchange: Evidence from a Field Experiment

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Distributional consequences of Endogenous and Compulsory Delegation

Eye-image in Experiments: Social Cue or Experimenter Demand Effect? by Subhasish M. Chowdhury* Joo Young Jeon** Bibhas Saha***

A Note On the Design of Experiments Involving Public Goods

Experimental Evidence on Gender Interaction in Lying Behavior

Self-centered and other-regarding behavior in the solidarity game

Gender Effects in Private Value Auctions. John C. Ham Department of Economics, University of Southern California and IZA. and

Trust, Fear, Reciprocity, and Altruism: Theory and Experiment. By James C. Cox*

Individual Risk Propensity in a Group Situation ; Cross-Cultural Comparison

VOLKSWIRTSCHAFTLICHE ABTEILUNG. Reputations and Fairness in Bargaining Experimental Evidence from a Repeated Ultimatum Game with Fixed Opponents

UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI DI SIENA QUADERNI DEL DIPARTIMENTO DI ECONOMIA POLITICA. Competition for power and altruism. Luigi Bosco

The Bystander Effect in a Multi-Player Dictator Game. The Bystander Effect Latané and Nida, The Bystander Effect Kitty Genovese (03/13/64)

Less is More: Information and Priming in Charitable Donations

Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science public lecture Human Cooperation

Incentives to cheat under loss aversion

Getting a Yes. An Experiment on the Power of Asking

The Impact of Time Restrictions on Economic Decisions

Tilburg University. Publication date: Link to publication

Transcription:

Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization Vol. 61 (2006) 50 54 Gender composition in teams Martin Dufwenberg a,, Astri Muren b,1 a Department of Economics and Economic Science Laboratory, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721-0108, USA b Department of Economics, Stockholm University, SE-106 91 Stockholm, USA Received 10 May 2004; accepted 10 January 2005 Available online 26 October 2005 Abstract How does gender composition influence team decisions? Our evidence from a group dictator game is: (i) groups are more generous and equalitarian when women are in majority, but (ii) the most generous groups are those with two men and one woman. 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. JEL classification: A13; C92; D63; D64; J16 Keywords: Gender composition; Groups; Teams; Generosity; Equalitarian 1. Introduction Does the presence of women, or the relative balance of women and men, on a decision-making committee matters for its decisions? Very little research seems to have been done on this topic. 2 Yet recent experimental research suggests that men and women often make different decisions, 3 and that groups and individuals often make different decisions. 4 The combination of these results gives reason to believe that gender composition matters for group decisions. Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 520 626 1540; fax: +1 520 621 8450. E-mail addresses: martind@eller.arizona.edu (M. Dufwenberg), astri.muren@ne.su.se (A. Muren). 1 Tel.: +46 8 163306; fax: +46 8 159482. 2 Studies on gender and entrepreneurship, for example, appear to focus on individual traits rather than group composition. See Sonfield et al. (2001) for a survey. 3 For example, Eckel and Grossman (1998) find that women are more generous, Andreoni and Vesterlund (2001) find that women are more equalitarian, and Gneezy et al. (2003) and Gneezy and Rustichini (2004) find that competition increases the performance of men relative to women (and this result may depend on the gender composition of the competing group). 4 See for example Cason and Mui (1997) and Bornstein and Yaniv (1998). 0167-2681/$ see front matter 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2005.01.002

M. Dufwenberg, A. Muren / J. of Economic Behavior & Org. 61 (2006) 50 54 51 Against this background, we report experimental results for a very simple context: a version of Cason and Mui s (1997) team dictator game. Groups of three people divide a sum of money between themselves and a fourth person. Gender composition differs across groups, and we explore how this influences decisions. 5 2. The experiment 2.1. Design 6 Two sessions (I and II) were held at Stockholm University as part of an economics class for prospective teachers. Participants wrote their first names on separate slips of paper that were collected. One or two (depending on session) were drawn at random and the name(s) copied onto a separate paper that was put in a sealed envelope. All original slips were arranged into groups of three, and each group was assigned a number. The group members were asked to sit together and given 5 10 min to propose a split of the sum of 1000 kr ( US$ 110, at the time of the experiment) between the group and the pre-selected person whose identity was documented in the sealed envelope. It was made clear that money allocated to the group would be divided equally between its members. The instructions explained that one (two in Session II) group(s) would be randomly selected to receive their self-determined share in cash, the rest going to the pre-selected recipient(s). 2.2. Hypotheses Following the work on gender differences for single decision makers by, respectively, Eckel and Grossman (1998) and Andreoni and Vesterlund (2001), the hypotheses we test relate to generosity (how much is given) and equalitarianism (making a donation of exactly 250 kr). If there are differences between female-majority and male-majority groups, we should be able to reject one or both of the following hypotheses: H 1. Female majority groups and male-majority groups make the same donations. H * 1. Female majority groups and male-majority groups give 250 kr equally often. We also compare groups with different gender composition, focusing on three cases: allfemale versus groups with two women, groups with two women versus groups with one woman, and groups with one woman versus all-male groups. The associated hypotheses are H 2. Groups make the same donations in pair-wise comparisons when the number of women is increased by one. H * 2. Groups give 250 kr equally often in pair-wise comparisons when the number of women is increased by one. 5 Cason and Mui have some results, which are precursory to ours. They compare behavior in team dictator and standard dictator games. In the process, they record data on and some results about gender composition although that is not their main focus. They also restrict attention to groups of two whereas we have groups of three so that we can say something about the difference between unisex groups and groups that are not unisex but still have a strict majority of persons of a given sex. 6 Dufwenberg and Muren (2004), includes a more detailed version of this section, as well as experimental instructions. Go to http://econ.eller.arizona.edu/downloads/working papers/econ-wp-04-08.pdf.

52 M. Dufwenberg, A. Muren / J. of Economic Behavior & Org. 61 (2006) 50 54 Table 1 Number of donations at each sum for all groups 0 kr 1 kr 10 kr 100 kr 250 kr 300 kr 400 kr 1000 kr Number of groups 3 6 2 10 30 1 1 3 Table 2 Mean (median) donations for FFF + FFM and FMM + MMM groups FFF+FFM 225 (250) FMM+MMM 203 (100) Table 3 Relative number of equalitarian donations for FFF + FFM and FMM + MMM FFF + FFM 25/38 FMM + MMM 5/18 2.3. Results One hundred and sixty-eight persons, comprising 56 groups, participated. Session I had 63 participants (21 groups); Session II had 105 participants (35 groups). In Session I seven of the students elected not to participate; in Session II all present at the lecture participated. We found no session effect on donations, 7 and thus look at data from all 56 groups. We use the following notation for the four possible gender-compositions: FFF = all-female; FFM = 2 women and 1 man; FMM = 1 woman and 2 men; MMM = 3 men. The number of groups for each group type are 26 FFF, 12 FFM, 10 FMM, and 8 MMM. Table 1 shows the distribution of donated amounts. It is remarkable that the data is distributed over exactly eight different amounts, although groups could pick any integer between 0 and 1000. 8 The overall mean donation is 218 kr; the median is 250 kr. Table 2 shows means and medians when the data is partitioned into female-/male-majority groups. We find that female-majority groups donate more. H 1 is rejected (P-value 0.03). 9 Moving to equalitarianism and H 1 *, we first note that 30 out of the 56 groups donated 250 kr. Table 3 breaks this down on female-/male-majority groups. The equalitarian donation is particularly frequent with a female majority and the difference is highly significant (P-value 0.008). 10 H 1 * is rejected. Next we consider H 2. Table 4 shows means and medians of donated amounts. Our result that female majority groups are more generous suggests that donations may increase in pair-wise comparisons as the number of women is increased by one: FMM versus MMM, FFM versus FMM, and FFF versus FFM. The differences between the FFF and FFM groups and the FFM and FMM groups are significant (P-values 0.04 and 0.03), while the difference between the FMM and MMM groups is not (P-value 0.68). 7 P = 0.82 in a Kolmogorov Smirnov test; P = 0.40 in a Wilcoxon Mann Whitney test. 8 The amounts 1, 10, 100, 250, 400, and 1000 share the characteristic that remainders are divisible by 3. 9 Two-sided Wilcoxon Mann Whitney tests are used when comparing donated amounts. 10 Two-sided tests for equality of proportions (the prtest in Stata) are used when comparing proportions.

M. Dufwenberg, A. Muren / J. of Economic Behavior & Org. 61 (2006) 50 54 53 Table 4 Mean (median) donations for each gender composition FFF 189 (250) FFM 304 (250) FMM 195 (100) MMM 334 (100) Table 5 Number of equalitarian donations relative to total number per group type FFF 16/26 FFM 9/12 FMM 3/10 MMM 2/8 The difference between the FFF and FFM groups actually goes in the opposite direction from what our first result may seem to suggest about generosity and the number of women. Two women and one man is the most generous gender composition! Moving to H 2 *, the relative numbers of equalitarian donations appear in Table 5. The FFM versus FMM test delivers a significant result (P-value 0.03), which neither the FFF versus FFM nor the FMM versus MMM does (P-values: 0.42 and 0.81, respectively). All in all, this may suggest a non-monotonic relation between the number of women in a group and the generosity of that group. 3. Discussion We find evidence of a gender effect in group decisions: female-majority groups give more to the individual recipient and also choose the equalitarian division more often than male-majority groups do. We also have the interesting but puzzling result that groups with two women and one man are the most generous. To end on a somewhat speculative note regarding the last result, one possibility is that the presence of a man triggers an exaggerated generosity among the women in the group. This argument receives some support from an observation by Stockard et al. (1988). In a public goods game, they found no gender difference in cooperation although female subjects seemed more eager to appear altruistic. If women use male altruism as a neutral level for comparison, two women may want to be more generous when the third person in the group is a man rather than another woman. Acknowledgements We are grateful for helpful comments from Jim Cox, Ken Troske, Rami Zwick, and seminar participants at the University of Arizona (Management and Policy Department), the Female Network of Economists in Stockholm, the Research Institute of Industrial Economics (Stockholm), the Second Asian Conference on Experimental Business Research (Hong Kong University of Science and Technology), and Örebro University.

54 M. Dufwenberg, A. Muren / J. of Economic Behavior & Org. 61 (2006) 50 54 References Andreoni, J., Vesterlund, L., 2001. Which is the fair sex? Gender differences in altruism. Quarterly Journal of Economics 116, 293 312. Bornstein, G., Yaniv, I., 1998. Individual and group behavior in the ultimatum game: are groups more rational players? Experimental Economics 1, 101 108. Cason, T., Mui, V.-L., 1997. A laboratory study of group polarisation in the team dictator game. Economic Journal 107, 1465 1483. Dufwenberg, M., Muren, A. 2004. Gender composition in teams. Working Paper 04-08, Department of Economics, University of Arizona. Eckel, C., Grossman, P., 1998. Are women less selfish than men? Evidence from dictator experiments. Economic Journal 108, 726 735. Gneezy, U., Niederle, M., Rustichini, A., 2003. Performance in competitive environments: gender differences. Quarterly Journal of Economics 118, 1049 1074. Gneezy, U., Rustichini, A., 2004. Gender and competition at a young age. American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings 94, 377 381. Sonfield, M.C., Lussier, R.N., Corman, J., McKinney, M.T., 2001. Gender comparisons in strategic decision-making: an empirical analysis of the entrepreneurial strategy matrix. Journal of Small Business Management 39, 165 173. Stockard, J., van de Kragt, A.J.C., Dodge, P.J., 1988. Gender roles and social dilemmas: are there sex differences in cooperation and in its justification? Social Psychology Quarterly 51, 154 163.